Edited by: Miriam Garrido-Miguel, University of Castilla-La Mancha, Spain
Reviewed by: Cecilia Forcato, Instituto Tecnológico de Buenos Aires, Argentina
María Fernanda Zerón-Rugerio, University of Barcelona, Spain
†ORCID: Tore Nielsen
Jade Radke
Claudia Picard-Deland
Russell Arnold Powell
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Despite centuries-old beliefs and anecdotal evidence that food can influence one's sleep and dreams—an example being the classic
We asked 1,082 participants to complete an online survey to test specific hypotheses on why people perceive that food affects their dreams, including whether specific foods influence dreams directly (food-specific effects), through physiological symptoms (food distress), or via altered sleep quality (sleep effects). Survey measures included standard demographic variables, targeted probes about self-perceived effects of specific foods on dreams, questions about diet, food intolerances and allergies, personality questionnaires, measures of sleep quality (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index) and the Nightmare Disorder Index.
A total of 40.2% of participants reported that certain foods either worsened (24.7%) or improved (20.1%) their sleep, while 5.5% of participants reported that food affected their dreams. The perceived effect of food on dreams was associated with higher nightmare recall and Nightmare Disorder Index scores, with changes being blamed primarily on desserts/sweets (31%) and dairy (22%). The effect was also associated with food allergies and Gluten Intolerance, while worse sleep perceptions were tied to Lactose Intolerance. Nightmare Disorder Index scores were strongly associated with Food Allergy and Lactose Intolerance, the latter being mediated by the severity of gastrointestinal symptoms. Healthy eating, such as less evening eating, predicted higher dream recall, while unhealthy eating—including gastric symptoms, lower reliance on hunger and satiety cues, and evening eating—predicted nightmares and dream negativity.
These results support the food-specific effects, food distress, and sleep effects hypotheses to varying degrees. They replicate associations between diet and dream features, highlighting food sensitivities, particularly Lactose Intolerance, as contributors to nightmare prevalence. Findings open new avenues of research on food-dependent dreaming by suggesting dairy-induced gastrointestinal symptoms as one plausible basis for bizarre or disturbing dreams. They have clear implications for understanding how dietary factors may influence sleep quality and the occurrence of nightmares and could inform non-pharmacological interventions for sleep disturbances.
香京julia种子在线播放
People have long believed in the power of food to influence a person's sleep and dreams (reviewed in Nielsen and Powell,
Protagonists in the cartoons of Winsor McCay frequently blamed their presleep ingestion of cheese dishes for occurrences of bizarre dream and nightmare events. (Reproduced from “Dream of the Rarebit Fiend” by Windsor McCay,
Whereas the above studies examined the association between food consumption and dreaming, our group (Nielsen and Powell,
Such findings led us to posit several possible explanations for the common perception that food affects dreaming. These include
In sum, there is a limited, but growing, body of research that supports age-old beliefs that food can influence the content of dreaming and, in particular, the occurrence of nightmares. The present study was designed to shed new light on why dreaming and nightmares are often thought to be food-dependent. Our objective was to test several hypotheses about food's relationship to dreaming, namely, the
A total of 1,082 participants (721 F; 338 M; 11 non-binary, 2 agender, 3 transgender, 3 gender fluid/queer, 2 not listed, 1 prefer not to disclose, 1 missing) completed an online survey that assessed relationships between eating, sleeping, and dreaming. Data collection took place from January to April 2023 using the Qualtrics Labs, Inc. web platform. Participants were undergraduate students (mean age = 20.3 ± 3.68 yrs old; range: 17–54) enrolled in psychology courses at MacEwan University. Students earned 2% toward their final grade for their participation. Participants identified as White (
The study was approved by MacEwan University's Research Ethics Board (ID: 102049). All participants provided informed consent through electronic signature prior to starting the study. They were free to leave blank any questions they did not want to answer and to withdraw from the study at any time without penalty.
Our online Food & Dreams Survey consisted of both validated and in-house questionnaires that assessed demographic information, dietary habits, health status, personality traits, perceived food-dependent sleep and dreaming, and various sleep and dreaming characteristics, including sleep quality, dream frequency, lucid dreaming, and nightmares.
The measures reported below are a subset of a larger battery of measures in the survey, many of which are not described here.
Information collected included gender, age, ethnicity, religion, medical conditions, psychiatric conditions, current medications, and pregnancy. Due to low numbers of gender diverse participants, only men and women were included in analyses where gender was considered.
Two measures were derived from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al.,
Nightmares were assessed with a modified version of the NDI (Dietch et al.,
One item assessed frequency of dream recall over the past month (
This questionnaire was adapted from the FDD questionnaire used in our previous study (Nielsen and Powell,
General health status was rated with a single item (
The PHQ4 (Kroenke et al.,
The IES-2 is a 23-item scale that measures an individual's tendency to follow their physical hunger and satiety cues when deciding when, what, and how much to eat (Tylka and Kroon Van Diest,
Food sensitivities were assessed with eight items. General food intolerance was assessed with a 4-choice item that asked “
We administered the GISQ (Bovenschen et al.,
EAH is a three-item measure of overeating that asks “
The HEA from the Government of Northwest Territories (adapted from Paxton et al.,
The NEQ is a 14-item scale that assesses the behavioral and psychological symptoms of Night Eating Syndrome (Allison et al.,
Analyses were conducted using SPSS 26 for Windows. Data reduction for multi-item questions was achieved using factor analysis with principal components extraction, Kaiser normalization and Varimax (factor-orthogonal) rotation. Regression analyses were achieved with linear regression. Spearman correlations were used when response distributions were non-Gaussian in nature.
Dream and nightmare recall frequencies over the last month for the entire sample are shown in
Overall health status was rated to be 4.91 ± 1.17 or almost 1 point above the midpoint of the 1–7 scale. Women rated their overall health status as lower (4.80 ± 1.17) than did men (5.20 ± 1.11;
Percentages of participants who named foods that produce either better or worse sleep are shown in
A total of 32.4% (351/1,082) of participants reported some form of food sensitivity. This was true for almost twice as many women (38.1%) as men (19.8%; χ2 = 35.32,
Many participants reported that eating affected their sleep in some way: 435 (40.2%) said that their sleep was either improved or worsened either by eating particular foods (374 or 34.6%) or by eating late (137 or 12.7%). A total of 267 (or 24.7%) of participants claimed that eating particular foods made their sleep worse while 218 (or 20.1%) claimed that foods made it better. As shown in
A total of 59 (or 5.5%) of 1076 participants responding reported that foods or eating late affected their dreaming; we refer to such reports as food-dependent dreaming (FDD+). Thirty-five (of 1070) participants (3.3%) reported that eating late affected their dreams (replies of No: 656 or 61.3%; Unsure: 379 or 35.4%). FDD+ was marginally higher in women (46 or 6.4%) than men (12 or 3.6%; χ2 = 3.56,
To test the
Number of times each of 14 food groups was named as causing dreams to become more vivid, disturbing, bizarre, pleasant, or lucid.
Desserts/Sweets | 13 | 33 | 3 | 11 | 10 | 31 | 10 | 38 | 3 | 50 | 39 | 29.8 |
Dairy | 9 | 23 | 3 | 11 | 7 | 22 | 7 | 27 | 1 | 17 | 27 | 20.6 |
Fruit | 5 | 13 | 4 | 15 | 1 | 3 | 2 | 8 | 0 | 0 | 12 | 9.2 |
Meat | 1 | 3 | 2 | 7 | 5 | 16 | 2 | 8 | 1 | 17 | 11 | 8.4 |
Cereals | 3 | 8 | 2 | 7 | 1 | 3 | 2 | 8 | 0 | 0 | 8 | 6.1 |
Spicy foods | 1 | 3 | 0 | 0 | 4 | 13 | 2 | 8 | 0 | 0 | 7 | 5.3 |
Vegetables | 3 | 8 | 3 | 11 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 6 | 4.6 |
Herb tea | 3 | 8 | 3 | 11 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 6 | 4.6 |
Non-Caf beverages | 1 | 3 | 1 | 4 | 1 | 3 | 1 | 4 | 1 | 17 | 5 | 3.8 |
Eggs | 1 | 3 | 2 | 7 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 3 | 2.3 |
Seafood | 0 | 0 | 1 | 4 | 2 | 6 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 3 | 2.3 |
Nuts | 0 | 0 | 1 | 4 | 1 | 3 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 2 | 1.5 |
Veg protein | 0 | 0 | 1 | 4 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0.8 |
Pickled | 0 | 0 | 1 | 4 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0.8 |
40 | 27 | 32 | 26 | 6 | 131 | 100 |
Alcohol, Caffeine, Vitamins, Other, and Unspecified food groups were excluded. Dream category percentages can be > 100 for some categories because participants could select more than one food group for each dream category. See text for label definitions.
To test the
Prevalence of food sensitivities for participants who perceive (Yes/No) that food leads to altered dreaming, better sleep, or worse sleep.
Food Allergy | 18.6 | 9.3 | 5.43 | 0.039 |
Gluten Intolerance | 6.8 | 2.1 | 5.46 | 0.043 |
Lactose Intolerance | 28.8 | 21.6 | 1.67 | 0.198 |
Food Allergy | 11.0 | 9.6 | 0.38 | 0.527 |
Gluten Intolerance | 2.8 | 2.3 | 0.14 | 0.629 |
Lactose Intolerance | 22.9 | 21.8 | 0.14 | 0.715 |
Food Allergy | 12.7 | 9.0 | 3.22 | 0.077 |
Gluten Intolerance | 4.1 | 1.8 | 4.46 | 0.062 |
Lactose Intolerance | 29.6 | 19.5 | 11.91 | <0.001 |
As shown in
The findings for worse sleep were confirmed by assessing self-rated sleep quality on the PSQI-M. The presence (vs. absence) of Lactose Intolerance was associated with lower sleep quality as indexed by a higher PSQI-M score (7.76 ± 2.90 vs. 6.90 ± 2.80;
In sum, participants with food allergies and Gluten Intolerance were more likely to report that food affects dreaming, while those with Lactose Intolerance were more likely to report that food causes worse sleep.
To assess relationships between dreaming and healthy eating habits, stepwise multiple regression analyses were conducted using each of three dreaming indicators (
In sum, regression analyses revealed relationships between eating habits and both dream recall and dream negativity, independent of gender and other pathological factors. While dream recall was predicted by healthy eating, as reflected in a high body-food congruence and less evening eating, dream negativity was predicted by three indicators of unhealthy eating: GI symptom severity, more evening eating, and lower reliance on hunger and satiety cues.
We next assessed whether food sensitivities were associated with dreaming and nightmares using group comparisons. As shown in
Nightmare Disorder Index (NDI) scores for presence/absence of food sensitivities. SEM error bars are shown. *
To explore possible graded relationships between dreaming and food sensitivities, three separate multiple regressions were conducted using
To assess whether GI symptoms mediated the relationship between Lactose Intolerance and NDI, we conducted a mediation analysis using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM;
Mediation analyses of the relationship between Lactose Intolerance and Nightmares with standardized beta coefficients. The mediating variables are GI Symptoms (Panel
It revealed a significant indirect effect of Lactose Intolerance on nightmares through GI symptoms (β = 0.046, 95% confidence interval = 0.026, 0.067;
Finally, to further test the
In sum, GI symptoms consistently and significantly mediated relationships between Lactose Intolerance and nightmares. One possible trajectory is that GI symptoms increase nightmares by increasing symptoms of anxiety and depression. Sleep quality also partially mediated the relationship between Lactose Intolerance and nightmares. However, the indirect effect of Lactose Intolerance on nightmares through GI symptoms and sleep quality was not significant, suggesting that variables other than GI symptoms may be important for explaining the relationship between Lactose Intolerance, sleep quality, and nightmares.
Having found general relationships between Lactose Intolerance, sleep, and nightmares, we looked more closely at the association of eating at night (Night-Eating) and eating close to bedtime (Evening-Eating) with sleep and dreaming measures. As shown in
Spearman correlations (2-tailed) between PSQI-M, PSQI-Q9, dream and nightmare measures, and night- and evening-eating factor scores.
Sleep quality (PSQI-M) | 0.115*** | 0.156*** |
Dream recall frequency (DRF) | 0.017 | −0.036 |
Dream emotional tone | −0.120*** | −0.067* |
Nightmare disorder index (NDI) | 0.126*** | 0.121*** |
*
Our findings provide evidence that both supports and refutes the hypotheses we proposed to possibly explain relationships between food and dreaming (Nielsen and Powell,
Some of the present findings are pertinent to the
With respect to participants' reports of food-dependent dreaming, there was a much lower prevalence of FDD+ participants in the present cohort (5.5%) than in our previous one (17.8%; Nielsen and Powell,
Thus, it is possible that awareness of food sensitivities, pandemic-induced fears and preoccupations combined with other cultural and societal changes somehow led to a net diminishment in our participants' abilities to perceive food-dependent dreaming. Further study of a number of these factors is clearly warranted.
The
Two additional findings in the present study suggest that the
Importantly, we also found a significant indirect effect of GI symptoms on the relationship between Lactose Intolerance and nightmares. This analysis supports the notion that specific food-induced symptoms such as bloating, cramping or excess gas arising during sleep have a negative impact on dreaming. Such a mediating role for GI symptoms is consistent with other findings that dreaming is more emotionally intense and conflictual when abdominal cramping is at its worst, e.g., during menstruation (see review in Nielsen,
In many of his closing panels, protagonists of the Rarebit Fiend cartoonist Winsor McCay explicitly blamed bizarre events occurring in their dreams and nightmares on symptoms associated with eating cheese dishes prior to sleep. We speculate that these symptoms may have been linked to the artist himself being afflicted with a Lactose Intolerance condition. (Reproduced from “Dream of the Rarebit Fiend” by Windsor McCay,
In sum, not only do participants perceive relationships to exist between disturbed dreaming and dairy (among other food groups), but such dreaming is associated with a specific sensitivity to dairy, and possibly to a wider variety of allergies and intolerances as well. GI symptoms may be an important factor mediating the association between Lactose Intolerance and nightmares.
This hypothesis stems from evidence that dietary habits can worsen sleep (Choi et al.,
First, with respect to dietary content, we found that well over a third (40.2%) of participants claimed that eating particular foods affected their sleep, with almost a quarter (24.7%) of them claiming that it worsened their sleep. Some specific foods were named as responsible for such changes, mainly, sweets/desserts, spicy foods and dairy. These participant observations are, in fact, consistent with many studies of diet and sleep quality finding that particular foods are associated with either better (see review in Peuhkuri et al.,
Note that the direction of causality in many studies of food and sleep remains unclear. For example, it is unclear if the fact that individuals who sleep less have poorer diets (Peuhkuri et al.,
Second, with respect to the timing of meals, misalignment of eating times can shift one's chronotype toward an eveningness pattern, and the night-eating of meals is associated with an eveningness chronotype (Kandeger et al.,
Although our study was not designed specifically to address the
Insofar as the present findings are essentially correlational in nature, it remains unclear whether the dietary factors assessed cause dream changes, whether dreaming affects dietary choices, or whether some third factor affects both diet and dreaming. In the case of dream recall, associations with two markers of healthy eating—less evening-eating and higher scores on the
Similarly, the direction of causality among the inter-related measures of nightmares, GI symptoms and evening eating are not resolved by the present findings. GI symptoms and evening eating are clearly factors that might disrupt sleep or bring about dysphoric bodily sensations during sleep that could then find their way into dreams as nightmarish sensations and images. Consistent with this possibility is the finding that a slowly digested (vs. a quickly digested) meal taken late in the evening disrupts sleep via effects on the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis, and that both types of evening meal increase morning cortisol (Ucar et al.,
Beyond such alternative effects, it is also feasible that nightmares and dietary symptoms both stem from an as yet unknown third, psychopathological, factor—one not involving anxiety or depression, as these were controlled in most of the present analyses. One possible factor is, alexithymia, a deficit in emotional expression, which is a known correlate of both GI disorders (Porcelli et al.,
Finally, it should be noted that effects observed in the present study may be due, at least in part, to biases introduced by our exclusive reliance on first-year undergraduate students as participants. These individuals may have had prior academic exposure to topics related to sleep/dreaming, diet, and mental health that influenced how they interpreted or reported the perceived effects of food on sleep and dreaming.
If food choices are indeed a factor causing changes in the nature of dreaming, then interventions focused on diet may well prove useful in treating disturbed dreaming. Our findings suggest that nightmares may turn out to be responsive to diets that minimize the potential for ingesting foods that cause gastric distress, most notably, foods with lactose that cause GI symptoms. Of particular note, there is a growing body of research examining the relationship of diet to post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a major characteristic of which is the frequent occurrence of nightmares. For example, one group (Ke et al.,
The present results further clarify long-standing questions about food's relationship to dreaming and nightmares. In addition to uncovering and controlling for several important confounding factors—gender and psychopathology in particular—we found evidence of food and diet being associated with both dream recall and dream negativity. Specifically, the frequency of dream recall was associated with indicators of healthy eating, i.e., higher scores on a subscale of the
Thus, the present results add empirical observations to age-old beliefs that food affects dreaming. They demonstrate that food-dependent dreaming (FDD+) predicts nightmare frequency and severity, with dairy and sweets most commonly being blamed. They show that food sensitivities, Lactose Intolerance in particular, are associated with nightmares–often mediated by GI symptoms. Together, the findings lend support to several hypotheses about food-dependent dreaming that clearly merit further study. Experimental paradigms in which food intake is systematically manipulated could be usefully brought to bear on exploring these hypotheses.
Ethics rules in our establishments preclude the sharing of data without a formal data-sharing agreement. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the corresponding author.
The studies involving humans were approved by MacEwan University Ethics Review Board (ID: 102049). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
TN: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis, Investigation. CP-D: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – review & editing, Validation. RP: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada grant RGPIN-2018-05065 (NSERC; TN), and Alexander Graham Bell Canada Graduate Scholarship-Doctoral Program (NSERC; CP-D).
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the scientific content of the article.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: