Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1519113 Psychology Original Research To empathize with a group or an individual? Investigating the role of cognitive cost and distress in empathy choice Moche Hajdi 1 2 * Noryd Ulrika 1 Rydén Sara 1 Västfjäll Daniel 1 2 3 1Department of Behavioral Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden 2JEDI-Lab, Department of Behavioral Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden 3Decision Research, Eugene, OR, United States

Edited by: Madson Alan Maximiano-Barreto, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Reviewed by: Juan Carlos Oliveros, Universidad Católica del Maule, Chile

Vitor Duncan Marinho, Federal University of São Carlos, Brazil

*Correspondence: Hajdi Moche, hajdi.moche@liu.se

These authors have contributed equally to this work

07 05 2025 2025 16 1519113 29 10 2024 03 03 2025 Copyright © 2025 Moche, Noryd, Rydén and Västfjäll. 2025 Moche, Noryd, Rydén and Västfjäll

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Why do we feel empathy in certain situations, but not others? Previous research has found that people often avoid empathizing when perceived as costly. This study aims to replicate and extend these findings by investigating whether the choice to empathize differs between a group or an individual. Following the empathy selection task paradigm, participants (N = 296) in a within-subject experiment made a series of free choices (empathy choice) selecting whether to engage in empathy or stay objective concerning a single individual and a group of individuals. They also rated perceived cognitive cost and distress concerning empathizing and staying objective. The results show that participants chose to remain objective more often than empathizing in the individual condition along with rating high levels of cognitive effort and distress. In contrast, participants in the group condition more often chose to empathize despite also rating it as more effortful and distressing. We discuss the importance of contextual factors as a main contributor to the difference in empathy choice between group and individual targets.

empathy empathy choice decision making cognitive cost distress empathy selection task emotion regulation section-at-acceptance Personality and Social Psychology

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      When do we empathize with other people and why? Although researchers across disciplines such as economics (Singer and Fehr, 2005), neuroscience (Decety, 2011), philosophy (Prinz, 2011), and psychology (Bloom, 2017) have attempted to understand these questions, there are still no clear answers. In this paper, we aim to add to the understanding of when and why people are willing to empathize with others by investigating the differences between empathizing with a single individual versus a group of individuals. Specifically, this paper aims to replicate and extend the findings of Cameron et al. (2019, 2022) who found that people often avoid empathizing when perceived as costly.

      The paradigm of Cameron et al. (2019, 2022) focuses on empathy regulation, measured by the empathy selection task (EST) (Cameron et al., 2019). This framework revolves around the idea that empathy is a motivated phenomenon, suggesting that the motivation to empathize depends on the subjective expected value (i.e., people weigh the costs against the rewards; Cameron, 2018; Zaki, 2014). Thus, the idea is that people avoid empathy-eliciting situations when empathy is judged to cost time, money, or—most relevant for this line of research—cognitive or emotional effort. The EST is a behavioral measure designed to investigate the avoidance of empathy according to this framework. The EST uses free choice to assess the tendency to empathize versus staying objective. Participants are presented with two card decks—one representing to empathize and one representing to stay objective—over repeated trials and asked to choose which one they prefer before being shown a photo of a person. If choosing to empathize, participants are instructed to share in the experiences of the person and indicate their internal experiences. If choosing to stay objective, participants are instructed to remain detached and indicate the person’s external features. The willingness to empathize (or not) is measured as the proportion of empathy choice (vs. staying objective) in the EST.

      In previous studies within this framework, the EST variations have mainly been about changes in the instructions or of the individual’s facial expressions (Cameron et al., 2019), or whether the person to empathize with is a human or an animal (Cameron et al., 2022). However, our paper will extend this paradigm to a novel context—the willingness to empathize when the target is either a single person or a group of people. This study will therefore contribute to the understanding of how people assess and react to opportunities to empathize with others, with a particular focus on what happens when the target goes from being an individual to being a group. This is especially relevant to investigate as previous research has shown that people perceive (Hamilton and Sherman, 1996) and respond (Butts et al., 2019; Västfjäll et al., 2014) differently to groups than they do to single individuals. For example, both affect and monetary donations are sometimes higher to a single individual than to a group (Kogut and Ritov, 2005a; Moche et al., 2022; Slovic, 2007). Thus, extending the EST paradigm to single vs. group recipients can have a bearing on how we understand people’s reaction and response to situations where not just single individuals, but several people are affected, such as in mass suffering situations (e.g., multiple victims of a disease, war, or natural disasters). Thus, differences in empathy choice between an individual and a group could possibly explain some of these previous findings but possibly also shed some light on how to resolve situations where our emotional response—and in turn, empathy—does not follow a linear increase for the number of people affected (i.e., psychophysical numbing; Slovic, 2007; Moche, 2022).

      Empathy

      The term empathy today applies to several phenomena (Batson, 2009) and many definitions have been offered. In a recent review of reviews of how empathy has been defined, the authors concluded that there has been progress during the past decades in defining the concept, with many review papers highlighting four overlapping aspects of empathy (Eklund and Meranius, 2021). The four aspects of empathy were there defined as (1) understanding, (2) feeling, and (3) sharing in another person’s world, while (4) maintaining a self-other differentiation. In the empathy choice paradigm adapted in this paper, the focus in on the third aspect—the sharing in another person’s world. This has also been suggested to be the core of a standard definition of empathy (although this aspect also requires an understanding, or identification, of the other person’s world; Coll et al., 2017).

      Further, the two central parts of empathy are the emotional sharing part (also referred to as affective empathy), meaning to subjectively experience and share in another’s psychological state or feelings (i.e., the second and third aspects in the definition above), and the perspective-taking part (also referred to as cognitive empathy), meaning to identify and understand another person’s feelings and perspective from an objective stance (i.e., the first and fourth aspects in the definition above; Decety and Cowell, 2014; Jeffrey, 2016). Although these two parts are interconnected in an empathic response (Coll et al., 2017), in our study, we focus primarily on empathy as experience sharing, since it is often at the center of debates about empathy (Bloom, 2017; Västfjäll et al., 2017), but importantly, because it is central in the methodological approach used by Cameron et al. (2019, 2022), which we apply. In the EST, participants who have chosen the empathy choice are instructed to share in the feelings of the target person (even though other facets of empathy can also become active in this task, as mentioned above, and also noted by the original authors; Cameron et al., 2019). Henceforth, we will use the word “empathy” as short for sharing internal experiences, measured as the number of empathy choices in the EST.

      Effort and empathy

      Although empathy is often considered important for altruism and social cognition (e.g., Batson, 2010), people also avoid empathizing when perceiving it as costly (Cameron et al., 2019, 2022). Previous research showed that empathy can be perceived as costly in different ways, such as when it includes an economic cost, requires time, or creates emotions of negativity (Andreoni et al., 2017; Cameron et al., 2016). The line of research using the EST has also identified that when people are given the choice to empathize with a single unknown individual, the majority decide to avoid it by staying objective, while experiencing high levels of cognitive cost and effort (Cameron et al., 2019, 2022).

      According to the motivated empathy perspective (Cameron, 2018; Zaki, 2014), the reason why people decide to empathize or turn away in a particular situation depends on their assessment of the costs and benefits of empathizing. These cost–benefit assessments may involve financial, material, emotional, and social costs and rewards (e.g., reputation), but also cognitive factors, such as effort or ease felt by empathizing (Cameron, 2018; Zaki, 2014). The cost/benefit assessment can affect whether a person decides to approach (vs. avoid) empathy by using emotion regulation strategies (Gross and Thompson, 2007). In this study, we will investigate the cognitive and emotional factors of ease, effort, and distress associated with the act of empathizing. Further, we also look at participants’ perceived efficacy when performing the tasks, since this can be relevant for the experienced effort.

      The emotional regulation strategy in focus for the EST is situational control. According to emotion regulation theories, people use strategies to adjust their emotions, such as how to express or experience them (Gross and Thompson, 2007). Situational control, as the specific strategy tested in the EST, involves adjusting one’s behavior or actions to make it more (or less) likely to find oneself in a situation that will give rise to desirable (or undesirable) emotions. This can for example entail keeping a distance from volunteers in the streets looking for donations or switching the channel to avoid hearing news about groups of people suffering. In our study, we investigate how participants exercise this strategy when empathizing with others, when “others” implies either a single individual or a group of individuals.

      Empathizing with individuals and groups

      People seem to perceive individuals and groups differently, which can affect the choice to empathize with either one of them. For example, Hamilton and Sherman (1996) found that a single individual is viewed as more concrete, unitary, coherent, consistent, and entitative than a group. Thus, when choosing to empathize with a single individual versus a group, the results could go in two opposite directions.

      One of the possible directions is that people will empathize more with a group than with an individual. As mentioned, an individual is perceived as a more coherent and consistent unit, leading to individuals eliciting more attention, elaborate processing, perspective-taking, and affect compared to a group of people (Hamilton and Sherman, 1996). Thus, the extra demand on information processing for an individual might elicit less willingness to empathize compared to a group. Further, Kogut and Ritov (2005b) found that participants seeing a single identified victim expressed significantly more distress than participants seeing a group of identified victims. Feelings of distress may evoke an egoistic motivation to reduce one’s aversive arousal (Cialdini et al., 1987), possibly leading people to turn away from what causes these emotions and thereby lead to less willingness to empathize with an individual compared to a group. Similarly, according to findings on compassion fade, people presented with one victim respond with more affect than those being presented with a larger number of victims (Butts et al., 2019; Moche et al., 2022; Slovic, 2007; Västfjäll et al., 2014). Last, it might be harder to empathize with an individual because of less external information. A group can give more context information and thereby make empathizing less effortful and error-prone (Dunn et al., 2017). In sum, these findings suggest that it is more cognitively and emotionally demanding to empathize with an individual, which would mean that people are more likely to empathize with a group than with an individual.

      The other possible direction is that people will empathize more with an individual compared to a group. For example, as one identified individual is more likely to arouse affective reactions than a group of individuals (Kogut and Ritov, 2005b; Västfjäll et al., 2014), it could be argued that empathizing with the individual would be experienced as easier since it happens more naturally. Further, a group of people has the potential to elicit stronger emotional responses than a single individual due to the overwhelming amount of affective information. Because of this, Cameron and Payne (2011) suggested that our emotion regulation system proactively downregulates our emotional response when introduced to an increased number of victims (but see also Hagman et al., 2022). Thus, avoiding to empathize with the group can be viewed as an active attempt to regulate and lessen emotions that are seen as cognitively or emotionally costly. Last, there is also a greater risk of making empathic errors in judgment of a group’s inner emotional state compared to a single individual’s emotional state, which also supports this direction (Dunn et al., 2017). In sum, these lines of reasoning would mean that participants more often would choose to empathize with the individual than the group. To be able to arbitrate between these two possible predictions, we will directly pit empathy choice between individual and group targets against each other.

      Hypotheses

      First, we expect that empathy is experienced as more cognitively costly than staying objective, and therefore the modal response in the EST will be to stay objective, in both the group and the single condition (hypothesis 1). This hypothesis is based on the findings from Cameron et al. (2019, 2022), who found that people are more likely to choose to stay objective than to empathize in the EST and that this is due to the increased cognitive costs associated with empathy choice. A possibly important distinction here is that avoiding empathy does not necessarily equate to staying objective in a real-life situation (e.g., one could focus on something completely else than the other person to avoid empathy), but since these are the two options pinned against each other in this particular paradigm and in the EST that we employ, we focus on these.

      Second, as the EST-studies conducted so far not has compared empathy choices for single individuals with groups, and as outlined above, there are two possible directions of hypotheses regarding this comparison: A greater proportion of participants will choose to empathize in the group condition compared to the single condition (Hypothesis 2a), or a greater proportion of participants will choose to empathize in the single condition compared to the group condition (Hypothesis 2b). These hypotheses, along with our methodological approach and analyses were pre-registered through AsPredicted,1 before data collection.

      Methods Participants

      Using G*Power, we calculated the number of participants needed in the study with our preferred power (0.80) and effect size (f = 0.2), yielding 266 participants. Taking possible dropouts into account, we aimed to recruit around 280–300 participants. Participants had to be American, over 18 years old, and speak English fluently. A total of 310 people from Prolific participated in the survey. Exclusion criteria were a failed attention check (N = 12) or not following the instructions of the task (N = 2), for example by using nonsense words (e.g., “cool whatever bro”). The final data set consisted of 296 participants (48% men, 50% women, 2% other, Mage = 39.2, SD = 15.09). 55.1% of the participants held a bachelor’s degree or higher.

      Design

      The study had a 2 (Card deck: feel vs. describe) × 2 (Picture block: single individual vs. group of individuals) within-subject design. Participants performed a modified version of the EST (Cameron et al., 2019, 2022), where they in addition to only making choices regarding one individual also made choices about a group. All participants also rated a number of items (described below). Completing the full survey took about 28 min and each participant was monetarily compensated for taking part in the experiment. The survey stimuli and exact instructions can be found in the Supplementary materials.

      Procedure

      The study started with a short introduction and a consent form. Subsequently, participants answered an attention check, followed by the EST. The survey consisted of two main blocks of 20 different sets of pictures each. One block had pictures of a single individual and one block had pictures of a group of individuals. The pictures used in the two conditions were all carefully selected from a website selling stock photos and vectors.2 When choosing pictures for the different blocks we opted for pictures with both men and women, with different ethnicities, from a similar age group (i.e., 30–40-year-olds), and with neutral facial expressions. These choices were made to attempt to control for things like effects of experienced racial in-group on empathy (Xu et al., 2009) and how different facial expressions might affect the tendency to empathize (Eisenberg, 1986). The choice to go for neutral facial expressions was primarily based on Cameron et al. (2022), who used neutral facial expressions in their studies—with similar results in empathy choice as when using more emotional facial expressions (i.e., happy or sad facial expressions; Cameron et al., 2019). We also decided to keep all facial expressions neutral to eliminate variations, such as how different facial expressions might affect the tendency to empathize. Last, neutral facial expressions were also chosen to minimize stimulus differences in the pictures between the individual task and the group task. However, it was easier to find pictures of neutral faces for the single individual block than the group block, since pictures of groups of people tended to show a variation of facial expressions. This resulted in a broader range of emotion displayed in the pictures of the groups compared to the individuals.

      To control for order effect, the starting block (individual vs. group) was randomly chosen. Each block had specific instructions and a test trial for participants to practice on (see Supplementary materials). If participants answered wrongly during the test trial, they were given two additional chances to answer to make sure they understood the instructions. Then, participants were given the choice to pick one out of two decks of cards, one named “feel” (i.e., to empathize) and one named “describe” (i.e., to stay objective). The instructions were the same in both the individual block and group block, requesting them to “choose one of the decks.” i.e. feel or describe. After making this choice, participants saw a picture (either a single individual or a group of individuals, depending on which block they were in) and were asked to write down three keywords that either described the inner state (instructions for the feel deck) or the external features (instructions for the describe deck) of the individual(s). See Figure 1 for a visualization of the task. Participants were able to move on to the next deck of cards after 8 s. This time limit was added to limit the risk that participants were disengaged in the task and just went through the survey without taking the time to think. A time limit was also used in the study by Cameron et al. (2019, 2022). The participants were then repeatedly presented with the “feel” and “describe” decks of cards for a total number of 20 sets, showing different pictures in each set.

      A schematic overview of survey flow for the individual block. Participants chose “feel” or “describe” and were then shown the picture with the instructions paired with their choice of card deck. Adapted with permission from https://www.123rf.com.

      After completing a block, participants were asked to answer a set of questions from the NASA Task Load Index (Hart and Staveland, 1988) assessing cognitive effort, aversion, and efficacy, followed by an additional question to assess distress. Participants were asked to rate the questions with regards to the previously completed deck. This was done twice after each block, once regarding the feel deck, and once regarding the describe deck. When the questions were all rated, the first block was finished and participants could move on to the second block, which repeated the same procedure as mentioned above. Last, after the two main blocks were completed, participants answered a block of demographic questions. This included questions about age, gender, education, and place of residence.

      Measures Primary measure

      Our primary measure was the proportion of each chosen card deck, i.e., choosing to empathize or staying objective over the repeated trials. The choice proportion of the empathizing card deck, compared against chance (0.5), will be the main variable to test our first hypothesis (once for the individual block and once for the group block). For the second hypothesis, the proportion of choosing to empathize in the individual block will be compared to the proportion of choosing to empathize in the group block.

      Secondary measures

      Our secondary measures were “cognitive cost” and “distress” related to the choice of card deck in the EST. Questions measuring cognitive cost were taken and adapted from the NASA Task Load Index (Hart and Staveland, 1988). These measures were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (=Very low) to 5 (=Very high). The two questions assessing effort concerned mental demand (“How mentally demanding was this deck?”) and effort (“How hard did you have to work to accomplish your level of performance with this deck?”), which we, using the approach from Cameron et al. (2019), combined. All samples on effort showed a good internal consistency with results above 𝛼 = 0.80. One question measured aversion (“How insecure, discouraged, irritated, stressed, and annoyed were you by this deck?”), and one question measured efficacy (“How successful were you in accomplishing what you were asked to do in this deck?”). As for the question on distress, it was taken and adapted from Kogut and Ritov (2005b). It was measured on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (=Very low) to 7 (=Very high). The question was: “How worried, upset and sad were you when seeing each person/the groups of people?.” For each of four secondary measures, the mean will be compared between the feel deck vs. describe deck, as well as between the individual vs. group block (and possible interactions).

      Results Primary analyses

      In our primary analysis, where we wanted to investigate whether the choice to empathize would be avoided most often, we conducted two one-sample t-tests. These t-tests compared the choice of empathizing against chance (0.5) in each of the two blocks (group, individual; as was done by Cameron et al., 2022).3 See Table 1 for descriptives and the results of the t-tests. In the individual block, participants showed a significant preference to avoid empathy choice (i.e., choosing to stay objective more often). In contrast, participants in the group block showed a significant preference for the empathy choice compared to staying objective. Our first hypothesis, which stated that the modal response in the EST will be to stay objective (i.e., avoid empathizing), was therefore confirmed for the individual pictures, but not for the group pictures.

      Tests of empathy choice against chance by task block.

      Picture M SD t p 95% CI difference Hedges’ g
      Group of individuals 0.53 0.28 1.2 0.047 [0.00, 0.06] 0.12
      Single individual 0.34 0.27 −10.17 <0.001 [−0.19, −0.13] −0.59

      Df of t is 295. Choice (M) refers to the proportion of times the card deck “feel” was selected out of the total number of choices (20) presented to the participant. Hedges’ g reflects effect deviation of choice from chance (0.5) for one test.

      Further, to explore if there was a significant difference between the two conditions (group, individual) in empathy choice, as highlighted in our second hypothesis, we conducted a paired sample t-test. The result showed a significant difference in empathy choice, t(295) = 12.04, p < 0.001, g = 0.28. Participants decided to empathize with the group to a greater proportion compared to the individual. This thus supports hypothesis 2a.

      Secondary analyses

      Second, we wanted to explore possible explanations to these result by looking at the ratings of effort, aversion, efficacy, and distress. We conducted three separate two-way ANOVAs for effort, aversion, and efficacy, respectively, with the independent variables of card deck (feel, describe) and picture block (individual, group). The data regarding the fourth dependent variable, distress, was not normally distributed and therefore did not fit the criteria for conducting a parametric analysis. The Friedman test was therefore used as a non-parametric replacement for the ANOVA. The descriptive statistics of the variables can be seen in Table 2.

      Mean and standard deviation of cognitive cost variables (effort, aversion, efficacy) and the distress variable for describe and feel choices for both the single and group blocks.

      Single individual Group of individuals
      Describe Feel Describe Feel
      Effort 2.74 (1.17) 3.56 (1.11) 3.30 (1.11) 3.46 (1.07)
      Aversion 2.04 (1.11) 2.60 (1.26) 2.56 (1.37) 2.54 (1.30)
      Efficacy 3.87 (0.98) 3.37 (1.14) 3.57 (1.06) 3.62 (1.04)
      Distress 2.00 (1.39) 2.51 (1.61) 2.21 (1.56) 2.59 (1.75)
      Cognitive cost Effort

      The two-way ANOVA for effort showed two significant main effects and one significant interaction effect. The significant main effect of deck showed that participants rated the feel deck as significantly more effortful than the describe deck, F(1, 295) = 62.44, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.18. There was also a main effect of picture, where participants rated the group block as significantly more effortful than the individual block, F(1, 295) = 21.74, p < 0.001. ηp2 = 0.07. Last and more importantly, there was a significant interaction effect, F(1, 295) = 49.83, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15. A Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparison showed that participants rated the describe deck in the group block as more effortful compared to the individual block (p < 0.001), whereas there was no significant difference between blocks for the feel deck (p = 0.111). The pairwise comparison also showed that the feel deck was rated more effortful compared to the describe block, both in the individual (p < 0.001) and the group block (p = 0.031).

      Efficacy

      The two-way ANOVA for perceived efficacy showed a significant main effect and an interaction effect. The significant main effect of deck showed that participants rated significantly higher efficacy for the describe deck than the feel deck, F(1, 295) = 15.78, p < 0.001. ηp2 = 0.05. No significant main effect was found regarding picture, F(1, 295) = 0.23, p = 0.632. More importantly, there was a significant interaction effect, F(1, 295) = 35.22, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.11. A Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparison showed that participants’ efficacy for the describe deck was significantly higher in the individual block than the group block (p < 0.001), whereas it for the feel deck was significantly higher in the group block than the single block (p < 0.001).

      Aversion

      The two-way ANOVA for perceived aversion showed two significant main effects and an interaction effect. The significant main effect of deck showed that participants rated the feel deck as significantly more averse than the describe deck, F(1, 295) = 22.11, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.07. The significant main effect of picture showed that participants rated the group block as significantly more averse than the individual block, F(1, 295) = 18.68, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.06. More importantly, we found a significant interaction effect, F(1, 295) = 38.44, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.115. A Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparison showed that participants rated the describe deck as significantly more averse for the group block than the individual block (p < 0.001), whereas there was no significant difference between blocks for the feel deck (p = 0.361).

      Distress

      Since the data for the distress variable was not normally distributed, we conducted a Friedman test, which is a non-parametric equivalent to a repeated-measures ANOVA. The results showed a significant difference between groups, χ2(3) = 59.38, p < 0.001. A Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparison showed that distress was significantly higher for the feel deck compared to the describe deck, in both the individual block (p < 0.001), and the group block (p < 0.001). This indicates that the feel deck in general elicited higher levels of distress than the describe deck. There was no significant difference between the group and individual block in rated distress for the describe (p = 0.166) or feel deck (p = 0.861).4

      Exploratory analyses

      To try to understand the relationship between our secondary variables, we conducted a Pearson’s correlation. The correlations are found in Table 3. All were significantly correlated, ranging from a small to large effect size (Cohen, 1988).

      Pearson’s correlations between cognitive cost variables and distress.

      Variable M SD 1 2 3
      1. Effort 3.27 1.16
      2. Aversion 2.43 1.28 0.591**
      3. Efficacy 3.61 1.07 −0.198** −0.295**
      4. Distress 2.33 1.60 0.344** 0.443** −0.175**

      **p < 0.01.

      Discussion

      This paper has used an adapted version of EST (developed by Cameron et al., 2019) to investigate how people choose to empathize when presented with a single individual and a group of individuals. We also investigated how experienced cognitive costs and distress were associated with empathy choice. First, we hypothesized that participants more often would avoid empathizing, in both the group and the individual block. This was confirmed in the individual block, but not in the group block. That is, participants chose to stay objective significantly more often than empathizing for individuals, whereas it was the opposite for the group. Further, we found that there was a preference to empathize with the group over the individual, in line with hypothesis 2a. Last, our results for cognitive cost and distress were partially in line with previous findings (Cameron et al., 2019, 2022), such that empathizing was more effortful, aversive, and distressing than staying objective.

      First, when the decision to empathize concerned a single individual, significantly more people decided to stay objective. Thus, our findings replicate those of Cameron et al. (2019). However, this was not the case for a group of individuals. Rather, the opposite was true then, showing that significantly more people decided to empathize with the group over staying objective. More specifically, participants chose the empathy choice on average in 34% of the trials in the single condition, whereas the same participants on average chose the empathy choice in 53% of the trials in the group condition. Although not a strong preference for empathizing (53%), it was still significant and in the opposite direction of our hypothesis and previous research. Further, when specifically comparing the empathy choice for the individual and the group block, it also showed that participants chose to empathize with the group significantly more often than they did for an individual, which taken together supports hypothesis 2a. Possible reasons for these results are discussed related to our secondary measures below.

      In line with Cameron et al. (2019, 2022), we further found that participants experienced the feel deck as significantly more effortful and aversive than the describe deck. Interestingly, participants in the group block rated the feel deck as more effortful than the describe deck but they still chose it more often. This result contrasts with the findings of Cameron et al. (2019) who hypothesized that due to its cognitive costs, higher experienced effort would result in participants choosing to avoid empathizing more often. A possible explanation for our result is the increased amount of information in the group condition. That is, the increased external information in the group condition can lead to more errors in empathic judgment and hence make it feel more effortful (Dunn et al., 2017). At the same time, the group also provides more context information and clues (e.g., body language) to use when processing people’s inner emotional state (Clark et al., 2020), possibly explaining why participants in the group condition still chose to empathize more often. Last, the increased number of external stimuli could make it harder for the participant to process the extra information, hence, rating the describe deck as more effortful for the group than the individual deck. Thus, these results can be understood in relation to description preference. That is, what makes a target easy or difficult to describe? It should be relatively easy to describe the features of a single individual, especially when conveying a neutral facial expression. Supporting this, the mean rating for the describe deck of the individual was relatively small, suggesting participants did indeed experience it as rather effortless. However, it should be harder to describe a group, or a single individual conveying a more emotional facial expression. Thus, the results regarding empathy choice and effort can perhaps best be understood in relation to description preference.

      Further, participants rated themselves to be more efficacious in describing than empathizing. This is possibly due to less likelihood of error when describing objective features in a picture (Dunn et al., 2017). It can also be connected to the scores in effort, where higher efficacy could mean it was felt as less effortful. This is confirmed by the correlations showing a negative correlation between the two variables. Thus, when people feel themselves to be more efficacious, they rate the task as less effortful and aversive. Also, participants perceived themselves to have more efficacy in the individual block than in the group block when choosing to describe, whereas it was the opposite for empathizing (i.e., higher efficacy in the group than the individual block for empathizing). Again, this might be due to more external information in the group condition, making it harder to describe but easier to empathize. The results of efficacy might also explain why participants chose to feel more often in the group condition. That is, even if participants in the group condition felt more effort to empathize, people might have chosen to do what they felt they were good at, despite the effort it also took.

      Looking at the distress variable, participants rated significantly higher levels of distress for the feel deck compared to the describe deck in both the group and individual block. This can be understood by previous research by Cialdini et al. (1987) where feelings of distress may evoke an egoistic motivation to reduce one’s aversive arousal, leading people to turn away from the stimuli causing distress. Regarding the group block, participants chose the feel deck more often even though they also rated it as eliciting higher levels of distress. These results are similar to the ones for effort and aversion (i.e., participants chose the feel deck more often even though they rated it as more effortful and aversive), evidenced by the positive correlations between the variables.

      When looking closer at the feel deck, we found no significant difference in rated distress between the group and individual block with the non-parametric test. To some extent, this contrasts with the results of Kogut and Ritov (2005b) who found that an identified single victim elicited higher distress than a group—which we did not find. However, there are differences between our and their study, such as the context and level of identifiability. Further, our results showing no difference in distress between the group and individual block can possibly be understood by psychophysical numbing. That is, our emotional response to a group does not follow a linear increase from those to a single individual (Slovic, 2007). Thus, a group of people does not necessarily elicit higher levels of distress compared to a single person. However, we acknowledge that our research and that on psychophysical numbing also differ in context (i.e., whether there is a need for help). Also, the distress variable showed a floor effect. This might be due to the neutral faces without context in the blocks, which are less likely to evoke feelings of distress in comparison to, e.g., an identified victim described to be in need of help (Kogut and Ritov, 2005b; Moche et al., 2020, 2024; Västfjäll et al., 2014).

      Limitations and future studies

      When choosing pictures to include, we opted for those with neutral faces. Further, the group pictures showed full bodies compared to the individual pictures with just a headshot. Just as Clark et al. (2020) mention, our emotional interpretations do not exist in a vacuum of only facial expressions but instead happen in a combined evaluation of multiple contextual factors, such as body language and surroundings. This differing aspect may have influenced the results, but it is hard to say how. Another aspect worth mentioning is how neutral facial expressions may affect the tendency to empathize. Clark et al. (2020) argue that when there is an absence of contextual information other than the facial expression of a person, there is greater variance in the interpretation of an emotion. The neutral faces can thus make it hard to identify (or understand) the person’s emotional state and subsequently make it harder to share in those feelings (Coll et al., 2017). A neutral face can be argued to give less information than a smiling or sad face, even though neutral faces still can convey affective information (Gasper, 2018). Often in empathy research, negative facial expressions are used. However, in Cameron et al. (2019), the pictures used were portraits with sad or happy faces and in Cameron et al. (2022) they portrayed neutral facial expressions—but both still gave the same result (i.e., participants avoided empathizing more). However, the faces of children in this context have not yet been studied (compared to adults, e.g., Moche and Västfjäll, 2021), which may be of interest for future studies.

      In one of the studies by Cameron et al. (2022), participants were instructed to choose to empathize with an individual or an animal, but instead of putting them in different blocks to be rated separately, they were pinned against each other (e.g., choose to empathize with the individual or the animal). This differs from the present study. Here, participants were never directly asked if they preferred to empathize with the individual or the group. However, since this was a within-subject design, participants saw both options in a counter-balanced order. Nevertheless, this version could be tested in a future study to see how the results compare against each other, and if ratings of cognitive cost or distress would change.

      Last, adding more context to the EST can be used to see the boundary condition on empathy avoidance. To further the cost–benefit analysis that is discussed, the added contextual information can also vary, such as information about the economic, material, and social costs to examine the robustness of the cognitive cost variable.

      Conclusion

      The purpose of this study was to replicate and extend the findings of Cameron et al. (2019, 2022) by testing if the choice to empathize differs between a group and an individual, and if so, how and why. Our results in the individual condition were similar to previous findings, such that people more often chose to stay objective over empathizing. Empathizing was also rated as more cognitively costly and distressing. However, participants in the group condition more often chose to empathize over staying objective, despite also rating it as more effortful and distressing. This might be due to higher ratings of efficacy at empathizing in the group block compared to the individual block, but also due to more contextual information in the group condition. This study shows that it may not only be the cognitive cost variable that affects the decision to empathize, but to a degree, so may feelings of distress. Our study is the first to compare empathy choice for individuals and groups and the results provide another nuance in explaining empathy and what may or may not motivate it.

      Data availability statement

      The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: OSF-website (https://osf.io/7vskg/).

      Ethics statement

      Ethical approval was not required for the studies involving humans because no sensitive personal information was collected that could be linked to specific persons. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

      Author contributions

      HM: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. UN: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. DV: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

      Funding

      The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. Support for this research was provided by the Swedish Research Council (grant number 2020-02788) and KAW foundation (grant number 2023.0289), granted to DV. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Generative AI statement

      The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Supplementary material

      The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1519113/full#supplementary-material

      1https://aspredicted.org/L6D_YQL

      2https://www.123rf.com/

      3The pre-registration made for this study stated that we would conduct three separate two-way ANOVAs and one sample t-test. However, we realized that we in fact needed to do t-tests in each block (group, individual) to compare them with chance, and then a paired t-test to test if the difference in empathy choice between the blocks (group, individual) was significant.

      4For comparison, we also did four Wilcoxon signed rank tests. The Wilcoxon signed rank tests showed the same comparisons as the Friedman test to be significant. It also found a significantly higher rating in distress for the describe deck in the group block compared to the individual block (p = 0.007). Since the Wilcoxon test is slightly less conservative than the Friedman test, this result is not surprising. Significance levels for the Wilcoxon tests were Bonferroni-corrected to reduce the risk of type I error.

      References Andreoni J. Rao J. M. Trachtman H. (2017). Avoiding the ask: a field experiment on altruism, empathy, and charitable giving. J. Polit. Econ. 125, 625653. doi: 10.1086/691703 Batson C. D. (2009). “These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena” in The social neuroscience of empathy. eds. Decety J. Ickes W. (Cambridge: MIT Press), 315. Batson C. D. (2010). Altruism in humans: the empathy-altruism hypothesis. New York: Oxford University. Bloom P. (2017). Empathy and its discontents. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 2431. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.004, PMID: 27916513 Butts M. M. Lunt D. C. Freling T. L. Gabriel A. S. (2019). Helping one or helping many? A theoretical integration and meta-analytic review of the compassion fade literature. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 151, 1633. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.12.006 Cameron C. D. (2018). Motivating empathy: three methodological recommendations for mapping empathy. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 12:e12418. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12418 Cameron C. D. Harris L. T. Payne B. K. (2016). The emotional cost of humanity: anticipated exhaustion motivates dehumanization of stigmatized targets. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 105112. doi: 10.1177/1948550615604453 Cameron C. D. Lengieza M. L. Hadjiandreou E. Swim K. J. Chiles M. R. (2022). Empathic choices for animals versus humans: the role of choice context and perceived cost. J. Soc. Psychol. 162, 161177. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2021.1997890, PMID: 35037571 Cameron C. D. Payne B. K. (2011). Escaping affect: how motivated emotion regulation creates insensitivity to mass suffering. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 115. doi: 10.1037/a0021643, PMID: 21219076 Cameron C. D. Scheffer A. J. Hadjiandreou E. Hutcherson A. C. Ferguson M. A. Inzlicht M. (2019). Empathy is hard work: people choose to avoid empathy because of its cognitive costs. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148, 962976. doi: 10.1037/xge0000595, PMID: 30998038 Cialdini R. B. Schaller M. Houlihan D. Arps K. Fultz J. Beaman A. L. (1987). Empathy-based helping: is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 749758. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.4.749, PMID: 3572736 Clark G. M. McNeel C. Bigelow F. J. Enticott P. G. (2020). The effect of empathy and context on face-processing ERPs. Neuropsychologia 147:107612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107612, PMID: 32882241 Cohen J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Edn. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Coll M. P. Viding E. Rütgen M. Silani G. Lamm C. Catmur C. . (2017). Are we really measuring empathy? Proposal for a new measurement framework. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83, 132139. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.009, PMID: 29032087 Decety J. (2011). Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emot. Rev. 3, 92108. doi: 10.1177/1754073910374662 Decety J. Cowell M. J. (2014). Friends or foes: is empathy necessary for moral behavior? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 525537. doi: 10.1177/1745691614545130, PMID: 25429304 Dunn T. L. Inzlicht M. Risko E. F. (2017). Anticipating cognitive effort: roles of perceived error-likelihood and time demands. Psychol. Res. 83, 10331056. doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0943-x, PMID: 29134281 Eisenberg N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. London: Taylor & Francis Group. Eklund J. H. Meranius M. S. (2021). Toward a consensus on the nature of empathy: a review of reviews. Patient Educ. Couns. 104, 300307. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2020.08.022, PMID: 32888755 Gasper K. (2018). Utilizing neutral affective states in research: theory, assessment, and recommendations. Emot. Rev. 10, 255266. doi: 10.1177/1754073918765660 Gross J. J. Thompson R. A. (2007). “Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations” in Handbook of emotion regulation. ed. Gross J. J. (New York: Guilford Press), 324. Hagman W. Tinghög G. Dickert S. Slovic P. Västfjäll D. (2022). Motivated down-regulation of emotion and compassion collapse revisited. Front. Psychol. 13:801150. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.801150, PMID: 35911053 Hamilton L. D. Sherman J. S. (1996). Perceiving persons and groups. Psychol. Rev. 103, 336355. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.336, PMID: 8637962 Hart S. G. Staveland L. E. (1988). Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv. Psychol. 52, 139183. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62386-9 Jeffrey D. (2016). Empathy, sympathy and compassion in healthcare: is there a problem? Is there a difference? Does it matter? J. R. Soc. Med. 109, 446452. doi: 10.1177/0141076816680120, PMID: 27923897 Kogut T. Ritov I. (2005a). The singularity effect of identified victims in separate and joint evaluations. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 97, 106116. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.02.003 Kogut T. Ritov I. (2005b). The “identified victim” effect: an identified group, or just a single individual? J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 18, 157167. doi: 10.1002/bdm.492 Moche H. (2022). Unequal valuations of lives and what to do about it: the role of identifiability, numbers, and age in charitable giving. [Ph.D. dissertation,: Linköping University. Moche H. Erlandsson A. Andersson D. Västfjäll D. (2020). Opportunity cost in monetary donation decisions to non-identified and identified victims. Front. Psychol. 10:3035. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03035, PMID: 32038400 Moche H. Gordon-Hecker T. Kogut T. Västfjäll D. (2022). Thinking, good and bad? Deliberative thinking and the singularity effect in charitable giving. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 17, 1430. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500009001 Moche H. Karlsson H. Västfjäll D. (2024). Victim identifiability, numbers of victims, and unit asking in charitable giving. PLoS One 19:e0300863. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0300863, PMID: 38547164 Moche H. Västfjäll D. (2021). Helping the child or the adult? Systematically testing the identifiable victim effect for child and adult victims. Soc. Influ. 16, 7892. doi: 10.1080/15534510.2021.1995482 Prinz J. (2011). Against empathy. South. J. Philos. 49, 214233. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-6962.2011.00069.x Singer T. Fehr E. (2005). The neuroeconomics of mind reading and empathy. Am. Econ. Rev. 95, 340345. doi: 10.1257/000282805774670103, PMID: 29125271 Slovic P. (2007). “If I look at the mass I will never act”: psychic numbing and genocide. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2, 7995. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500000061 Västfjäll D. Erlandsson A. Slovic P. Tinghög G. (2017). Commentary: empathy and its discontents. Front. Psychol. 8:542. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00542, PMID: 28458644 Västfjäll D. Slovic P. Mayorga M. Peters E. (2014). Compassion fade: affect and charity are greatest for a single child in need. PLoS One 9:e100115. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0100115, PMID: 24940738 Xu X. Zuo X. Wang X. Han S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. J. Neurosci. 29, 85258529. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2418-09.2009, PMID: 19571143 Zaki J. (2014). Empathy: a motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140, 16081647. doi: 10.1037/a0037679, PMID: 25347133
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.haztcm.com.cn
      www.kesilai.com.cn
      www.guasheng.net.cn
      www.gidnht.com.cn
      haztcm.org.cn
      ibigshit.com.cn
      www.mununo.org.cn
      woooyoo.com.cn
      manytour.net.cn
      www.marsovin.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p