Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1588280 Psychology Original Research Changing tracks: how different visual presentations of travel itineraries impact the choice between plane and train Catarci Daniele * Laasner Vogt Lea Reijnen Ester School of Applied Psychology, ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences, Zurich, Switzerland

Edited by: Eirini Mavritsaki, Birmingham City University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by: Hajdi Moche, Linköping University, Sweden

Milan Moleman, Delft University of Technology, Netherlands

*Correspondence: Daniele Catarci, daniele.catarci@zhaw.ch
21 07 2025 2025 16 1588280 05 03 2025 02 07 2025 Copyright © 2025 Catarci, Laasner Vogt and Reijnen. 2025 Catarci, Laasner Vogt and Reijnen

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Despite the negative impact flying has on the environment, people too often seem to choose the plane over the train because it supposedly “saves them time.” However, these perceived time savings are often overestimated, and in reality, can be significantly smaller because people have (deliberately or not) forgotten to consider the time costs incurred at the airport for security checks or baggage collection, for example. We therefore wondered whether this illusion of time savings could be prevented or reduced by visually highlighting the total travel time, thereby increasing the choice of train. In our first randomized online study (N = 614) on work-related travel scenarios, we were indeed able to show that presenting a comprehensive itinerary (visualizing the total travel time) instead of just the flight time (standard itinerary) increased train choice from 66 to 79%. A second study (N = 383) confirmed the robustness of this effect across different travel distances and price scenarios. Although our intervention worked, it may prove challenging to implement. A third study (N = 198) therefore examined an alternative intervention, a company guideline discouraging plane travel, by emphasizing both the environmental impact and the limited net time savings. The results showed a comparable increase in train choice. Overall, these results show that drawing attention to overlooked but critical attributes of decision-making, such as the actual total travel time, can serve as a powerful nudge for more sustainable travel choices.

mode of transportation choice travel itinerary presentation travel duration corporate policies sustainability plane/train travel randomized controlled trial section-at-acceptance Cognition

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      One way to reduce climate changing CO2 emissions is to change our travel behavior, in particular to fly less. Although flights are “only” responsible for around 4% of global emissions (Klöwer et al., 2021), this figure is noteworthy when you consider that only a relatively small proportion of the world’s population, mainly from wealthy countries (e.g., Switzerland), flies. For example, only 11% of the total world population flew in 2018 (Gössling and Humpe, 2020). Hence, in Europe, for example, flights cause 14% of transport-related emissions, which is far more than trains (0.4%). In addition, aviation infrastructure also causes more emissions than that of rail transport (European Environment Agency, 2021). Therefore, especially in Europe, which is an area with dense air traffic, switching from plane to train could significantly reduce emissions.

      However, literature shows that behavioral change is anything but easy (e.g., see Halpern, 2015; Sheeran and Webb, 2016; Thaler and Sunstein, 2021). In addition, the planned change involves a problem that can be illustrated using the scenario of “Ben,” who has to plan a business-related conference trip. While planning this trip (from Zurich to Florence), Ben carefully weighs up his travel options. One of them is a train journey through beautiful landscapes at a relatively low price. However, its duration of almost 6 h seems too long. After all, “time is money.” Ben therefore opts for the other option, a direct flight of just over an hour. On the day of the trip itself, however, when Ben finally arrives at the conference center in Florence, he realizes that the time it took him to get to Zurich airport, go through security, board the plane, wait for his luggage in Florence and take the bus to the city center, in addition to the flight itself, added up to a total travel time that was surprisingly close to that of the train journey. What happened to Ben is referred to as “time cost neglect” (Pan et al., 2024). But what annoys Ben most about his realization is that he should have been aware of these additional time costs; after all, it was not his first flight. This situation is not unlike US consumers not taking VAT (sales taxes) into account when buying groceries, as the prices of the goods on the shelves are shown without VAT. However, if those consumers are asked about the amount of tax to be added, they can state them fairly accurately for a whole range of products. Interestingly, when the VAT is stated in advance, it causes consumers to shop differently, and the demand for those goods decreases (Chetty et al., 2009). The question we now ask ourselves is whether the explicit addition of (implicitly) known missing information (i.e., the additional time cost) at the time of booking would have changed Ben’s (or anyone else’s) decision and thus led to less plane travel overall (and therefore less demand for this “good” or service)? Early indications that this could be the case were provided by Tversky and Kahneman’s (1981) famous “Asian disease” problem, or more precisely from its critics.

      Kahneman and Tversky (1986) used this problem to illustrate that people do not always act according to the principles or basic assumptions formulated in “rational choice” theories (e.g., von Neumann and Morgenstern, 1953; Savage, 1954), such as descriptive invariance. Descriptive invariance assumes that the way in which identical (choice) options (e.g., yogurt A and yogurt B) are described linguistically or framed (e.g., whether yogurt A is 90% fat-free or contains 10% fat) should not influence people’s decisions (i.e., choosing yogurt A over yogurt B). They illustrated the violation of the principle by presenting participants with two versions of the problem (see Box 1), each giving participants a choice between a program with a “certain” outcome (A or C) and a program with a “risky” outcome (B or D).

      A closer look at the problem reveals that the options or programs in each version have the same expected value (“EV”; 200 in version 1, 400 in version 2) and that the number of lives that can be saved in both versions is also the same [in version 1, 200 (out of a total of 600) people are saved; in version 2, 400 (out of a total of 600) people die].

      Consequently, from a rational point of view, participants should therefore be indifferent in both versions regarding their choice between the two options or, if not, at least deviate in the same direction in their choice (i.e., always choose the “risky” option).

      “Asian disease” problem.

      Imagine that the United States is preparing for an outbreak of an unusual Asian disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the programs are as follows:

      [Version 1]

      If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved.

      If Program B is adopted, there is a 1/3 probability that 600 people will be saved and a 2/3 probability that no people will be saved.

      It should be noted that half of the participants received version 1, while the other half received version 2.

      [Version 2]

      If Program C is adopted, 400 people will die.

      If Program D is adopted, there is a 1/3 probability that nobody will die and a 2/3 probability that 600 people will die.

      However, Tversky and Kahneman found that the way the options or programs were “framed” in the two versions—either as gaining/saving lives (gain framing) or losing lives (loss framing)—caused people to choose differently. That is, in the gain framing (version 1), 72% of participants chose program A, the certain option, while the opposite was observed in the loss framing (version 2): Here, 78% of participants chose program B, the risky option. Accordingly, gain framing makes people risk-averse, while loss framing makes people risk-seeking. Although these results are not consistent with rational choice theories, they can be well predicted by Kahneman and Tversky’s (1979) prospect theory, which uses a (neutral) reference point to categorize outcomes into gains or losses relative to that point. The resulting concave curve for gains or convex curve for losses results in the behavioral pattern observed above (e.g., risk aversion for gains). These “risky-choice framing” effects have not only been replicated many times (e.g., Kühberger, 1998; Levin et al., 1998) but have also been applied to everyday problems.

      In this context (i.e., everyday problems) it was found that depending on how the problem is described, people can be encouraged to engage in certain (e.g., putting on sunscreen) or risky activities (e.g., going for a mammography or prostate screening), the problem at hand is described differently (whether the use of this method is morally/ethically right is another question, but needs to be considered). In this sense, for example, Detweiler et al. (1999) found that depending on whether the message on a flyer distributed at the beach about the use of sunscreen was described as a gain or a loss (e.g., “Protect yourself from the sun and you will help yourself stay healthy” or “Expose yourself to the sun and you risk getting sick,” p. 191), beachgoers intention (and also actual use) to use sunscreen with a sun protection factor (SPF) of 15 and higher was higher in the gain framing, especially if they had not previously planned to use it.

      However, as authors such as Kühberger (1995); see also Mandel (2001, 2014), Kühberger and Tanner (2010), Tombu and Mandel (2015), and Gigerenzer (2018) have noted—and what is crucial for our work here—is that while in Tversky and Kahneman’s (1981) “Asian disease” problem in the risky options (programs B and D) all information is explicitly stated, this is not the case in the certain options (programs A and C). For example, program A says nothing about what happens to the remaining 400 lives. In this respect, however, it has been argued that people rightly assume (as in our case with air travel or VAT) that the remaining 400 people would lose their lives (Mandel, 2014), and accordingly it should be obvious by these simple mathematical calculations that, for example, program A and C are identical (called: proof by arithmetic argument, see, for example, Mandel, 2014). But, Kühberger (1995) has shown that if the missing information “400 people will die” (in program A) or “200 people will be saved” (in program C) is added to certain options, that is, made explicit (see also Mandel’s experiment 3), the effect demonstrated by Tversky and Kahneman (1981) disappears. There are many reasons why missing information could change the result. One of these is that, for example, the sentence “200 will be saved” is not understood as meaning that exactly 200 people will be saved, but rather that at least that many people will be saved (e.g., Mandel, 2014). Note that omitting part of the information in the risky options, as Reyna and Brainerd (1991) did (e.g., “2/3 probability that no people will be saved”) also changes the results (see also Kühberger and Tanner, 2010).

      Hence, the explicit mention of missing information in the adapted versions of Tversky and Kahneman’s (1981) “Asian disease” problem influenced people’s decisions in a way that made them more similar across different versions (i.e., there was no preference for one program over another anymore). Let us come back to the plane-versus-train problem. It is important to note that in this regard both choice options (plane and train) presumably lie in the loss quadrant of prospect theory, as it is about “lost time,” and that they are both certain (here we momentarily overlook the possibility of delays and strikes for both modes of transport). The difference between the two options is that all time costs are listed for the train option, whereas this is not the case for the other option, the flight. In this case, only the time for the flight itself (approx. 1 h) is listed, but not the additional time costs incurred (approx. 4 h; missing information) for traveling to the airport, checking in, etc.

      Now, the psychology of attention or, more specifically, the salience theory by Bordalo et al. (2012); see also Bordalo et al. (2022) may give us a clue as to why the explicit presentation of missing information as in Tversky and Kahneman’s (1981) “Asian disease” problem might change decisions. Consider thereby the following quote from Bordalo et al. (2012), p. 1255: “the decision maker evaluates lotteries1 by focusing on, and weighting more, their most salient states.” Applied to Ben’s situation, the most salient state is the available information of a short flight of about an hour. Consequently, this information is disproportionately weighted in his decision (see also Dertwinkel-Kalt et al., 2017 or the focus illusion by Kahneman et al., 2006). The presentation of the missing information could now lead to the information of the short flight becoming less salient, which reduces its weighting and thus also the decision in favor of the plane.

      Although the Asian disease problem and its criticisms cannot be mapped one-to-one to our problem, we expect different decision-making behavior or choices (plane or train) depending on the completeness of the information provided. More specifically, the explicit mention of the (implicitly known) missing information should lead to the plane option becoming less salient, as its originally perceived advantage (short travel time) is reduced by the perception of the actual travel time, which should lead to the train being chosen more frequently.

      In our case, we present the missing information not only verbally (i.e., with words), but primarily visually, for example, through a segmented visual timeline of the entire journey and the use of simple graphic elements (e.g., a train pictogram). This dual-modal approach is based on proven findings: visual information is better retained than purely verbal information [Paivio and Csapo, 1973; see also, for example, Osman and Thornton (2019) or Kühne et al. (2023) for the improved impact of labels as a supplement to purely textual nutritional information for healthier and more sustainable food choices]. Similar benefits (e.g., retention, understanding, decisions) can also be observed with pictograms. For example, the risk of reduced fitness to drive can be better assessed (and thus a more informed decision made) if the verbal information is supplemented by corresponding pictograms (Emich et al., 2014).

      What is new about our work? First, testing the effect of displaying missing information in the appropriate decision context (i.e., switching from plane to train). Second, the mainly visual presentation of the missing information. Third, testing the effects through a real (randomized) experiment. And last but not least, testing the influence of other factors such as destination, travel distance and price structure in this context.

      Study 1

      Study 1 examines, among other things, how the travel itinerary presentation form (standard vs. comprehensive) affects the choice of the mode of transport (plane or train). More specifically, a standard itinerary that only shows flight or train travel time is compared to a comprehensive itinerary that includes door-to-door travel time.

      Materials and methods Participants

      Six hundred and fourteen ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences students aged between 18 and 51 (Mage = 24.6; SDage = 4.9; 62.7% were female) took part in this smartphone-based online study. As an incentive, participants could either enter a lottery for an Apple iPad (which 87.8% did) or students from the ZHAW School of Applied Psychology (13.7%) could receive course credit instead (which 53.6% of them did). All participants gave their informed consent.

      Stimulus material, procedure and design

      At the beginning of the study (implemented and run with the help of the Unipark software), participants were told (by means of a scenario) that they—as an employee of a medium-sized company—were to attend a conference abroad. They had to indicate which mode of transport (plane or train) they wanted to use for the main part of the journey, starting in Zurich HB (i.e., Zurich main station) and ending at the conference hall in Florence Campo di Marte (about 280 miles beeline). The travel itineraries for both modes of transport were presented to them visually, similar to a mobile booking app (see Figure 1). Depending on which (itinerary) presentation form condition participants were randomly assigned to, the journey was presented to them in the standard form (only the pure flight or train travel time) or in the comprehensive form (the complete door-to-door travel time, including, for example, the waiting time at the airport2; the two conditions form together the independent variable “presentation form”). In both presentation form conditions, participants had to indicate their mode of transport preference3 on a 0–1004 scale (0 = definitely by plane; 100 = definitely by train; dependent variable “preference”).

      Stimulus material from Study 1, featuring itineraries for the trips from Zurich to Florence via train and plane, translated from German to English. In the standard condition, only the airport-to-airport flight and station-to-station train segments are shown (similar to most booking platforms). The comprehensive condition includes the entire journey to the hotel, incorporating travel to the airport, time for security checks, and local public transportation.

      Comparison of two travel-itinerary styles for a trip from Zurich to Florence: “Standard” and “Comprehensive.” The standard versions show only the main travel leg, such as the direct flight time or the station-to-station train time. The comprehensive versions present the full, door-to-door journey, making “hidden” time costs visible. This includes initial transfers to the station or airport, explicit time for security checks, and the final local transport needed to reach the destination, Firenze Campo di Marte.

      Thereafter, participants were asked to choose between two lunch options for the conference: one vegetarian and one meat based (dependent variable “menu choice”). This decision allowed for checking for any spillover or rebound effects (e.g., whether choosing the plane as mode of transport makes people more likely to choose the vegetarian menu). After these two tasks (mode of transport preference and menu choice), participants first had to rate both travel options (i.e., plane and train) in terms of the specific travel factors5 of convenience, time and reliability on a 0–100 scale (0 = very low; 100 = very high). They were then asked to rate how important the general factors6 of comfort, time effort, number of changes, arrival and departure time, sustainability and possibility to work were to them in relation to their chosen mode of transport (i.e., plane or train) on a scale of 1 to 5 (1 = not at all; 5 = very important). Finally, demographic data (age, gender, income and education) were collected to describe the study population.

      Results

      Sixteen participants that needed more than 18 min to complete (see text footnote 6) the study were excluded from the analysis (2.5%). Statistical analyses were (also in Studies 2 and 3) performed with R software (version 4.2.3; RStudio Team, 2023).7 Wherever possible (i.e., in the case of continuous dependent variables), data were analyzed using t-tests or analyses of variance (ANOVAs), which are the so-called “bread and butter” methods of analysis in basic research (see Strasak et al., 2007); for details, see the relevant sections.

      Mode of transport preference

      A calculated unpaired t-test showed that participants preferred the train more often (about 20%) in the comprehensive itinerary than in the standard itinerary, t(612) = −5.23, p < 0.001, d = −0.42 (see Figure 2).

      Mode of transport preferences by itinerary presentation, with each point representing an individual response. The dotted line indicates no preference for either option. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      A scatter plot displaying individual transport-preference scores on a 0–100 scale, where 0 represents a definite preference for the plane and 100 for the train. The data points form two horizontal bands, corresponding to the "standard" and "comprehensive" itinerary conditions. Black bars with whiskers show the mean score and standard error for each condition. The plot shows that presenting a comprehensive itinerary significantly shifts preference, increasing the mean score from about 66 in the standard condition to about 79 in the comprehensive one.
      Decision time

      To investigate whether participants mode of transport preference (plane or train) was influenced by their decision time we conducted two one-way ANCOVAs (continuous factor: decision time in seconds8) on the dependent variable preference separately for plane travelers (n = 132; participants with <50% train preference) and for train travelers (n = 482; participants with ≥50% train preference). We found, for both plane and train travelers, a significant effect of decision time on preference [plane travelers: F(1, 128) = 4.56, p = 0.035, ηp2 = 0.034; train travelers: F(1, 472) = 31.83, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.063], although this effect was more pronounced for train travelers (see Figure 3A). Hence, with increasing decision time, preferences for both plane and train travelers shifted toward indifference (50%).

      (A) Mode of transport preferences by decision time for train and plane travelers. The density plot shows the distribution of decision times, with each point representing an individual response. The dotted line indicates no preference for either option. (B) Mean importance scores for general decision factors for train and plane travelers, rated on a scale from 1 (not important at all) to 5 (very important). Factors are ordered by their overall mean. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      Panel A shows a scatter plot of travel preference versus decision time. Trend lines indicate that as decision time increases, the initial preferences of both train choosers (starting high) and plane choosers (starting low) regress toward the indifference point of 50. Panel B shows a horizontal bar chart of the mean importance of seven travel factors on a five-point scale. For train travelers, sustainability is the highest-rated factor. For plane travelers, time effort is most important. These two factors also show the largest difference between the groups.
      Specific travel factors

      How do participants (depending on the presentation form) rate traveling by plane compared to traveling by train, for example in terms of convenience? We conducted 2 (travel option: plane, train9; within-subject factor) × 2 (presentation form: standard, comprehensive; between-subject factor) ANOVAs separately on the dependent variable of participant’s responses to the specific travel factors of convenience, time effort, and reliability (see Figure 4).

      Specific travel factor ratings for the train and plane journeys under standard and comprehensive itinerary conditions. The figure shows mean ratings for convenience, time effort, and reliability on a scale from 0 (very low) to 100 (very high). Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      Three line graphs show mean ratings for train and plane travel on the factors of Convenience, Time Effort, and Reliability. In the "Convenience" graph, the train's rating increases significantly in the comprehensive condition, while the plane's decreases. In the "Time Effort" graph, the plane's initial advantage is eliminated when the full itinerary is shown, with both modes being rated similarly. In the "Reliability" graph, the plane is rated as consistently more reliable than the train across both presentation conditions, with little change.
      Convenience

      We found a significant main effect of travel option, F(1, 612) = 54.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.081, indicating that the plane is viewed as a less convenient mode of transport than the train. We also found a significant main effect of presentation form, F(1, 612) = 8.76, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.014, indicating that the standard itinerary presentation is viewed as more convenient than the comprehensive one. More importantly, however, is that we also found a significant travel option × presentation form interaction, F(1, 612) = 45.95, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.070, indicating that the difference in convenience between plane and train was caused by the comprehensive itinerary presentation.

      Time effort

      We again found a significant main effect of travel option, F(1, 612) = 83.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.120, indicating that traveling by plane is viewed as less time-consuming than traveling by train. In contrast to the previous findings, the main effect of presentation form was not significant, F(1, 612) = 1.02, p = 0.313. However, we again found a significant travel option × presentation form interaction, F(1, 612) = 84.37, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.121, which indicates that the difference in time between plane and train was caused (this time) by the standard itinerary presentation and disappears when all trip-relevant information is displayed.

      Reliability

      We again found a significant main effect of travel option, F(1, 612) = 42.83, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.065, with planes being viewed as more reliable than trains. However, neither the main effect of presentation form nor the travel option × presentation form interaction was significant (all F < 0.21, and all p >0.65).

      General travel factors

      How do participants [depending on the preferred mode of transport (plane traveler or train traveler)] rate general travel factors such as, for example, comfort (see Figure 3B)? The calculated unpaired t-tests (Bonferroni-corrected) showed that there is a significant difference between plane travelers and train travelers for each factor10 (comfort: t(612) = 3.49, p = 0.004, d = 0.34, time effort: t(612) = 13.27, p < 0.001, d = 1.30, number of changes: t(612) = 4.06, p < 0.001, d = 0.40, arrival time: t(612) = 3.99, p < 0.001, d = 0.39, sustainability: t(612) = −15.99, p < 0.001, d = −1.57, possibility to work: t(612) = −5.68, p < 0.001, d = −0.56, and departure time: t(612) = 6.08, p < 0.001, d = 0.60). However, time effort (travel time) seemed to be the most important factor for plane travelers, while sustainability was most important for train travelers. And it is precisely these two factors where the difference between plane and train travelers is greatest.

      Menu choice

      Finally, did participants choose the meat menu more or less frequently depending on their preferred mode of transport? A calculated Pearson’s chi-squared test (with Yates’ continuity correction) showed a significant association between “menu choice” (meat or vegetarian) and mode of transport (plane or train), χ2 (1, n = 610) = 4.51, p = 0.03, V = 0.01. While 40.15% of plane travelers chose the meat menu, the figure for train travelers was only 29.92%. Hence, a less sustainable mode of transport preference (i.e., plane) is therefore associated with the consumption of a less sustainable, meat-containing menu (and vice versa).

      Discussion

      Study 1 shows that the presentation of comprehensive travel itineraries, which also include “hidden” time costs, increases the preference for train travel. However, it also became apparent that the relative disadvantage of the plane over the train diminished the more time the participants needed to make their decision. The observation that preferences can turn out differently depending on the duration of the “decision time” is also reflected in studies with decoys. In these studies, where there is a choice between two options (e.g., A and B), a third option (i.e., a decoy) is added to encourage the choice of a particular option (e.g., A). Now, research has shown that the effect of the decoy, a type of nudge (see Thaler and Sunstein, 2021), disappears when, for example, the decision maker is given enough time to make a deliberate cognitive decision (Gaudeul and Crosetto, 2019). In considering that our travel itinerary manipulation (making all travel time information explicit) is also a kind of nudge [probably a type-2 nudge; see Grüne-Yanoff (2025) or Hansen and Jespersen (2013) for a discussion of the different types of nudges], it should show the same effect, which was the case. In addition, it was found that while the time required (travel time) and the departure time were important factors for plane travelers in the decision, for train travelers it was sustainability and the opportunity to work. Finally, what is the link between the preferred mode of transport and the choice of menu? Do we observe “moral licensing,” that is, the phenomenon that a person who has made a decision that is perceived as morally good feels justified in subsequently making a less responsible decision (Merritt et al., 2010)? Or, in the context of our study, would participants who chose the environmentally friendly train option subsequently choose the less environmentally friendly menu (meat)? Our results show the opposite of that which would be expected under moral licensing: participants who preferred the train were more likely to also choose the vegetarian menu. This suggests that participants were more likely to act on an underlying attitude toward the environment, which in general leads to the choice of more sustainable services or products.

      Study 2

      In addition to repeating the key results from Study 1, the aim of Study 2 was to examine selected results from Study 1 in more detail and to expand on the results. As can be seen in Figure 3B of Study 1, there is a greater difference between plane and train travelers in the factor “time effort (travel time)” than in the “number of changes.” This could indicate that these two factors do not cover the same underlying concept. Therefore, we also manipulate the number of changes in Study 2. Furthermore, we are interested in whether we can observe the same results as in Study 1 in relation to a longer trip. Last but not least, we are interested in how certain price structures (absolute vs. relative differences; see Kahneman and Tversky, 1984) affect the choice between plane and train.

      Materials and methods Participants

      Three hundred eighty-three ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences students aged 18 to 49 (Mage = 25.2; SDage = 4.9; 66.3% were female) took part in this smartphone-based online study. As an incentive, participants could either enter a lottery for an Apple iPad (which 92.2% did) or students from the ZHAW School of Applied Psychology (8.6%) could receive course credit instead (which 54.5% of them did). All participants gave their informed consent.

      Stimulus material, procedure and design

      The stimulus material, procedure, etc. were similar to those in Study 1, such as the manipulation of the presentation form (standard, comprehensive; between-subject variable). However, this study introduced several key extensions. The following (between- or within-subject) variables were also manipulated:

      The number of train changes (from 0 to 3 times) in the comprehensive travel itinerary (independent between-subject variable: changes; see Figure 5).

      The destination, by adding to the relatively short route from Zurich HB (i.e., Zurich main railway station) to Cologne (main railway station; beeline: about 260 miles) a longer route from Zurich HB to Vienna (main railway station beeline: about 370 miles—see Table 1 for travel durations; independent within-subject variable: destination).

      The price information, in that participants had to make the same choice twice, once on the condition that the company would pay for the trip and once on the condition that they would have to pay for the trip themselves. For the latter choice, the corresponding prices were communicated. Since we are primarily interested in how companies can influence employees’ travel choices, the first choice was always the one without price information. This manipulation [independent within-subject variable price (with/without)] allows us to investigate whether employees would choose differently if they had to pay the costs out of their own pocket. Furthermore, with regard to the “with price” condition, the following within-subject variables were manipulated: The relative price difference between the plane and train was set to be either “small” or “large” for a participant, and this was applied consistently to both destinations. The absolute price level (low and high) was also manipulated. This was varied for each participant in a counterbalanced manner, such that one destination was randomly set to a high price level and the other to a low level. The details of the price structures are shown in Table 2. Note that the train price for these routes—in line with reality—was always the lower one.

      Exemplary stimulus material from Study 2 for the Vienna trip. Plane itineraries are displayed on the left, while train itineraries are on the right, featuring a single standard version at the top, and multiple comprehensive versions with 0–3 changes.

      A chart comparing itinerary formats for a trip from Zurich to Vienna. The "standard" plane itinerary shows a flight of 1 hour 20 minutes. Its "comprehensive" counterpart shows a full door-to-door journey of 4 hours 45 minutes. The "standard" train itinerary shows a journey of 7 hours 51 minutes. Its "comprehensive" counterparts, which illustrate journeys with 0, 1, 2, or 3 changes, all have a total travel time of 7 hours 56 minutes, showing how additional details are incorporated.

      Travel durations.

      Destination For the entire travel For the main part
      Plane Train Plane Train
      Cologne 4 h 25 min 5 h 20 min 1 h 10 min 5 h 15 min
      Vienna 4 h 45 min 7 h 56 min 1 h 20 min 7 h 51 min

      Prices across the different conditions.

      Price structure Ticket price (in CHF)
      Price level Price difference Plane Train
      Low Small (−10%) 80 72
      Large (−50%) 80 40
      High Small (−10%) 210 189
      Large (−50%) 210 105

      Further, reliability, price and simplicity were added to the more general factors already tested above.11 Finally, participants were asked to rate their mobility habits (e.g., use of car, plane, cruise ship) as well as their beliefs about the environment (captured with the Environmental Portrait Value Questionnaire, or E-PVQ). However, this data or its results are not included in this manuscript.

      Results

      Thirty participants that needed less than 4 or more than 30 min to complete the study were excluded from the analysis (which represented 7.3% of the total number of participants).

      Mode of transport preference Destination

      We conducted a 2 (presentation form: standard, comprehensive; between-subject) × 2 (destination: Cologne [closer], Vienna [further]; within-subject) ANOVA12 with preference as the dependent variable. We found a significant main effect of presentation form, F(1, 381) = 29.37, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.072, with a 15.4% higher preference for the train in the itinerary that was presented comprehensively. We also found a significant main effect of destination, F(1, 381) = 148.15, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.28, with a 16.2% higher preference for the train in closer destination (Cologne). However, we found no significant presentation form × destination interaction, F(1, 381) = 0.98, p = 0.323, suggesting that the effect of itinerary presentation form on mode of transport preference was the same across destinations (see Figure 6A).

      Mode of transport preferences by plane itinerary presentation and (A) destination or (B) number of train changes. The dotted lines indicate no preference for either option. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      Panel A shows that train preference is higher for the nearer destination (Cologne) than the farther one (Vienna), but the comprehensive itinerary consistently results in a stronger train preference for both. Panel B shows that while train preference declines slightly as the number of required train changes increases from 0 to 3, the comprehensive presentation maintains its advantage over the standard one across all levels.
      Number of train changes

      To investigate the role of the number of train changes in more detail, we conducted a 2 (presentation form: standard, comprehensive; between-subject) × 4 (number of changes: 0–3; between-subject) ANOVA13 with preference as the dependent variable. We again found a significant main effect of presentation form, F(1, 305) = 8.67, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.028. In addition, we again found a significant main effect of the number of changes, F(1, 305) = 4.69, p = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.015, with participants being less inclined to take the train if they had to change trains more often. However, we again found no presentation form × number of changes interaction, F(1, 305) = 0.006, p = 0.938, suggesting that the effect of presentation form on mode of transport preference is the same over the number of changes (see Figure 6B).

      Decision time

      We again investigated whether participants’ mode of transport preference was influences by their decision time (the specifics are the same as in Study 1). Independent of the destination (Cologne: short, Vienna: long), decision time affected both plane and train travelers (Cologne: plane travelers [F(1, 123) = 13.85, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.101], train travelers [F(1, 620) = 53.91, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.080]; Vienna: plane travelers [F(1, 257) = 37.96, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.129], train travelers [F(1, 492) = 31.89, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.061], see Figure 7). Hence, as in Study 1, with increasing decision time, preferences for both plane and train (independent of destination) travelers shifted toward indifference (50%).

      Mode of transport preferences by decision time and destination for train and plane travelers. The density plot shows the distribution of decision times, with preference trends for plane and train travelers for Vienna and Cologne. Each point represents an individual response.

      Two side-by-side scatter plots show the relationship between transport preference (0=plane, 100=train) and decision time in seconds, for trips to Cologne and Vienna. In both plots, a density curve of the decision time peaks sharply in the first 15 seconds. Regression lines show a consistent pattern: the preference of participants who choose the train starts high and declines over time, while the preference of plane-choosers starts low and rises, with both groups moving towards indifference with longer deliberation.
      Price

      First, we investigated whether the indication of price (as opposed to no indication of price) influences the mode of transport preference at all. Therefore, we conducted a 2 (presentation form: standard, comprehensive; between-subject) × 2 (price: without, with; within-subject) ANOVA with preference as the dependent variable. We found significant main effects for both presentation form, F(1, 381) = 29.37, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.072, and price, F(1, 381) = 36.45, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.087. However, we did not find a significant presentation form × price interaction, F(1, 381) = 0.12, p = 0.727. This means that although participants are more likely to choose the (cheaper) train option when they must pay for the tickets themselves, the observed effect of itinerary presentation form does not differ from that observed when the employer pays for the tickets (see Figure 8). Second, in the condition where the price was indicated, we investigated how the specific price structure (i.e., variation in price level and price difference) influenced the mode of transport preference (for Cologne and Vienna separately). Therefore, we calculated a 2 (presentation form: standard, comprehensive; between-subject) × 2 (price level: low, high; between-subject) × 2 (price difference: small, large; between-subject) ANOVA with preference as the dependent variable, separately for each destination (Cologne, Vienna). For Cologne, we only found a significant main effect of presentation form, F(1, 375) = 32.19, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.079, but not for price difference, F(1, 375) = 0.65, p = 0.42, or price level, F(1, 375) = 2.25, p = 0.13. Furthermore, none of the interactions (2-fold or higher) were significant (all F ≤ 1.89, p ≥ 0.17; see Figure 8). For Vienna, we found a significant main effect of presentation form, F(1, 375) = 15.28, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.039. All the other main effects were not significant, as were all interactions (2-fold or higher; all F ≤ 3.24, p ≥ 0.073), except for the presentation form × price level interaction, F(1, 375) = 4.58, p = 0.033, ηp2 = 0.012 (see Figure 8). Looking more closely at this interaction, we found no significant effect of presentation form, F(1, 180) = 1.48, p = 0.23, when the price level was low, while the same effect was significant, F(1, 195) = 19.32, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.090, when the price level was high.

      Mode of transport preferences by plane itinerary presentation and price structure. The dotted line indicates no preference for either option. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      A line graph showing how price information affects train preference. Introducing a price where the train is cheaper significantly boosts train preference. Across almost all conditions, the comprehensive itinerary (solid lines) increases train preference over the standard one (dashed lines). However, the effect of the comprehensive itinerary disappears for the longer journey to Vienna when the overall price level is low.
      General travel factors

      Again, we investigated how participants [depending on the preferred mode of transport (plane or train) and destination (Cologne or Vienna)] rated general travel factors such as, for example, comfort (see Supplementary Figure 1). The calculated unpaired t-tests (Bonferroni-corrected; see Supplementary Table 1) again showed that time effort (travel time) seemed to be the most important factor for plane travelers, while sustainability was the most important factor for train travelers.

      Discussion

      We demonstrated that the effect of presentation form observed in Study 1 is robust, independent of distance (destination: Cologne or Vienna) or the number of changes. This effect persisted when prices were included; however, the price conditions did not align with Kahneman and Tversky’s (1984) predictions. There was no overall effect of relative price differences, and while itinerary presentation remained consistent across most price conditions, there were exceptions. Specifically, itinerary presentation form had no impact when the flight was very cheap. This suggests that when perceived time savings from flying exceed a certain threshold and the plane option is clearly the better deal, comprehensive itineraries do not increase the likelihood of choosing the train.

      Study 3

      Studies 1 and 2 showed that presenting a trip through a comprehensive itinerary encouraged participants to make more sustainable travel choices (i.e., preferring the train more often). By implementing comprehensive itineraries in existing booking platforms (e.g., booking.com), these platforms could contribute to more sustainable travel. Not wanting to rely solely on booking platforms to implement this intervention, the question arises whether there are other ways of encouraging participants—employees of companies—to choose the train more often. In Study 3, we explore whether introducing a company guideline which indicates that traveling by plane does not save (much) time would be an effective option.

      Materials and methods Participants

      One hundred and ninety-eight ZHAW Zurich University of Applied Sciences students aged 20 to 49 (Mage = 26.1; SDage = 5.29; 68.7% were female) took part in this smartphone-based online study. As an incentive, participants could either enter a lottery for four 100 CHF gift cards (which 91.9% did) or students from the ZHAW School of Applied Psychology (16.2%) could receive course credit instead (which 28.1% of them did). All participants gave their informed consent.

      Stimulus material, procedure and design

      The stimulus material, procedure, etc., were similar to Studies 1 and 2, with the following exceptions: First, participants were informed that they needed to choose a mode of transport for a business trip14 that they would be taking with a senior colleague. Second, the effect of a company guideline15—which included a detailed explanation of the relatively low time savings of plane travel within Europe and emphasized the higher CO2 emissions associated with flying—was tested (see Figure 9). For this purpose, three groups (to which the participants were randomly assigned) were formed. The first group was first presented with the guideline and then had to choose a mode of transport. The second group, or its participants, first had to choose their mode of transport. They were then presented with the guideline. In a next step, they were then confronted again with their preference of mode of transport, which they could adjust as they wished. The last group (classic control group) did not receive a guideline; its participants only had to indicate a mode of transport preference (these three groups together form the independent variable: guideline). Third, in addition to plane and train, the bus was provided as a third mode of transport for the choice (dependent variable: preference). Fourth, participants had to answer guideline-related questions [each on a scale from 1 to 5 (1 = not at all, to 5 = very much)]. These questions concerned arguments mentioned in the guideline (e.g., “Within Europe, the time saved by flying is not that significant”; see “Argument ratings” section, also for the complete list of questions), or about the guideline itself (e.g., “It makes sense for a company to introduce such a guideline”; see “Guideline Acceptance and Compliance” section). Finally, participants had to indicate their perceived likelihood that they themselves and other employees would comply with the guideline, rated on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 100 (definitely).

      Stimulus material from Study 3, translated from German to English. On the left, the guideline is shown, and on the right, the itineraries, which were presented in the standard way for all groups.

      A figure displaying the materials for the sustainability guideline intervention. The left panel shows the "New Sustainability Guideline," which states that flying in Europe saves minimal time and produces up to 10 times more CO₂ emissions than train travel. The right panel presents a comparison chart for a Zurich-Cologne return trip, detailing three options: a plane journey for CHF 198 with a flight time of about 1h 20m; a train journey for CHF 135 taking about 6 hours; and a bus journey, the cheapest at CHF 72, which takes over 9 hours.
      Results

      Four participants that needed less than 2 or more than 11 min to complete the study were excluded from the analysis (these represented 2% of the total number of participants).

      Mode of transport preference

      Because the dependent variable—the distribution of 100 percentage points across plane, train, and bus—reflects a mutually dependent choice, we calculated a fractional multinomial logit model (see Papke and Wooldridge, 1996), choosing “plane” as the reference mode of transport (N = 266; log pseudo-likelihood = −237.25). Average marginal effects16 were calculated for each independent variable (guideline: without vs. with17; design: between vs. within18) on each mode of transport. For each mode of transport, the average marginal effect reflects the change from one level of the independent variable (e.g., guideline) compared to the other level of this variable chosen as the reference value. The results show that the presence of a guideline decreases the probability of using the plane by 22% (p < 0.001), increases the probability of using the train by 22% (p < 0.001), and has no effect on the probability of using the bus (p = 0.961). The design had no significant effect on either mode of transport (all other p > 0.054; see Figure 10).

      Mode of transport preferences with or without a guideline, showing the probability of choosing train, plane, or bus (summing to 100) on a 0–100 scale (0 = not an option, 100 = definitely preferred). Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      A line graph showing the probability of choosing train, plane, or bus, with and without exposure to a sustainability guideline. The y-axis represents preference, with the three modes summing to 100. When the guideline is present, the preference for the train rises sharply. This increase is mirrored by a corresponding drop in preference for the plane. The preference for the bus remains consistently low and is unaffected by the guideline.
      Argument ratings

      To investigate whether the presentation of the guideline or not [independent variable: guideline (with, without)] influences the assessment of various arguments, such as environmental harm, differently, we conducted unpaired t-tests (Bonferroni-corrected) on all these arguments. We only found a significant effect of guideline regarding the argument “Within Europe, the time saved by flying is not as significant (abbr. Time Savings),” t(196) = −2.95, p = 0.04, d = −0.45; (all other p-values ≥0.22). The guideline thus made the participants aware that flying short distances does not really save time (see Figure 11A); just as the comprehensive travel itinerary presentation did (see Studies 1 and 2).

      (A) Mean agreement ratings on a 5-point scale (1 = not convincing at all, 5 = very convincing) for arguments supporting the introduction of a company travel guideline. Translated from German to English: Environmental harm: “Flying is much more harmful to the environment than traveling by train or bus.” Company climate action: “The company is making a significant step towards climate neutrality.” Air travel emissions: “Air travel is responsible for 35% of the company’s greenhouse gas emissions.” Corporate social responsibility: “The company is acknowledging its social responsibility.” Sustainability strategy: “It is part of the company’s sustainability strategy.” Eco-friendly transport: “Traveling by train and bus are very environmentally friendly ways to travel.” Travel convenience: “For short distances, flying is often more cumbersome than traveling by train.” Work efficiency: “You can work more efficiently on a train than on an airplane.” Time savings: “Within Europe, the time saved by flying is not that significant.” Scenic travel: “On a train or bus, you can enjoy the landscape better.” Guideline comparison: “Many other comparable companies have introduced similar guidelines.” (B) Mean agreement levels on a 5-point scale (1 = “does not apply at all,” 5 = “completely applies”) for company travel guidelines among train & bus travelers versus plane travelers. The statements that were evaluated (translated from German to English) include Incentive: “Incentives should be created to fly less,” Meaningfulness: “It makes sense for a company to introduce such a guideline,” Strictness: “A non-binding guideline is not effective enough,” and Personal responsibility: “The decision to fly or not to fly should ultimately be left to the employees.” (C) Comparison of individuals’ expectations of their own compliance (self) versus their expectations of others’ compliance (others) with the travel guideline. Each point represents an individual response. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

      Panel A shows a bar chart of mean ratings for arguments supporting a travel guideline. Arguments like "Environmental harm" are rated as convincing. The argument that "Time savings" from flying are not significant is rated as more convincing by participants who saw the guideline.Panel B shows bar charts comparing attitudes of plane travelers versus train/bus travelers on four dimensions: Incentive, Meaningfulness, Strictness and Personal Responsibility.Panel C shows a scatter plot of expected compliance with the guideline on a 0-100% scale. The data points form two distinct clouds, visually demonstrating that participants rate their own expected compliance ("self") significantly higher than that of their colleagues ("others").
      Guideline acceptance and compliance

      Here we investigated, for example, guideline-related questions. Overall, the “meaningfulness” argument shows that participants appear to be in favor of the introduction of a company guide, although this agreement is significantly weaker among plane travelers (n = 55) than among train and bus travelers (n = 143), t(196) = −4.06, p < 0.001, d = −0.64. However, it is also clear that incentives are desired (see Figure 11B). Yet, it is still necessary to define what these should look like (e.g., travel time on the train could be regarded as working time). It can also be seen that plane travelers are significantly more likely to believe that the decision to fly or not should be their personal responsibility, t(196) = 2.60, p = 0.010, d = 0.41. Last but not least, participants rated themselves (M = 79.69) as more compliant with such a guideline, t(394) = 8.69, p < 0.001, d = 0.87, than others (M = 64.89; see Figure 11C).

      Discussion

      Despite small differences in design, Study 3 shows that raising participants’ awareness of the fact that flying in Europe (i.e., for short distances) does not really save time has a similar effect on the choice of train, also expressed as a percentage, as in Studies 1 and 2, in which the comprehensive travel itinerary was presented. Accordingly, the goal of reducing business-related flight emissions can also be achieved with a simple guideline, even without coercion. However, possible intensifications should not be carelessly brushed aside. The difference in judging one’s own behavior compared to the behavior of others, in this case compliance to the mentioned guideline, is consistent with other psychological research showing that “most people strongly believe that they are just, virtuous, and moral, but view the average person as significantly less just” (Tappin and McKay, 2017, p. 1).

      Conclusion and discussion

      We were able to show, that when implicitly known information such as “total travel time for flights also includes the time for security checks, etc.” is made explicit, the preference of mode of transport changes (i.e., fewer plane travels). Moreover, the results found were seen regardless of whether the information was made explicit, through the (visual) presentation of comprehensive travel itineraries (Studies 1 and 2) or through the simple (written) mention of a travel guideline (Study 3).

      What underlying mechanism could explain the results? In this regard, we postulate selective attention, which brings implicit information into consciousness and thus enables its semantic processing (e.g., Treisman, 1964; Treisman et al., 1974). This allows “times for security checks, etc.” or more generally the attribute “additional time” to be considered in decision-making. Note that decision-making involves evaluating all relevant attributes (e.g., price, time effort) of the options (e.g., plane, train), weighing them up properly and then choosing the option with the highest expected value (EV). Hence, if attributes—as in the case of implicitly known attributes—are not attended, they are not considered when calculating the expected value of the option. Wright (1974) found that product evaluations and therefore product choice are often based on the few attributes that attract attention. Research in this area has furthermore shown that “the way people screen product information is related to the benefits they are seeking” (see Haley, 1971, p. 8). For example, consumers who want the benefit of caries prevention in a toothpaste pay particular attention to information about the product feature fluoride, while consumers who are looking for the taste of a toothpaste are more likely to look for features such as mint flavor (Haley, 1968). A similar observation can be made in Study 1, for example, regarding participants’ preference to travel by plane or train. When choosing the plane, time seems to be the decisive factor, while when choosing the train, convenience is the decisive factor (Ratneshwar et al., 1997). However, we also noted that such attention effects appear to be short-lived. The more time passes before a decision (plane or train) is made, the more the advantage of the formerly attended attribute disappears. The participants seem to have turned their attention to other attributes in the meantime, so that the attribute “additional time” is no longer included in the calculation of the EV. However, research still needs to be conducted into exactly how attention works. For example, findings from studies that have investigated how humor in advertising maintains attention could be helpful here (Goodrich et al., 2011).

      In addition to a closer look at the role of attention, the question of whether information about attributes such as “additional time” may be deliberately avoided or ignored with regard to the decision must also be investigated. Although research on the topic of “information avoidance,” defined as “any behavior intended to prevent or delay the acquisition of available but potentially unwanted information” (Sweeny et al., 2010, p. 341), is still in its infancy, various authors (Sweeny et al., 2010; Gigerenzer and Garcia-Retamero, 2017; Golman et al., 2017) have shown its impact on people’s decisions. Thereby, information avoidance seems to be influenced by factors such as whether the information is a threat to one’s current beliefs or social norms (Hart et al., 2009; Foust and Taber, 2025). Since these factors are also important for choosing a more sustainable mode of transport, future research should investigate whether information avoidance also plays a role in scenarios like ours, and if so, what the driving factors are.

      What might be potential study limitations? First, the content of the scenarios—a work-related conference trip as an employee of a medium-sized company. Such a work-related scenario could lead participants to be more sensitive to time efficiency, which—compared to a leisure travel scenario—could lead to a different response to the information on total travel time. Future studies should therefore investigate whether the observed results can also be generalized for other travel scenarios (e.g., leisure, visiting family). Second, the study sample which consisted primarily of university students residing in Switzerland—a cohort that is young, female and likely to be environmentally-friendly as in, for example, Meyer (2015); or Kirby and Zwickle (2021). This contrasts with the typical European business traveler (Cats, 2025; Schmalz et al., 2021), who, according to Cats (2025), is in his early 40s, male and less environmentally friendly. This is because the business traveler only takes the train for around a third of business trips in Europe. The fact that the nudge led to a substantial additional shift (i.e., toward the train)—even in Studies 1 and 2, where environmental issues were never mentioned—suggests that sudden awareness of the hidden time costs, rather than pre-existing environmentally-friendlier behavior, is responsible for the effect. However, future field studies with real business travelers and real booking data would be required to verify to what extent the effect exceeds the effect generated with our student sample. Third, hypothetical (via experiments) rather than actual travel preferences were measured. Although experimental results are often valid approximations of real-world behavior (Hainmueller et al., 2015; Mullinix et al., 2015; Stroud and Van Duyn, 2020), eliciting actual preferences can confirm the effectiveness of our manipulation in the real world. Fourth, the effect of price was only examined in scenarios where plane travel was more expensive than train travel. In this context, it was found that the effectiveness of the manipulation disappeared when flights were very cheap. Future research could also investigate scenarios where plane travel is cheaper than train travel. This could support the generalizability of the results of our manipulation. Lastly, the focus of our studies was on analyzing the impact of the itinerary presentation form on mode of transport. Although we also examined factors such as time and cost, our samples (although already quite large) would have been too small to include these factors in an extended analysis and examine their interaction with the itinerary presentation form on mode of transport. However, it would be interesting to investigate in future studies whether, for example, people for whom saving time is most important are more influenced by the indication of total travel time.

      Before we end with a conclusion, we would like to point out the general significance of the results of these studies. Every day we make choices with incomplete information. For example, when we book a hotel room in Switzerland, we are told the price but not the tourist tax, which must be paid additionally, on arrival at the hotel. Even if the consequences in this example are less far-reaching, they are still important in other cases. For example, if we have to decide on the treatment of a serious illness, where doctors provide information on the success rate and side effects, but not, for example, on the recovery time. In both cases, this could mean that our choice would be different than if we had been explicitly provided also with the implicitly known information.

      To conclude, our studies have shown that simple manipulations (e.g., changing the standard itinerary presentation form) have a major impact on people preferring a more environmentally friendly mode of transport. Hence, by implementing these insights (e.g., by booking platforms), an important contribution could be made to reducing CO2 emissions and thus the climate crisis.

      Data availability statement

      The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because only aggregated data is available upon request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to catr@zhaw.ch.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving humans were prepared in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the ZHAW (e.g., “Z_CL_Checkliste_Ethikantraege”). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

      Author contributions

      DC: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LLV: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. ER: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

      Funding

      The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. Open access funding by Zurich University of Applied Sciences (ZHAW).

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Generative AI statement

      The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Supplementary material

      The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1588280/full#supplementary-material

      1Risky decisions such as in the Asian disease problem.

      2As a reminder: all participants knew the starting and ending point of the trip. In addition, all participants were informed, for example, that the time from the Florence airport to the convention hall was approximately 1 h and 15 min. Therefore, participants in the standard condition also had a rough estimate of the door-to-door travel time.

      3For conciseness we use the term preference (not choice) from now on.

      40–100 scales were used to assess, for example, preferences, intentions, etc. (see, e.g., Haase et al., 2013). A detailed justification for the use of this type of scale can be found in the Supplementary material.

      5Question translated from German to English: Specific travel factors: “How would you rate the (plane/train) option in terms of the following dimensions? Please note that this refers to the entire journey from Zurich Main Station (Zürich HB) to the conference venue (Florence Campo di Marte).” (The dimensions were as follows: convenience, time effort, reliability); General travel factors: “How important were the following aspects when choosing your travel option?” (The aspects were: comfort, time effort, number of changes, etc.; see Figure 3B).

      6Based on reaction time (RT) histograms.

      7The detailed itinerary contained a slight error: the total travel time (4 h 12 min) did not match the sum of the individual trip sections (4 h 48 min). The mistake was fixed after about half the participants had already completed the study. We statistically checked and found no significant difference for the participants before and after the mistake was corrected.

      8Decision time (in seconds), measured from the time participants were asked the question about their preferred mode of transportation to the time they responded. Furthermore, only participants with decision times greater than 0 s and less than 100 s were included in the analysis (n = 604).

      9Note that participants had to answer the question regarding plane and train.

      10If the travel option was assigned a value of below 50, it is coded as if the participant has chosen the plane; for values of 50 or more, as if he/she has chosen the train.

      11Note, questions translated from German to English. General travel factors: “How important were the following aspects when choosing your travel option?” (The aspects were: reliability, number of changes, price, time effort, etc.; see Supplementary Table 1 or Supplementary Figure 1).

      12Unless otherwise stated, both decisions, with and without price, were included in the model calculations, whereby this factor was treated as a within-subject one.

      13Participants in the standard train itinerary condition were excluded from this analysis.

      14This trip is a shorter journey, with the destination Cologne.

      15Note, that only the standard itinerary presentation was used.

      16The estimated p-values of the marginal effects were corrected using the Benjamini–Hochberg correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

      17Note, that the “guideline” level with is formed from the data of groups 1 and 2.

      18Note, that the “design” level between is formed from the data of groups 1 and 3, while the “design” level within level is only formed from the data of group 2.

      References Benjamini Y. Hochberg Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x Bordalo P. Gennaioli N. Shleifer A. (2012). Salience theory of choice under risk. Q. J. Econ. 127, 12431285. doi: 10.1093/qje/qjs018 Bordalo P. Gennaioli N. Shleifer A. (2022). Salience. Annu. Rev. Econ. 14, 521544. doi: 10.1146/annurev-economics-051520-011616 Cats O. (2025). The long journey towards a shift to rail in the European long-distance passenger transport market. npj Sustain. Mobil. Transp. 2:7. doi: 10.1038/s44333-025-00025-9, PMID: 39911596 Chetty R. Looney A. Kroft K. (2009). Salience and taxation: theory and evidence. Am. Econ. Rev. 99, 11451177. doi: 10.1257/aer.99.4.1145 Dertwinkel-Kalt M. Köhler K. Lange M. R. J. Wenzel T. (2017). Demand shifts due to salience effects: experimental evidence. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 15, 626653. doi: 10.1093/jeea/jvw012 Detweiler J. B. Bedell B. T. Salovey P. Pronin E. Rothman A. J. (1999). Message framing and sunscreen use: gain-framed messages motivate beach-goers. Health Psychol. 18, 189196. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.18.2.189, PMID: 10194055 Emich B. van Dijk L. Monteiro S. P. de Gier J. J. (2014). A study comparing the effectiveness of three warning labels on the package of driving-impairing medicines. Int. J. Clin. Pharm. 36, 11521159. doi: 10.1007/s11096-014-0010-2, PMID: 25204257 European Environment Agency (2021). Transport and environment report 2020—train or plane?. Available online at: https://www.eea.europa.eu/publications/transport-and-environment-report-2020. (Accessed August 6, 2024) Foust J. L. Taber J. M. (2025). Information avoidance: past perspectives and future directions. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 20, 241263. doi: 10.1177/17456916231197668, PMID: 37819241 Gaudeul A. Crosetto P. (2019). Fast then slow: a choice process explanation for the attraction effect. Available at: https://hal.science/hal-02408719. (Accessed February 6, 2025) Gigerenzer G. (2018). The bias bias in behavioral economics. Rev. Behav. Econ. 5, 303336. doi: 10.1561/105.00000092 Gigerenzer G. Garcia-Retamero R. (2017). Cassandra’s regret: the psychology of not wanting to know. Psychol. Rev. 124, 179196. doi: 10.1037/rev0000055, PMID: 28221086 Golman R. Hagmann D. Loewenstein G. (2017). Information avoidance. J. Econ. Lit. 55, 96135. doi: 10.1257/jel.20151245 Goodrich K. Schiller S. Galletta D. (2011). Intrusiveness of online video advertising and its effects on marketing outcomes. Proceedings of the International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS 2011). Available online at: https://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2011/proceedings/visualmedia/5. (Accessed June 10, 2025) Gössling S. Humpe A. (2020). The global scale, distribution and growth of aviation: implications for climate change. Glob. Environ. Change 65:102194. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2020.102194, PMID: 36777089 Grüne-Yanoff T. (2025). Boosts are not educative or system-2 nudges. Mind Soc., 113. doi: 10.1007/s11299-025-00324-1 Haase N. Renkewitz F. Betsch C. (2013). The measurement of subjective probability: evaluating the sensitivity and accuracy of various scales. Risk Anal. 33, 18121828. doi: 10.1111/risa.12025 Hainmueller J. Hangartner D. Yamamoto T. (2015). Validating vignette and conjoint survey experiments against real-world behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 23952400. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416587112, PMID: 25646415 Haley R. I. (1968). Benefit segmentation: a decision-oriented research tool. J. Mark. 32, 3035. doi: 10.1177/002224296803200306 Haley R. I. (1971). Beyond benefit segmentation. J. Advert. Res. 11, 38. Halpern D. (2015). Inside the nudge unit: how small changes can make a big difference. New York: Random House. Hansen P. G. Jespersen A. M. (2013). Nudge and the manipulation of choice: a framework for the responsible use of the nudge approach to behaviour change in public policy. Eur. J. Risk Regul. 4, 328. doi: 10.1017/S1867299X00002762 Hart W. Albarracín D. Eagly A. H. Brechan I. Lindberg M. J. Merrill L. (2009). Feeling validated versus being correct: a meta-analysis of selective exposure to information. Psychol. Bull. 135, 555588. doi: 10.1037/a0015701, PMID: 19586162 Kahneman D. Krueger A. B. Schkade D. Schwarz N. Stone A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science 312, 19081910. doi: 10.1126/science.1129688, PMID: 16809528 Kahneman D. Tversky A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263291. doi: 10.2307/1914185 Kahneman D. Tversky A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. Am. Psychol. 39, 341350. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.341 Kahneman D. Tversky A. (1986). Rational choice and the framing of decisions. J. Bus. 59, 251278. Kirby C. K. Zwickle A. (2021). Sustainability behaviors, attitudes, and knowledge: comparing university students and the general public. J. Environ. Stud. Sci. 11, 639647. doi: 10.1007/s13412-021-00717-x Klöwer M. Allen M. Lee D. Proud S. Gallagher L. Skowron A. (2021). Quantifying aviation’s contribution to global warming. Environ. Res. Lett. 16:104027. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/ac286e Kühberger A. (1995). The framing of decisions: a new look at old problems. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 62, 230240. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1995.1046 Kühberger A. (1998). The influence of framing on risky decisions: a meta-analysis. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 75, 2355. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2781, PMID: 9719656 Kühberger A. Tanner C. (2010). Risky choice framing: task versions and a comparison of prospect theory and fuzzy-trace theory. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 23, 314329. doi: 10.1002/bdm.656 Kühne S. J. Reijnen E. Laasner Vogt L. Baumgartner M. (2023). Can carbon labels encourage green food choices? Front. Psychol. 13:902869. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.902869, PMID: 36778167 Levin I. P. Schneider S. L. Gaeth G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 76, 149188. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804, PMID: 9831520 Mandel D. R. (2001). Gain–loss framing and choice: separating outcome formulations from descriptor formulations. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 85, 5676. doi: 10.1006/obhd.2000.2932, PMID: 11341817 Mandel D. R. (2014). Do framing effects reveal irrational choice? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 11851198. doi: 10.1037/a0034207, PMID: 23978186 Merritt A. C. Effron D. A. Monin B. (2010). Moral self-licensing: when being good frees us to be bad. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 4, 344357. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00263.x Meyer A. G. (2015). Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from Europe. Ecol. Econ. 116, 108121. doi: 10.1016/J.ECOLECON.2015.04.018 Mullinix K. J. Leeper T. J. Druckman J. N. Freese J. (2015). The generalizability of survey experiments. J. Exp. Polit. Sci. 2, 109138. doi: 10.1017/XPS.2015.19 Osman M. Thornton K. (2019). Traffic light labelling of meals to promote sustainable consumption and healthy eating. Appetite 138, 6071. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.03.015, PMID: 30880087 Paivio A. Csapo K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: imagery or dual coding? Cogn. Psychol. 5, 176206. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90032-7 Pan J. J. Etkin J. Berger J. (2024). Time cost neglect. Marketing Science Institute Working Paper Series 2024. Report No. 24-126. (Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science Institute). Available online at: http://thearf-org-unified-admin.s3.amazonaws.com/MSI_Report_24-126.pdf. (Accessed May 23, 2025) Papke L. E. Wooldridge J. M. (1996). Econometric methods for fractional response variables with an application to 401(k) plan participation rates. J. Appl. Econ. 11, 619632. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1255(199611)11:6<619::AID-JAE418>3.0.CO;2-1 Ratneshwar S. Warlop L. Mick D. G. Seeger G. (1997). Benefit salience and consumers’ selective attention to product features. Int. J. Res. Mark. 14, 245259. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8116(97)00007-4 Reyna V. F. Brainerd C. J. (1991). Fuzzy-trace theory and framing effects in choice: gist extraction, truncation, and conversion. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 4, 249262. doi: 10.1002/bdm.3960040403 Savage L. J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons. Schmalz U. Paul A. Gissibl V. (2021). An explorative study of corporate travellers’ perception at a German airport. J. Air Transp. Manag. 92:102040. doi: 10.1016/j.jairtraman.2021.102040 Sheeran P. Webb T. L. (2016). The intention–behavior gap. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 10, 503518. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12265 Strasak A. M. Zaman Q. Marinell G. Pfeiffer K. P. Ulmer H. (2007). The use of statistics in medical research: a comparison of The New England Journal of Medicine and Nature Medicine. Am. Stat. 61, 4755. doi: 10.1198/000313007X170242 Stroud N. J. Van Duyn E. (2020). Assessing the external validity of using news websites as experimental stimuli. Commun. Methods Meas. 14, 212218. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2020.1718630 Sweeny K. Melnyk D. Miller W. Shepperd J. A. (2010). Information avoidance: who, what, when, and why. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 14, 340353. doi: 10.1037/a0021288 Tappin B. M. McKay R. T. (2017). The illusion of moral superiority. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 623631. doi: 10.1177/1948550616673878, PMID: 29081899 Thaler R. H. Sunstein C. R. (2021). Nudge: The final edition. New York: Penguin. Tombu M. Mandel D. R. (2015). When does framing influence preferences, risk perceptions, and risk attitudes? The explicated valence account. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 28, 464476. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1863 Treisman A. M. (1964). Selective attention in man. Br. Med. Bull. 20, 1216. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070274, PMID: 14104089 Treisman A. Squire R. Green J. (1974). Semantic processing in dichotic listening? A replication. Mem. Cogn. 2, 641646. doi: 10.3758/BF03198133, PMID: 24203732 Tversky A. Kahneman D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. NASWA Unempl. Insur. Integr. 211:4481. doi: 10.5555/20.500.11941/3953 Von Neumann J. Morgenstern O. (1953). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Wright P. L. (1974). Analyzing media effects on advertising responses. Public Opin. Q. 38, 192205. doi: 10.1086/268151
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016mgwfge.com.cn
      hxjebf.com.cn
      www.holdzhu.net.cn
      www.lucoqn.com.cn
      www.epepiy.com.cn
      www.fzqplscd.org.cn
      www.vrvision.net.cn
      rhiwip.com.cn
      nyfhrq.com.cn
      nbhstxd.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p