Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1462980 Psychology Original Research How does playfulness (re)frame the world? Evidence for selective cognitive and behavioral redirecting in times of adversity Shen Xiangyou 1 * Crawley Zoe 2 1Forest Ecosystems and Society, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, United States 2Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, United States

Edited by: Konrad Schnabel, International Psychoanalytic University Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by: Kay Brauer, Martin Luther University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany

Jessica Wells, Boise State University, United States

*Correspondence: Xiangyou Shen, sharon.shen@oregonstate.edu
10 02 2025 2024 15 1462980 11 07 2024 13 11 2024 Copyright © 2025 Shen and Crawley. 2025 Shen and Crawley

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Introduction

Do playful people perceive, approach, and respond to their environment and life events differently than less playful individuals? While playfulness has been theorized to affect how individuals frame or reframe situations, this widely accepted premise lacks theoretical specification and empirical validation. This study examined playfulness as a perceptual lens and its potential broader (re)framing effects spanning cognition, emotion, and behavior in the disruptive pandemic context.

Methods

Two groups with contrasting levels of playfulness (high vs. low as measured by the Adult Playfulness Trait Scale) were derived from a nationwide US adult sample (n = 503) and compared across 19 criterion variables representing diverse perceptual, emotional, and behavioral responses during COVID-19. Sequential analyses including MANOVA, ANOVA, and ANCOVA were performed to examine overall, univariate, and adjusted group differences, respectively, validated by sensitivity analysis across three group categorization methods.

Results

Three sets of contrasting findings evidenced selective playful (re)framing effects, wherein more playful individuals (1) shared similar perceptions of current risk and protective factors while adopting a more optimistic future outlook, (2) perceived similar levels of vulnerability and isolation but engaged in significantly higher levels of resilient coping and adaptive leisure, and (3) participated in similar categories and frequencies of leisure activities but with higher experiential quality, marked by greater immersion, activeness, and positive affect.

Discussion

Playfulness functions as a “color spotlight” rather than “rose-tinted glasses,” with selective influence through “lemonading”—creatively imagining and pursuing positive possibilities to cultivate adaptive, enjoyable experiences while maintaining a clear-eyed realism about challenges. This advances a nuanced understanding of playful (re)framing as operating primarily through intrinsic goal-oriented cognitive and behavioral redirecting, underscoring playfulness’ potential as an integrative resilience factor, experiential quality amplifier, and character strength for promoting individual flourishing.

adult playfulness playful reframing lemonading resilient coping cognitive and behavioral redirecting experiential quality adaptive engagement color spotlight effect section-at-acceptance Personality and Social Psychology

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      When studying how and why well-being changes, researchers may wish to focus less on nominating specific life events that could alter these trajectories, and instead turn to understanding the individual differences that influence our interpretation of these events (Hill et al., 2014, p. 248).

      Introduction

      In the context of personality psychology, playfulness can be conceptualized as a multifaceted disposition comprising interconnected motivational and cognitive propensities, characterized by fun-oriented intrinsic motivation, uninhibitedness, and spontaneity, which collectively predispose an individual to engage in playful behavior (Shen et al., 2014a). As a highly permeating trait network, playfulness has the potential to influence behavior across wide-ranging situations and life domains, with profound implications for diverse individual and social experiences and outcomes (Shen, 2020). Cumulative empirical evidence from recent decades suggest that more playful individuals generally function better, maintain better health, and experience greater happiness than their less playful counterpart. Specifically, correlational studies have revealed significant associations between heightened playfulness and various favorable workplace outcomes, including increased productivity (Martocchio and Webster, 1992), creativity and innovation (Felsman et al., 2020; Lee et al., 2021), and job satisfaction (Yu et al., 2007). Young adults characterized by higher levels of playfulness reported greater emotional intelligence (Holmes and Hart, 2022), better academic performance (Proyer, 2011), and higher adaptability across multiple domains, from learning and problem-solving to handling stress and uncertainty (Shen et al., 2017). Other studies have documented positive links between adult playfulness and physical fitness (Proyer et al., 2018), mental health (Erez et al., 2016), subjective happiness (Yue et al., 2016), and life satisfaction (Brauer et al., 2024) across wide age ranges.

      Despite increasing recognition of playfulness’ potential to enhance adult performance and wellbeing, empirical inquiries into the underlying mechanisms of this potential remain sparse. A recent integrative review by Shen and Masek (2023) elucidated a central pathway wherein playful engagement serves as a mediator or medium leading to positive mental health outcomes in adults. Their analysis revealed that the emerging body of playfulness-related intervention literature predominantly focuses on the playing process as a catalyst for positive change. However, as a highly permeating dispositional quality that may activate across diverse life domains and settings, playfulness likely influences individuals’ performance and wellbeing through multiple pathways as it interacts with other personal factors and various aspects of the environment (Shen, 2020). Among these possible pathways, scarce research has examined a putative mechanism suggested by what is often cited as a definition of playfulness:

      Playfulness is the predisposition to frame or reframe [emphasis added] a situation in such a way [emphasis added] as to provide oneself (and possibly others) with amusement, humor, and/or entertainment (Barnett, 2007, p. 955).

      Proyer et al. (2018) proposed a variation of the above statement, describing adult playfulness as “a personality trait that enables people to frame or reframe [emphasis added] everyday situations in such a way [emphasis added] that they experience them as entertaining, intellectually stimulating, or personally interesting” (p. 1). Despite their slightly different wording, both propositions imply that playfulness acts as a transformative device through which the external environment are framed or reframed in ways that facilitate enjoyment or engagement. However, neither statement specifies the exact nature or mechanism of this framing effect.

      The premise of playful (re)framing embedded in Barnett’s (2007) and Proyer et al.’s (2018) propositions is often accepted without question, yet its merit has not undergone rigorous empirical validation. With much unknown and ambiguities packed in the phrase “in such a way,” both propositions leave the precise nature and formulation of playful framing or reframing vague and open to interpretation. It remains unclear whether they conceptualize playful (re)framing as a perceptual lens that shapes perceptions, a cognitive filter that influences interpretations, or a broader mechanism that extends beyond cognitive encoding. Furthermore, when treated as definitions, these propositions lack direct correspondence with their respective playfulness measurements (e.g., the Playfulness Scale for Young Adults, Barnett, 2007; the Other-directed-Light-hearted-Intelligent-Whimsical Model, Proyer, 2017), revealing an incongruence between conceptualization and operationalization.

      While empirical validation of playful (re)framing is lacking, the broader literature on personality research has examined how personality traits influence framing effects. This body of work has primarily focused on how personality predicts individuals’ susceptibility to externally generated framing effects, such as those produced by information manipulation (Levin et al., 2002; Anderson, 2010; Gamliel et al., 2014). Existing studies typically conceptualize personality as a moderator of framing effects, where framing is operationalized as an externally-imposed environmental stimulus. This approach addresses a fundamentally different research question than the intrinsic (re)framing mechanism attributed to playfulness by Barnett (2007) and Proyer et al. (2018), which emphasizes playful individuals’ innate tendency to frame situations in ways that enhance their experiences.

      A more pertinent framework for investigating playful (re)framing is offered by Shen’s (2020) interactionist model, which addresses potential playful (re)framing effects more explicitly in the forms of (1)situation perceptions—psychologically meaningful situations that directly shape play behavior while being influenced by playfulness and/or other individual attributes, and (2) playfulness’ direct influence on behavioral expressions. The model calls for empirical validation of playfulness’ role in shaping both situation perceptions and subsequent behaviors, highlighting the need to understand the extent and nature of potential playful (re)framing effects.

      Many questions remain regarding the forms and boundaries of playfulness’ presumed (re)framing effect. For instance, if playfulness serves as a perceptual lens, does its effect apply to perceptions of all situations, thereby functioning like a pair of rose-tinted glasses? Or does (re)framing only occur in a select subset of situations, resembling a color spotlight that illuminates certain aspects of an environment while leaving others unaffected? Moreover, it is crucial to discern whether a broader cascade effect emerges, whereby playful (re)framing extends beyond circumscribed cognitive encoding processes to engender directional shifts in behavioral and emotional responses. For instance, does playfulness catalyze engagement in playful behavior (e.g., by lowering behavioral thresholds for play, Grosul and Feist, 2014) to dynamically reshape ongoing experiences along with environmental factors and situational perceptions, collectively reframing the unfolding experience for both the player and those with whom they interact (Shen, 2020)? Elucidating the depth and breadth of this presumptive chain spanning cognitions, emotions, and behaviors constitutes a critical area for empirical investigation.

      Given scant evidence substantiating or specifying the contours of playful (re)framing, there is a pressing need to scrutinize the existence and functioning of playfulness’ potential transformative effect. Testing this widely accepted yet empirically unvalidated effect would reveal whether our popular conception of playfulness is supported by evidence, and potentially refine or redefine theoretical understandings of playfulness and its functioning. Specifically, this line of research could enrich our understanding on (1) how playfulness might serve as a cognitive filter through which individuals perceive, interpret, and make sense of their world (Crum et al., 2013; Neisser, 1976) to modulate subjective experiences of the environment or events, and (2) to what extent playfulness propels individuals to actively construct or reconstruct their living environment and life experiences. Answers to these questions will illuminate broader issues such as whether and how more playful people perceive, approach, and respond to the world differently than less playful individuals.

      In this study, we make an initial effort to address these knowledge gaps by investigating playfulness’ potential (re)framing effect, broadly conceptualized to encompass cognitive, affective, and behavioral dimensions. We accomplish this by examining whether individuals with contrasting levels of playfulness differ in their perceptual, emotional, and behavioral responses when exposed to similar macro-environmental conditions and events. We conduct our inquiry in the context of COVID-19 pandemic, a high-stress and widely disruptive environment that provided an opportune condition for studying the playful trait’s potential (re)framing effect in the face of adversity among the general population.

      To explore the potential “rose-tinted glasses” vs. “color spotlight” effect, and possible cascade effect in emotional and behavioral responses, we examined a wide range of criterion variables. These include (1) perceptions of various aspects of the COVID environment (e.g., risk of infections, effects of public health preventative measures, anticipated improvement associated with vaccination rollout, access to social support), (2) emotional responses (e.g., sense of isolation/loneliness, perceived vulnerability), and (3) behavioral responses (e.g., precautionary health behavior, coping, various aspects of engagement in leisure and daily activities).

      Materials and methods Participants and procedure

      Data for this study were collected via an online survey distributed in the United States through the crowdsourcing research platform Prolific (2021) during the first two weeks of February 2021. In the U.S., this period was marked by the peak of the second wave of COVID-19 cases and an early stage of vaccination rollout (CRC, 2022). Prolific employs stratified sampling and quota sampling to recruit participants from a pool of pre-screened individuals who have provided detailed demographic information. The platform uses census data to match participants to the general population based on key demographics when drawing a representative sample.

      A total of 503 valid responses were collected from adult participants residing in the U.S. at the time of survey. Of these, 481 participants reported 469 unique ZIP codes across 34 states and the District of Columbia. The study sample was representative of the U.S. adult population in terms of age, sex, and race. Participants ranged in age from 20 to 79 years (M = 46.6 years). Approximately half of the sample was female (50.7%), with 73.7% identifying as White, 13.9% as African American, and 8% as Asian. The majority of participants had completed college (36.5%) or some college (32.9%), and reported household incomes between $30,000 and $70,000 (39.2%). More than one-third of participants (38%) reported having a chronic disease or pre-existing medical/psychiatric illness.

      Instrumentation Playfulness

      We measured playfulness using the established Adult Playfulness Trait Scale (APTS, Shen et al., 2014a). This instrument endorses a latent network trait conception, emphasizing a trait-specific network of internal motivational, cognitive, and dispositional qualities that jointly explain and predict manifested perceptual, emotional, and behavioral expressions of the trait (Shen, 2010, 2020). The APTS contains 19 items measuring three theory-informed and empirically validated dimensions: (1) the fun-seeking motivation (9 items, e.g., “I can find fun in most situations”), (2) uninhibitedness (5 items, e.g., “I do not fear losing anything by being silly”), and (3) spontaneity (5 items, e.g., “I often do things on the spur of the moment”). All items are measured using a 6-point Likert scale (1 = “Disagree strongly” and 6 = “Agree strongly”). The APTS has demonstrated robust reliability and validity in previous studies, with a validated higher-order measurement model supporting the use of summed scores for both the entire scale and its subscales (e.g., Shen et al., 2014a,b). In the current study, the overall scale and subscales displayed acceptable to excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.93, 0.70, 0.66, and 0.73 for the overall scale, fun-seeking motivation, uninhibitedness, and spontaneity, respectively). Mean scores were calculated for the overall scale and each subscale.

      Perceptions of the pandemic environment and social support

      To assess participants’ perceptions of the pandemic environment, we examined three distinct aspects using a 6-point Likert scale (1 = “Disagree strongly” and 6 = “Agree strongly”). First, we measured perceived risk of infection using two separate items: perceived personal risk (COVID Risk-Self) and perceived general likelihood of acquiring COVID-19 (COVID Risk-General). Second, we assessed perceptions of public health preventative measures using five items that measured both positive (e.g., measures helping to lower infection risk and create a safe environment) and negative perceptions (e.g., measures being constraining, anxiety-inducing, and limiting a full life). We calculated summary scores by averaging items within each index (α = 0.82 & 0.73, respectively). Third, we measured participants’ future outlook regarding the evolving COVID situation. This forecasted perception was measured using two items assessing optimism about vaccine development and the eventual return to normalcy. These items were averaged to create a summary score (α = 0.61).

      To capture participants’ perception of immediate social support network, we employed the well-established Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS, Zimet et al., 1990). This 12-item instrument measures perceived social support from three sources: family, friends, and significant others, each assessed by four items using a 7-point scale (1 = “Very strongly disagree”; 7 = “Very strongly agree”). Mean scores were calculated for the overall scale (α = 0.94).

      Emotional responses

      We measured emotional responses to two major challenges imposed by the pandemic: perceived vulnerability stemming from infection risk and feelings of isolation resulting from widely enforced social distancing. Perceived vulnerability was measured using a single item adapted from Gainforth et al. (2012): “thinking about being infected with COVID-19 stresses me out,” rated on a 6-point scale (1 = “Disagree strongly,” 6 = “Agree strongly”). Feelings of isolation was assessed by two items developed for this study (“I felt lonely”; “I felt socially isolated”), rated on a 4-point scale (0 = “Did not apply to me at all”; 4 = “Applied to me most of the time”). Isolation scores were calculated by averaging the two items (α = 0.91).

      Behavioral responses

      We examined four sets of behavioral responses. First, precautionary health behavior was measured by one ad-hoc item (“I take active precautionary measures to lower the risk of infection”) rated on a 6-point scale (1 = “Disagree strongly,” 6 = “Agree strongly”).

      Second, we assessed resilient coping using the 4-item Brief Resilient Coping Scale (BRCS, Sinclair and Wallston, 2004), which includes items such as “I look for creative ways to alter difficult situations” and “I believe I can grow in positive ways by dealing with difficult situations.” Participants responded on a 5-point scale (1 = “Does not describe me at all”; 5 = “Describes me very well”) and mean scores were used for this index (α = 0.91).

      Third, we examined three aspects of leisure engagement: (1) types of valued activity, categorized as home-based offline activities, screen-based digital/online activities, and physical or outdoor activities [following Shen et al.’s (2022) categorization]; (2) frequency of participation in valued leisure activities, physical activity, and outdoor recreation (5-point scale: 1 = “Less than once a week;” 5 = “Almost every day”); and (3) adaptive leisure engagement, measured through efforts to maintain active living (“I do what I can to stay active”) and adaptive outdoor recreation (four items, e.g., “I have explored new outdoor places”; “I changed my schedule in order to fit outdoor activities into my day/week”). All adaptive engagement items used a 6-point scale (1 = “Disagree strongly,” 6 = “Agree strongly”). Mean scores were calculated for the adaptive outdoor recreation index (α = 0.86).

      Lastly, we measured quality of playful engagement in daily activities using a shortened version of the Playful State Scale (PSS, Shen, 2020). Participants rated their frequency of playful engagement over the past month across four dimensions: immersion (e.g., “was deeply absorbed”), sense of mastery (e.g., “good at creating fun”), activeness (e.g., “feeling energetic”), and positive affect (e.g., “experiencing joy”). Each dimension was measured with a 3-item sub-scale on a 5-point scale (1 = “Never,” 5 = “Almost every day”). Mean scores were calculated for each subscale (α = 0.76, 0.89, 0.66, and 0.90, respectively).

      Data analysis strategies Data processing and inspection

      Initial data inspection revealed a small proportion of missing values (0.22%, ranging from 0 to 1.79% per variable) that were missing completely at random (Little’s MCAR test: χ2 = 2185.26, df = 2,220, p = 0.696). We used complete cases for all subsequent analyses. We also verified that most model assumptions (e.g., homogeneity of variance, multicollinearity, linearity) were met with the exception of normality. However, given our large sample size, the planned analyses were deemed relatively robust to this violation.

      Creating groups with contrasting levels of playfulness

      We evaluated four common methods for creating groups using different thresholds based on participants’ overall playfulness scores. (1) Quartile split assigns participants scoring in the bottom quartile (0–25th percentile) to the “low-playfulness” (LP) group and those in the top quartile (75th-100th percentile) to the “high-playfulness” (HP) group, with the middle quartiles dropped from the analysis. (2) Median split assigns participants scoring below/above the median to LP/HP groups. (3) Mean split assigns participants scoring below/above the mean to LP/HP groups. (4) Extreme group analysis assigns participants scoring one standard deviation below/above the median to LP/HP groups, dropping cases in between (DeCoster et al., 2011).

      We selected the quartile threshold method for several reasons. This approach is less sensitive to outliers, ensures approximately equal-sized groups, and does not require normally distributed data. It also generates a clearer division of participants compared to mean or median splits while capturing more data points (50%) than extreme group analysis (approximately 34%; Preacher et al., 2005). By focusing on participants with distinctly contrasting levels of playfulness, this method facilitates the detection of potential differences in perception, emotion, and behavior patterns, yielding clearer and more interpretable results (DeCoster et al., 2011).

      Primary analyses of group differences

      We first conducted a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to examine whether HP and LP groups differed in multivariate means across all 19 continuous dependent variables, evaluating results using Pillai’s Trace. Provided a statistically significant result, separate univariate analyses of variance (one-way ANOVAs) for each dependent variable would be performed to identify specific sources of group differences. Chi-square test of association was performed to examine whether playfulness levels were significantly associated with the categories of valued leisure activities.

      To evaluate potential confounding effects of socio-demographic and health conditions, we performed follow-up analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for each continuous dependent variable that showed significant group differences (referred to as differing dependent variables). The selection of covariate(s) for each ANCOVA model was informed by the association matrix between the overall playfulness index, dependent variables, and potential covariates. The latter included four socio-demographic variables (age, sex, family income, education level) and pre-existing health conditions. We used Pearson correlations for continuous covariates (age, income, education) and t-tests for binary covariates (sex, pre-existing health conditions) to examine the significance and strength of associations. Only covariates significantly correlated with a differing dependent variable were included in the corresponding ANCOVA model. Vaccination status and infection status were initially considered as candidate covariates but dropped from final analyses due to highly uneven group sizes. Place of residence was not considered a likely confounder, as participants were widely distributed across 34 states, minimizing the possibility of geographic clustering of high- or low-playfulness individuals.

      To assess the stability of group comparison results, we performed sensitivity analyses using two alternative group categorization methods: median split and extreme groups. Mean split was initially considered but excluded from sensitivity analyses because it produced groupings very similar to the median split due to the relatively symmetric distribution of playfulness scores.

      All analyses were conducted using R (Version 4.4.0; R Core Team, 2024), with MANOVA, ANOVA, and ANCOVA models analyzed using the stats package (Version 3.6.2; R Core Team, 2024). Results were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05. For continuous dependent variables, we used partial Eta-squared coefficient (η2) to indicate effect size, with values around 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 considered small, medium, and large, respectively (Cohen, 1988). For categorical dependent variables, we used Cramer’s V, with values of 0.1, 0.3, and 0.5 indicating small, medium, and large effects, respectively (Cohen, 1988).

      Results Characteristics of contrasting groups

      Using the quartile method, we created two equal-sized groups (n = 126 each) representing HP and LP participants. Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of playfulness scores for the pooled sample and each group. The HP and LP groups showed distinct, contrasting scores across the overall playfulness index and all three sub-dimensions, with HP participants scoring consistently higher on all measures.

      Pooled and group means and standard deviations of playfulness and subdimension scores.

      Pooled Low-playfulness High-playfulness
      M SD M SD M SD
      Overall playfulness 3.93 0.78 2.93 0.41 4.92 0.35
      Fun-seeking motivation 4.63 0.75 4.00 0.72 5.27 0.50
      Uninhibitedness 3.71 0.97 2.64 0.69 4.66 0.68
      Spontaneity 3.44 1.20 2.14 0.67 4.82 0.69

      n = 503, all index scores have a range of 1–6.

      Unadjusted differences between high- and low-playfulness group

      The one-way MANOVA yielded a significant multivariate result (Pillai’s Trace = 0.35, F [19, 238] = 6.31, p < 0.001), indicating overall differences between the HP and LP groups across the 19 continuous dependent variables. Follow-up unadjusted univariate ANOVA results revealed significant mean differences in several perceptual and behavioral responses (Table 2). In the perceptual domain, compared to LP individuals, HP participants reported a more optimistic future outlook (F [1, 250] = 6.30, p = 0.013, small effect) and stronger perceived social support (F [1, 242] = 20.18, p < 0.001, medium effect).

      ANOVA results of differences in 19 criterion variables between high- and low-playfulness groups.

      Dependent variable Low-playfulness High-playfulness Univariate ANOVAs(df = 1, 250)a
      M SD M SD F p partial η2, 90% CI [LL, UL]
      Environmental perceptions
      Perceived risk of infection – general 4.50 1.26 4.49 1.44 0.00 0.963 0.00 [0.00, 1.00]
      Perceived risk of infection – self 3.22 1.31 3.10 1.43 0.54 0.463 0.00 [0.00, 0.02]
      Positive effects of preventative measures 5.07 1.17 5.10 1.17 0.06 0.809 0.00 [0.00, 0.01]
      Negative effects of preventative measures 3.40 1.28 3.64 1.38 2.13 0.146 0.01 [0.00, 0.04]
      Future outlook 3.86 1.13 4.23 1.20 6.30 0.013 0.02, [0.00, 0.06]
      Social support 4.75 1.62 5.62 1.38 20.18 <0.001 0.08, [0.03, 0.13]
      Emotional responses
      Isolation/loneliness 0.92 0.98 1.07 1.05 1.33 0.250 0.01, [0.00, 0.03]
      Perceived vulnerability 3.92 1.61 3.87 1.78 0.05 0.824 0.00 [0.00, 0.01]
      Behavioral responses
      Precautionary health behavior 5.49 0.94 5.46 0.88 0.06 0.807 0.00 [0.00, 0.01]
      Resilient coping 3.38 0.81 4.09 0.56 64.57 <0.001 0.21, [0.14, 0.27]
      Leisure engagement
      Valued leisure activity frequency 3.72 1.41 3.66 1.46 0.13 0.715 0.00 [0.00, 0.01]
      Physical activity frequency 2.29 1.36 2.68 1.34 5.47 0.020 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Outdoor recreation frequency 2.34 1.37 2.60 1.42 2.08 0.151 0.01 [0.00, 0.04]
      Adaptive active living 3.83 1.51 4.29 1.51 6.03 0.015 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Adaptive outdoor recreation 2.75 1.20 3.46 1.43 18.19 <0.001 0.07 [0.03, 0.12]
      Engagement in daily activities
      Immersion 2.46 0.83 2.81 0.90 10.37 0.001 0.04 [0.01, 0.10]
      Sense of mastery 3.66 0.90 3.85 0.90 2.82 0.094 0.01 [0.00, 0.05]
      Activeness 2.91 0.74 3.34 0.72 22.26 <0.001 0.08 [0.03, 0.15]
      Positive affect 2.81 0.84 3.46 0.93 32.89 <0.001 0.12 [0.05, 0.19]

      M, SD, LL, and UL represent the mean, standard deviation, lower-limit and upper-limit of the partial η2 confidence interval, respectively. Bold texts indicate statistically significant results.

      Exceptions to df: precautionary health behavior (df = 1,249), social support (df = 1, 242), isolation/loneliness (df = 1,249), adaptive outdoor recreation (df = 1,249), valued leisure activity frequency (df = 1,249).

      In the behavioral domain, HP participants reported significantly higher levels of resilient coping (F [1, 250] = 64.57, p < 0.001, large effect) while performing similar levels of precautionary health behavior. No group differences were found in the frequency of general leisure activities and outdoor recreation, though HP participants reported slightly higher levels of physical activity (M = 2.68 vs. M = 2.29 for LP group, p = 0.02, small effect). The Chi-square test revealed no significant association between playfulness levels and valued leisure activity categories (Likelihood Ratio = 3.72, df = 2, p = 0.156), suggesting both groups valued similar categories of leisure activities.

      HP participants did, however, report significantly higher levels of adaptive engagement to maintain active living (p = 0.015, small effect) and outdoor recreation (p < 0.001, large effect). They also reported generally more playful engagement in daily activities, characterized by higher levels of immersion (p = 0.001, small-to-medium effect), activeness (p < 0.001, medium-to-large effect), and positive affect (p < 0.001, approaching large effect).

      Group differences controlling for socio-demographic and health covariates

      Table 3 presents ANCOVA results for the nine dependent variables that showed significant group differences in the preceding ANOVAs. The selection of covariate(s) for each ANCOVA model was based on Pearson correlations and t-tests (see Supplementary Table S1). After controlling for relevant socio-demographic and health status covariates, playfulness remained a significant predictor of eight dependent variables—future outlook, perceived social support, resilient coping, adaptive active living, adaptive outdoor recreation, immersion, activeness, and positive affect—with effect sizes similar to unadjusted results. The only exception was physical activity frequency, where the difference between HP and LP groups was no longer significant (F [1,250] = 3.56, p = 0.61).

      ANCOVA results of differences in nine criterion variables between high- and low-playfulness groups.

      Dependent variable Predictor df F p Partial η2 Partial η2, 90% CI [LL, UL]
      Future outlook Playfulness 1 6.67 0.010 0.03 [0.00, 0.07]
      Education 1 10.15 0.002 0.04 [0.01, 0.09]
      Sex 1 2.95 0.087 0.01 [0.00, 0.05]
      Social support Playfulness 1 18.62 0.000 0.07 [0.03, 0.13]
      Income 1 6.29 0.013 0.03 [0.00, 0.07]
      Resilient coping Playfulness 1 62.05 0.000 0.21 [0.14, 0.28]
      Sex 1 2.14 0.145 0.01 [0.00, 0.04]
      Physical activity frequency Playfulness 1 3.56 0.61 0.02 [0.00, 0.05]
      Education 1 15.63 <0.001 0.06 [0.02, 0.12]
      Adaptive active living Playfulness 1 4.32 0.039 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Sex 1 4.58 0.033 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Preexisting condition 1 3.68 0.056 0.02 [0.00, 0.05]
      Adaptive outdoor recreation Playfulness 1 16.16 <0.001 0.07 [0.02, 0.12]
      Age 1 6.47 0.012 0.03 [0.00, 0.07]
      Income 1 6.83 0.010 0.03 [0.00, 0.07]
      Preexisting condition 1 0.64 0.426 0.00 [0.00, 0.03]
      Immersion Playfulness 1 7.43 0.007 0.03 [0.00, 0.08]
      Age 1 30.66 <0.001 0.12 [0.06, 0.18]
      Preexisting condition 1 14.36 <0.001 0.06 [0.02, 0.11]
      Activeness Playfulness 1 13.19 <0.001 0.05 [0.02, 0.11]
      Sex 1 4.56 0.034 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Preexisting condition 1 4.19 0.042 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Positive affect Playfulness 1 29.99 <0.001 0.12 [0.06, 0.18]
      Preexisting condition 1 2.10 0.149 0.01 [0.00, 0.04]

      LL and UL represent the lower-limit and upper-limit of the partial η2 confidence interval, respectively. Bold texts indicate statistically significant results.

      Supplemental ANCOVA models for the remaining dependent variables revealed no significant differences after adding relevant covariates, confirming the robustness of our initial ANOVA results.

      Results of sensitivity analysis: stability of group differences across categorization methods

      Tables 4, 5 present results of sensitivity analyses comparing three group categorization methods: quartile split, median split, and extreme groups. MANOVA results using all 19 criterion variables showed consistent significant differences between HP and LP participants across all three methods (Table 4), indicating robust overall group differences.

      One-Way MANOVA results by categorization methods.

      Threshold Pillai’s trace Approx. F df p
      Quartiles 0.352 6.31 19, 238 <0.001
      Median 0.178 5.27 19, 462 <0.001
      Median ± 1 SD 0.392 4.52 19, 133 <0.001

      ANOVA results by categorization methods.

      Dependent variable Threshold df df error F p Partial η2 Partial η2, 90% CI [LL, UL]
      Future outlook Quartiles 1 250 6.30 0.013 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Median 1 501 3.54 0.060 0.01 [0.00, 0.02]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 162 3.28 0.072 0.02 [0.00, 0.07]
      Social support Quartiles 1 242 20.18 <0.001 0.08 [0.03, 0.13]
      Median 1 492 16.93 <0.001 0.03 [0.01, 0.06]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 242 20.18 <0.001 0.08 [0.03, 0.13]
      Resilient coping Quartiles 1 250 64.57 <0.001 0.21 [0.14, 0.27]
      Median 1 500 52.13 <0.001 0.09 [0.06, 0.14]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 250 64.57 <0.001 0.21 [0.14, 0.27]
      Physical activity frequency Quartiles 1 250 5.47 0.020 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Median 1 501 6.77 0.010 0.01 [0.00, 0.03]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 162 2.02 0.157 0.01 [0.00, 0.05]
      Adaptive active living Quartiles 1 250 6.03 0.015 0.02 [0.00, 0.06]
      Median 1 501 5.35 0.021 0.01 [0.00, 0.03]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 162 6.69 0.011 0.04 [0.01, 0.10]
      Adaptive outdoor recreation Quartiles 1 249 18.19 <0.001 0.07 [0.03, 0.12]
      Median 1 500 18.45 <0.001 0.04 [0.01, 0.07]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 161 17.05 <0.001 0.10 [0.04, 0.17]
      Immersion Quartiles 1 250 10.37 0.001 0.04 [0.01, 0.09]
      Median 1 501 8.17 0.004 0.02 [0.00, 0.04]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 162 10.07 0.002 0.06 [0.01, 0.12]
      Activeness Quartiles 1 250 22.26 <0.001 0.08 [0.04, 0.14]
      Median 1 501 17.57 <0.001 0.03 [0.01, 0.06]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 162 21.48 <0.001 0.12 [0.05, 0.20]
      Positive affect Quartiles 1 250 32.89 <0.001 0.12 [0.06, 0.18]
      Median 1 501 31.81 <0.001 0.06 [0.03, 0.10]
      Median ± 1 SD 1 162 29.11 <0.001 0.15 [0.08, 0.24]

      LL and UL represent the lower-limit and upper-limit of the partial η2 confidence interval, respectively.

      Univariate ANOVAs for nine variables showing initial group differences revealed consistent results across methods, with two exceptions involving small effects (Table 5). Future outlook differences became borderline significant using median split and extreme groups methods (p = 0.06 and 0.07, respectively), while physical activity differences became non-significant using the extreme groups method (p = 0.157).

      Effect sizes were comparable between quartiles and extreme groups methods, while the median split method produced smaller effects. This pattern is expected, as both quartiles and extreme groups method create sharper contrasts between groups. Overall, these sensitivity analyses demonstrate the robustness of our findings across different categorization approaches.

      Discussion

      This study represents an initial effort to empirically investigate the potential framing or reframing effect of playfulness as a perceptual lens, a cognitive filter, and/or an instigator of emotional and behavioral shifts in perceiving, interpreting, and experiencing environment and events. We compared individuals with higher levels of playfulness (HP) and those with lower levels of playfulness (LP) across 19 criterion variables representing diverse perceptual, emotional, and behavioral responses during a high-stress, widely disruptive period—the COVID-19 pandemic. Our findings remained largely consistent across different group categorization methods, revealing that HP and LP individuals differed significantly in some, but not all, aspects of their responses. Three sets of contrasting findings emerged, providing novel insights into how playful individuals function during times of turmoil and constraints, while informing a more nuanced understanding of playfulness’ role in shaping how environment and life events are experienced and approached.

      Optimistic future outlook despite realistic assessment of current circumstances: cognitive redirecting toward positive possibilities

      Compared to less playful participants, more playful individuals anticipated a more optimistic future outlook regarding situations improving with vaccine rollout and life returning to normal. At first glance, this optimism might seem counterintuitive given that HP and LP individuals shared similar perceptions of COVID infection risks and public health measures. However, closer inspection of these perceptual domains reveals an intriguing pattern: convergence occurred in areas relying on critical thinking and objective assessment (e.g., risk assessment), while divergence emerged in domains with more room for subjective interpretation and creative imagination (e.g., future outlook).

      Specifically, COVID-19 posed a global threat to public health, with its danger and associated risks widely recognized by the public (Wilke et al., 2021) at the time of our survey. After initial uncertainty surrounding virus infection risk and preventative measure effectiveness, ample governmental guidelines, public health messaging, and media coverage had led to normalized risk perceptions and understanding of preventative measures’ effects (Kim et al., 2020). The abundance of science-based data and accessible factual information aided realistic risk assessment—a cognitive analysis task relying heavily on logical and critical thinking—while leaving less room for subjective interpretation. In contrast, envisioning future possibilities relies more on creative thinking and intuitive imagination, as future scenarios are hypothetical, abstract, and often uncertain. Our study reveals that while playful individuals did not differ in their critical assessment of immediate threats and protective factors where abundant information existed, they showed a significantly stronger inclination to focus on positive possibilities when envisioning the future. This suggests that playfulness may not override critical thinking but rather complement it by enabling a more optimistic perspective when interpreting uncertainties. This optimistic future orientation aligns with Shen’s (2010) finding that more playful people tend to view themselves, others, and the world more positively. Our results extend this understanding by identifying that such positive “bias” is most likely to emerge in domains with high uncertainty and ample room for creative thinking and imagination.

      The APTS (Shen et al., 2014a) used to measure playfulness in this study contains subscales that tap into both motivational and cognitive components of the trait, offering valuable insights into the positive “bias” observed in playful individuals’ future-oriented imagination. Our results revealed that HP individuals were characterized by higher levels of fun-seeking motivation and uninhibitedness, two key facets of playfulness that likely contributed to this bias. The heightened fun-seeking motivation might have predisposed HP individuals” to accentuate possibilities for creating fun, enjoyment, and other positive experiences when envisioning future possibilities, a tendency that persisted despite their realistic assessment of current circumstances. This would support the interpretation of a goal-framing effect (Levin et al., 1998) associated with playfulness, wherein situations or environments are interpreted with a focus on their potential to fulfill salient goals—in this case, the desire to seek inherent fun, enjoyment, and/or amusement. This intrinsic goal-oriented framing may act as a cognitive filter, highlighting opportunities for positive experiences while maintaining a realistic appraisal of present circumstances.

      Furthermore, the higher level of uninhibitedness found in playful individuals might have helped expand their imagined possibilities. Uninhibitedness, characterized by the willingness and ability to negotiate constraints and explore alternatives or novel ideas (Bateson and Nettle, 2014; Shen, 2010), likely enabled HP individuals to envision futures that diverged considerably from their “here and now” circumstances. This cognitive boldness and agility allowed hope and optimism to emerge against a backdrop of constraints, disruptions, and other challenges that characterized our COVID-19 study context.

      The interplay between fun-seeking motivation and uninhibitedness in playful individuals may create a synergistic effect. While fun-seeking motivation directs attention toward potential positive outcomes, uninhibitedness enables the needed cognitive freedom to explore and expand on these possibilities, unrestricted by current constraints or conventional thinking. This combination could explain the pronounced positive bias in future-oriented thinking among HP individuals, even in the face of challenging circumstances. These findings not only elucidate the potential mechanisms underlying playful individuals’ optimistic future orientation but also highlight the adaptive potential of playfulness. By maintaining hope and envisioning positive possibilities during stressful and uncertain times, playful individuals may be better equipped to cope with challenges and maintain psychological resilience—a prediction aligned with past studies and substantiated by our second set of findings, elaborated below.

      Resilient coping amidst vulnerability and isolation: adaptive behavioral redirecting as a key to playfulness-driven resilience

      We observed the largest group difference in resilient coping. Although HP and LP participants reported similar levels of vulnerability and isolation, more playful individuals engaged in significantly higher levels of resilient coping—actively altering difficult situations, replacing losses, viewing challenges as opportunity for growth, and exhibiting strong internal control. These behavioral responses jointly conveyed a flexible approach to problem-solving (Polk, 1997) and contributed to positive adaptation when confronted with significant stressors (Sinclair and Wallston, 2004), vividly contextualizing Bolger’s (1990, p. 525) notion “coping is personality in action under stress.”

      Researchers have proposed personality trait substrate of resilience (Rutter, 1987), identifying psychosocial attributes such as intelligence and self-efficacy as protective factors (Polk, 1997). Empirical studies on adaptive coping (e.g., Antoni and Goodkin, 1988; Rabkin et al., 1993) have identified a broader set of personal characteristics associated with resilience, including individual attributes (e.g., wide-ranging interests, optimism, adaptive problem-solving ability), behavioral patterns (e.g., active adaptive coping style), and relational factors (e.g., ability to elicit social support). Many of these characteristics relate directly or indirectly to playfulness, suggesting its potential as a central construct for integrating seemingly unrelated evidence across diverse studies. However, existing coping research has yet to begin exploring the theoretical potential of playfulness-driven resilience.

      A small number of studies have linked playfulness to resilience through mediating factors such as perceived self-efficacy (Clifford et al., 2024), positive affect (Chang et al., 2016), or specific coping strategies (Magnuson and Barnett, 2013). The present study extends previous findings by evidencing a direct link between playfulness and resilience. We argue that playfulness, as a highly permeating trait capable of inducing and influencing cognitions, behaviors, and emotions across life domains (Shen and Masek, 2023), provides a promising integrative concept for explaining diverse resilient factors. For example, the optimism stemming from fun-seeking motivation and cognitive flexibility arising from uninhibitedness among highly playful individuals constitute signature indicators of resilience, directly contributing to a growth mindset in the face of challenges (Sinclair and Wallston, 2004). We also observed a higher level of perceived social support among playful individuals, echoing findings from previous research on leisure stress coping (Qian and Yarnal, 2011). While this social support perception did not alter sense of isolation stemming from pandemic-related restrictions such as physical distancing and social gathering bans, it likely served as a protective factor that mitigated the potentially detrimental effects of isolation and perceived vulnerability on psychological wellbeing. It did so by enhancing coping efficacy—the belief that one had the resources necessary to overcome stressors (Magnuson and Barnett, 2013)—which shaped subsequent coping strategies (Clifford et al., 2024). Rather than resulting from reframing, we believe this heightened perception of social support reflects the actual larger social networks in which playful individuals are often embedded. While not all play activities occur in a social context, play frequently involves interpersonal interactions and cooperation, fostering social bonds that develop and strengthen over time, ultimately contributing to broader and more supportive social networks.

      The intimate links between playfulness and resilience are further elucidated in patterns of leisure engagement, a domain with particular relevance to playfulness given its capacity to afford a time and space that support one of human’s most free and unconstrained expressions—play. After controlling for demographic background, HP and LP participants showed similar frequencies of valued leisure activities, physical activities, and outdoor recreation. However, playful individuals reported significantly higher levels of adaptive engagement—maintaining active living and exploring creative ways (e.g., adjusting schedules, exploring new places) to continue outdoor recreation despite constraints. This concrete example further evidences playfulness’ reframing effect through adaptive behavioral redirection, wherein playful individuals actively shape their experiences through flexible adjustment and creative exploration when encountering constraints and obstacles, charting a resilient course of coping and functioning.

      Experiential quality over quantity and variety: the focus on “how” in playful reframing

      Our comparative results on the quality of engagement in daily activities offer insights into a quintessential feature of the playful behavioral approach—one that is meaningful to the player but less visible to outside observers. Using the PSS (Shen, 2020), a state playfulness measure, we detected that more playful individuals experienced deeply engaged states more frequently than their less playful counterparts, characterized by deep immersion, active mind or body, and positive affect. This finding presents an intriguing contrast to the observed lack of differences in the broad categories and frequencies of valued leisure activities pursued by both groups. It suggests another important form of playful (re)framing: instead of altering what activities are pursued or how frequently they are engaged in, playful individuals shape experiences through elevated experiential quality—the quintessential how that defines the way an activity is invested and experienced.

      Our finding supports Shen and Masek’s (2023) proposition, which emphasizes the experiential quality of play as a clinically decisive change agent in health interventions. While less palpable than the manifested forms and mechanical features of play activities, the internal psychological experience of playful engagement provides a lens that transcends conventionally defined behavioral categories (e.g., work vs. leisure). This perspective allows researchers to capture the functionally critical quality of behavior that often lead more directly to health and well-being changes.

      With assessment tools such as the PSS (Shen, 2020) now available for measuring the quality of playful engagement, we encourage future studies to examine possible differential effects of qualitative and quantitative aspects of experience on various outcomes. This line of research could shed light on whether focusing on the “how” of playful experience offers a more fruitful approach than the current dominant paradigm of exposure studies that emphasize behavioral frequencies and duration.

      Integrative finding: the lemonading core of playful reframing and a refined proposition

      In this study, we addressed the evidence gap concerning the widely accepted assumption about playfulness’ (re)framing effect (Barnett, 2007; Proyer et al., 2018). Our examination of diverse perceptual, emotional, and behavioral experiences among individuals with distinct levels of playfulness yielded rich insights. Integrating three sets of contrasting findings, we suggest that playful individuals do not wear “rose-tinted” glasses that indiscriminately color the surrounding world or ongoing events. Rather, their inner playfulness functions more like a “color spotlight,” illuminating or (re)framing only certain aspects of the environment or experiences. Furthermore, the pattern of our findings suggests that playful (re)framing effect is less prominent in perception formation, and more salient in cognitive and behavioral redirecting.

      Specifically, we detected a “forward-shining” spotlight effect—in times of adversity, playful individuals focused on positive future possibilities while maintaining clear-eyed realism about current circumstances. Meanwhile, they engaged in flexible adaptation, creative exploration, and quality experiences despite challenges. These findings reveal that lemonading lies at the heart of playful (re)framing, wherein playful individuals creatively imagine and pursue positive possibilities to cultivate enjoyment, resilience, and growth, without denying or distorting realistic assessments of threats and challenges. These insights collectively inform a more nuanced understanding of how playful (re)framing works and where it is most likely to exert an impact, leading to a refined proposition:

      Playfulness predisposes one to frame or reframe situations and experiences through cognitive redirecting accentuating positive possibilities and behavioral redirecting emphasizing adaptive and playful engagement to enable or enhance enjoyment and quality experience.

      This proposition differs from previous ones (Barnett, 2007; Proyer et al., 2018) in several important ways: (1) it expands the subject of (re)framing from situations to situations and experiences, reflecting interactionist understanding of the dynamic interplay between the person, playing process, and environment (Shen, 2020; Shen and Masek, 2023). (2) It specifies cognitive and behavioral redirecting as two main forms of playful (re)framing, removing ambiguities embedded in previous propositions. (3) It reflects empirical evidence from this study while aligning with theory-informed conceptualizations of playfulness (e.g., Shen et al., 2014a) by capturing the flexible, adaptive nature of playful engagement and its emphasis on experiential quality.

      Importantly, playful (re)framing represents a functional aspect of playfulness in person-environment interactions. While it enhances our understanding by addressing what playfulness “does,” it does not define what playfulness “is.” The latter can be better captured by definitions that explicitly specify the trait’s constitutional components, as illustrated by the one cited at the beginning of this paper. Therefore, we caution against the popular practice of citing the playful framing effect as a playfulness definition.

      Limitations

      This study, conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, provided an excellent opportunity to examine population-wide responses to adversity. However, the findings may not fully generalize to less challenging periods. Although we controlled for demographic and health covariates to ensure robust estimates, we do not assert that all observed differences between playful and less playful individuals are immediate functions of the playful trait. Multiple mediating paths likely exist, shaped by playfulness while also influencing the observed reframing effects. While extensive, our list of perceptions and experiences is not exhaustive. The areas of reframing identified in this study (e.g., future outlook and behavioral adaptation) provide initial insights into the specific contours of playful reframing.

      Future research directions

      Building on our findings and refined theoretical proposition, future studies should: (1) examine playful (re)framing across different contexts and life domains to validate the “color spotlight” effect and further delimitate its boundary conditions, (2) explore how the “forward-shining” spotlight influences decision-making and problem-solving in uncertain situations, (3) investigate various forms of playful cognitive reframing and behavioral redirecting, and examine the interplay between the two in fostering resilience, (4) investigate a broader set of criterion variables to expand and refine our understating of playful reframing mechanisms and outcomes.

      Additionally, we encourage researchers to model the relationships between playful (re)framing—both cognitive and behavioral—and various aspects of well-being and resilience factors. The observed lemonading effect, supported by existing theories (Fredrickson, 2006) and empirical evidence (e.g., Clifford et al., 2024; Magnuson and Barnett, 2013; Qian and Yarnal, 2011; Shen et al., 2022), suggests that cultivating enjoyment and quality experience through playful (re)framing can build resilience and foster long-term growth and well-being. Elucidating these connections is crucial for harnessing playfulness’ transformative potential in promoting individual flourishing and can inform interventions that enhance adaptability and well-being across life contexts.

      Conclusion

      Understanding how personality shapes perceptions and behaviors can help people leverage their strengths to live more fulfilling lives. This study offers an initial focused scrutiny of playfulness’ widely accepted, presumed (re)framing effect in the context of high-stress, disruptive COVID-19 pandemic. Our findings provide compelling evidence for a selective (re)framing effect of playfulness, revealing its function as a “color spotlight” that accentuates positive future possibilities without biasing perceptions of current situations. This observed optimism, a “forward-shining” effect, was accompanied by patterns of resilient coping and adaptive engagement among highly playful individuals, extending playfulness’ influence to behavioral redirecting.

      These findings inform a refined understanding of playfulness as a trait that predisposes individuals to frame or reframe situations and experiences, primarily through cognitive redirecting that accentuates positive possibilities and behavioral redirecting that emphasizes flexible, adaptive, and playful engagement in pursuit of enjoyment and quality experience. This proposition underscores playfulness’ intimate link with resilience, positioning it as a potential integrative construct that threads diverse resilience factors such as optimism, psychological flexibility, and adaptive coping. The emergent “lemonading” core of playful (re)framing represents a significant theoretical advancement, suggesting that playful individuals excel at creatively envisioning and pursuing opportunities for positive experience and growth amid adversity.

      Our study underscores the importance of cultivating playfulness as a character strength, understanding the when and how of playful (re)framing, and attending to the experiential quality of playful engagement. The latter two hold the key to unlocking playfulness’ transformative potential across life domains. By empirically validating playfulness’ (re)framing effect and illuminating its complex contours, this study lays the groundwork for future research into the mechanisms, boundary conditions, and practical applications of this intriguing phenomenon.

      Data availability statement

      The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving humans were approved by Human Research Protection Program and Institutional Review Board, Oregon State University Research Office. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The ethics committee/institutional review board waived the requirement of written informed consent for participation from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin because the IRB has determined that the protocol meets the minimum criteria for approval under the applicable regulations pertaining to human research protections. The only record linking the subject and the research would be the informed consent form and the principal risk would be potential harm resulting from a breach of confidentiality. The research presents no more than minimal risk of harm to subjects and involves no procedures for which written consent is normally required outside of the research context. Participants were informed that they could print out or take a screenshot of the consent form for their own record. No signatures were collected.

      Author contributions

      XS: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision. ZC: Formal analysis, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

      Funding

      The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The authors wish to thank the Hallie E. Ford Center for Health Children and Families at Oregon State University for providing funding to support the data collection of this study.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Supplementary material

      The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1462980/full#supplementary-material

      References Anderson K. (2010). Framing traits: the role of personality in framing effects. Doctoral dissertation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska at Lincoln. Antoni M. H. Goodkin K. (1988). Host moderator variables in the promotion of cervical neoplasia—I. Personal facets. J. Psychosom. Res. 32, 327338. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(88)90075-X, PMID: 3184021 Barnett L. A. (2007). The nature of playfulness in young adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 43, 949958. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.018 Bateson P. Nettle D. (2014). Playfulness, ideas, and creativity: a survey. Creat. Res. J. 26, 219222. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2014.901091 Bolger N. (1990). Coping as a personality process: a prospective study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59, 525537. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.3.525 Brauer K. Stumpf H. S. C. Proyer R. T. (2024). Playfulness in middle- and older age: testing associations with life satisfaction, character strengths, and flourishing. Aging Mental Health 28, 15401549. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2024.2372471, PMID: 38940664 Chang P. J. Yarnal C. Chick G. (2016). The longitudinal association between playfulness and resilience in older women engaged in the red hat society. J. Leis. Res. 48, 210227. doi: 10.18666/JLR-2016-V48-I3-6256 Clifford C. Paulk E. Lin Q. Cadwallader J. Lubbers K. Frazier L. D. (2024). Relationships among adult playfulness, stress, and coping during the covid-19 pandemic. Curr. Psychol., 110. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-02870-0, PMID: 35221637 Cohen J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Pblishers. CRC (2022). Johns Hopkins coronavirus resource center mortality analyses. Available at: https://coronavirus.jhu.edu/data/mortality Crum A. J. Salovey P. Achor S. (2013). Rethinking stress: the role of mindsets in determining the stress response. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 716733. doi: 10.1037/a0031201, PMID: 23437923 DeCoster J. Gallucci M. Iselin A.-M. R. (2011). Best practices for using median splits, artificial categorization, and their continuous alternatives. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 2, 197209. doi: 10.5127/jep.008310 Erez A. B.-H. Katz N. Waldman-Levi A. (2016). Protective personality variables and their effect on well-being and participation in the elderly: a pilot study. Healthy Aging Research 5, 19. doi: 10.1097/01.HXR.0000508388.87759.42 Felsman P. Gunawardena S. Seifert C. M. (2020). Improv experience promotes divergent thinking, uncertainty tolerance, and affective well-being. Think. Skills Creat. 35:100632. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100632 Fredrickson B. L. (2006). Unpacking positive emotions: investigating the seeds of human flourishing. J. Posit. Psychol. 1, 5759. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510981 Gainforth H. L. Cao W. Latimer-Cheung A. E. (2012). Message framing and parents' intentions to have their children vaccinated against HPV. Public Health Nurs. 29, 542552. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1446.2012.01038.x Gamliel E. Zohar A. H. Kreiner H. (2014). Personality traits moderate attribute framing effects. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 5, 584592. doi: 10.1177/1948550613516874 Grosul M. Feist G. J. (2014). The creative person in science. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 8, 3043. doi: 10.1037/a0034828 Hill P. L. Mroczek D. K. Young R. K. (2014). “Personality traits as potential moderators of well-being: setting a Foundation for Future Research” in Stability of happiness: theories and evidence on whether happiness can change. eds. Sheldon K. M. Lucas R. E. (San Diego, California: Elsevier Academic Press), 245259. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-411478-4.00012-6 Holmes R. Hart T. (2022). Exploring the connection between adult playfulness and emotional intelligence. J. Play Adulthood 4, 2851. doi: 10.5920/jpa.973 Kim S. Cho S. K. LoCascio S. P. (2020). The role of media use and emotions in risk perception and preventive behaviors related to COVID-19 in South Korea. Asian J. Public Opin. Res. 8, 297323. doi: 10.15206/ajpor.2020.8.3.297 Lee A. Y.-P. Wang Y.-H. Yang F.-R. (2021). Feeling exhausted? Let’s play – how play in work relates to experienced burnout and innovation behaviors. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 16, 629648. doi: 10.1007/s11482-019-09794-1 Levin I. P. Gaeth G. J. Schreiber J. Lauriola M. (2002). A new look at framing effects: distribution of effect sizes, individual differences, and independence of types of effects. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 88, 411429. doi: 10.1006/obhd.2001.2983 Levin I. P. Schneider S. L. Gaeth G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 76, 149188. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804, PMID: 9831520 Magnuson C. D. Barnett L. A. (2013). The playful advantage: how playfulness enhances coping with stress. Leis. Sci. 35, 129144. doi: 10.1080/01490400.2013.761905 Martocchio J. J. Webster J. (1992). Effects of feedback and cognitive playfulness on performance in microcomputer software training. Pers. Psychol. 45, 553578. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1992.tb00860.x Neisser U. (1976). Cognition and reality: principles and implications of cognitive psychology. New York City, NY: W H Freeman/Times Books/Henry Holt & Co. Polk L. V. (1997). Toward a middle-range theory of resilience. Adv. Nurs. Sci. 19, 113. doi: 10.1097/00012272-199703000-00002, PMID: 9055026 Preacher K. J. Rucker D. D. MacCallum R. C. Nicewander W. A. (2005). Use of the extreme groups approach: a critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychol. Methods 10, 178192. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.2.178, PMID: 15998176 Prolific (2021). Version February 2021. London, UK, first released 2014. Available at: https://www.prolific.co (Accessed December 2, 2021). Proyer R. T. (2011). Being playful and smart? The relations of adult playfulness with psychometric and self-estimated intelligence and academic performance. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 463467. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.02.003 Proyer R. T. (2017). A new structural model for the study of adult playfulness: assessment and exploration of an understudied individual differences variable. Personal. Individ. Differ. 108, 113122. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.12.011 Proyer R. T. Gander F. Bertenshaw E. J. Brauer K. (2018). The positive relationships of playfulness with indicators of health, activity, and physical fitness. Front. Psychol. 9:1440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01440, PMID: 30154749 Qian X. L. Yarnal C. (2011). The role of playfulness in the leisure stress-coping process among emerging adults: an SEM analysis. Leis/Loisir 35, 191209. doi: 10.1080/14927713.2011.578398 R Core Team (2024). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Rabkin J. G. Remien R. Katoff L. Williams J. B. (1993). Resilience in adversity among long-term survivors of AIDS. Hosp. Community Psychiatry 44, 162167. doi: 10.1176/ps.44.2.162, PMID: 8432502 Rutter M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 57, 316331. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1987.tb03541.x Shen X. (2010). Adult playfulness as a personality trait: its conceptualization, measurement and relationship to psychological well-being. (OCLC No. 859524715) [Doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University] Penn State University Libraries: Electric theses and Dissertations for Graduate School. Shen X. (2020). Constructing an interactionist framework for playfulness research: adding psychological situations and playful states. J. Leis. Res. 51, 536558. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2020.1748551 Shen X. Chick G. Pitas N. (2017). From playful parents to adaptable children: a structural equation model of the relationships between playfulness and adaptability among young adults and their parents. Int. J. Play 6, 244254. doi: 10.1080/21594937.2017.1382983 Shen X. Chick G. Zinn H. (2014a). Playfulness in adulthood as a personality trait: a reconceptualization and a new measurement. J. Leis. Res. 46, 5883. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2014.11950313 Shen X. Chick G. Zinn H. (2014b). Validating the adult playfulness trait scale (APTS): an examination of the personality, behavioral, attitudinal, and perceptual nomological network of playfulness. Am. J. Play 6, 345369. Available at: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1032064 Shen X. MacDonald M. Logan S. W. Parkinson C. Gorrell L. Hatfield B. E. (2022). Leisure engagement during covid-19 and its association with mental health and wellbeing in U. S. Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:1081. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031081, PMID: 35162106 Shen X. Masek L. (2023). The playful mediator, moderator, or outcome? An integrative review of the roles of play and playfulness in adult-centered psychological interventions for mental health. J. Posit. Psychol. 19, 10371050. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2023.2288955 Sinclair V. G. Wallston K. A. (2004). The development and psychometric evaluation of the brief resilient coping scale. Assessment 11, 94101. doi: 10.1177/1073191103258144, PMID: 14994958 Wilke J. Hollander K. Mohr L. Edouard P. Fossati C. González-Gross M. . (2021). Drastic reductions in mental well-being observed globally during the COVID-19 pandemic: results from the ASAP survey. Front. Med. 8:578959. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2021.578959, PMID: 33842492 Yu P. Wu J. J. Chen I. H. Lin Y. T. (2007). Is playfulness a benefit to work? Empirical evidence of professionals in Taiwan. Int. J. Technol. Manag. 39, 412429. doi: 10.1504/IJTM.2007.013503 Yue X. D. Leung C. L. Hiranandani N. A. (2016). Adult playfulness, humor styles, and subjective happiness. Psychol. Rep. 119, 630640. doi: 10.1177/0033294116662842, PMID: 27507855 Zimet G. D. Powell S. S. Farley G. K. Werkman S. Berkoff K. A. (1990). Psychometric characteristics of the multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J. Pers. Assess. 55, 610617. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5503&4_17
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.ksygmy.com.cn
      www.geqxnq.com.cn
      www.ekqzsp.com.cn
      www.gzcwuk.com.cn
      kdamen.com.cn
      qbjjyyun.net.cn
      uhfjwz.com.cn
      www.viwing.net.cn
      x-nv.com.cn
      wsxti.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p