Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1193915 Psychology Original Research Comparison of behaviorally inhibited and typically developing children’s play behaviors in the preschool classroom Druskin Lindsay R. 1 Novick Danielle R. 2 3 Smith Kelly A. 4 Chronis-Tuscano Andrea 2 Wagner Nicholas J. 5 Pham Stephanie 4 Fleece Hailey M. 6 Danko Christina M. 2 Rubin Kenneth H. 4 * 1Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, United States 2Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States 3Yale Child Study Center, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States 4Department of Human Department & Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States 5Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States 6Department of Counseling, Higher Education, and Special Education, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, United States

Edited by: Junsheng Liu, East China Normal University, China

Reviewed by: Jessica Grady, The University of the Pacific, United States; Nejra van Zalk, Imperial College London, United Kingdom

*Correspondence: Kenneth H. Rubin, krubin@umd.edu
12 07 2023 2023 14 1193915 27 03 2023 22 06 2023 Copyright © 2023 Druskin, Novick, Smith, Chronis-Tuscano, Wagner, Pham, Fleece, Danko and Rubin. 2023 Druskin, Novick, Smith, Chronis-Tuscano, Wagner, Pham, Fleece, Danko and Rubin

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Introduction

Behavioral inhibition (BI) is a temperamental trait characterized by a bias to respond with patterns of fearful or anxious behavior when faced with unfamiliar situations, objects, or people. It has been suggested that children who are inhibited may experience early peer difficulties. However, researchers have yet to systematically compare BI versus typically developing children’s observed asocial and social behavior in familiar, naturalistic settings.

Method

We compared the in-school behaviors of 130 (M = 54 months, 52% female) highly inhibited preschoolers (identified using the parent-reported Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire) to 145 (M = 53 months, 52% female) typically developing preschoolers. Both samples were observed on at least two different days for approximately 60 min. Observers used the Play Observation Scale to code children’s behavior in 10-s blocks during free play. Teachers completed two measures of children’s behavior in the classroom.

Results

Regression models with robust standard errors controlling for child sex, age, and weekly hours in school revealed that preschoolers identified as BI engaged in significantly more observed reticent and solitary behavior, and less social play and teacher interaction than the typically developing sample. Children with BI also initiated social interaction with their peers and teachers less often than their counterparts who were not inhibited. Teachers reported that children identified as BI were more asocial and less prosocial than their non-BI counterparts.

Discussion

Significantly, the findings indicated that inhibited children displayed more solitude in the context of familiar peers. Previous observational studies have indicated behavioral differences between BI and unfamiliar typical age-mates in novel laboratory settings. Children identified as BI did not receive fewer bids for social interaction than their typically developing peers, thereby suggesting that children who are inhibited have difficulty capitalizing on opportunities to engage in social interaction with familiar peers. These findings highlight the need for early intervention for children with BI to promote social engagement, given that the frequent expression of solitude in preschool has predicted such negative outcomes as peer rejection, negative self-regard, and anxiety during the elementary and middle school years.

behavioral inhibition solitude peer interactions temperament anxiety preschoolers R01MH103253–01 NIH section-at-acceptance Developmental Psychology

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Behavioral inhibition (BI) is a temperamental style characterized by a bias to respond with vigilant patterns of fearful or anxious responses when exposed to unfamiliar situations, objects, or people (Kagan et al., 1988; Fox et al., 2005). Behavioral inhibition can be reliably measured as early as infancy, and it is estimated that 15–20% of children present with BI (Degnan and Fox, 2007).

      Researchers have reported that BI is manifested by a variety of behaviors at different ages. Significantly, researchers have demonstrated that there exists continuity from laboratory assessments of BI in infancy to observed fearfulness in the face of unfamiliar objects, adults, and toddlers at two-years, and subsequently to specific forms of social withdrawal expressed in the company of unfamiliar peers during the preschool and early elementary school years (e.g., Fox et al., 2001; Rubin et al., 2002; Henderson et al., 2004; Degnan et al., 2008; Kiel and Buss, 2011; Buss et al., 2013; Brooker et al., 2016). Toddlers who have been identified as behaviorally inhibited display fewer smiling, speaking, and approach behaviors than their age-mates who are not inhibited when in the presence of unfamiliar objects and adults (Garcia Coll et al., 1984). During the preschool years, children who are behaviorally inhibited require longer “warm up” periods before approaching or initiating play with unfamiliar children and adults (Kagan et al., 1987). When observed during free play in a laboratory setting with unfamiliar age-mates who are not behaviorally inhibited, inhibited preschoolers have been found to display more reticent behavior (watching peers from afar; being unoccupied) and to engage in more solitary activity (Rubin et al., 2002; Henderson et al., 2004) than their non-inhibited age-mates.

      As noted above, the extant evidence base has focused largely on BI in the context of novel situations and unfamiliar peers (see Rubin et al., 2009, 2018 for reviews). Researchers have consistently demonstrated links between BI, as assessed during infancy and toddlerhood, and observed displays of social reticence in groups of unfamiliar peers in preschool-, kindergarten- and early elementary-aged children (e.g., Fox et al., 2001; Rubin et al., 2002; Henderson et al., 2004; Degnan et al., 2008). Furthermore, in laboratory settings, elevated BI in early childhood has been shown to predict less observed interpersonal behavior (e.g., the display of fewer positive social initiations/reactions, less time spent in social play) and poorer social skills (e.g., incompetent social problem-solving skills) during the elementary school years (Nelson et al., 2005; Walker et al., 2014; Penela et al., 2015). These findings underscore how the socially avoidant tendencies of inhibited children may impede proficiency in the age-appropriate social skills derived from engagement with peers.

      The developmental cascade from BI to social withdrawal has been captured in a conceptual model that has guided much of the current research on the predictors, concomitants, and consequences of BI in infants, preschoolers, and school-age youth (Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano, 2021; see Rubin et al., 1991 for a review). Briefly, within this conceptual model, BI, as assessed in the infant and toddler years, serves as an early predictor of anxiogenic parental behaviors (e.g., oversolicitousness and overprotectiveness). For example, in the case of oversolicitous parenting, parents may interrupt social situations where children would have the opportunity to experience challenges because they may believe that children are unable to navigate social difficulties without parental support (Rubin et al., 1997). Kiel et al. (2015) also identified curvilinear associations between parental encouragement and children’s separation anxiety, such that overly protective maternal behavior or overly encouraging behavior (i.e., to the point of intrusiveness) was related to greater separation anxiety in inhibited children. Moreover, in the aforementioned model it is proposed that the resulting lack of sufficient opportunities to engage in novel social experiences places children who are inhibited on a trajectory leading to broad impairment in both unfamiliar settings as well as in situations that are experienced on a daily basis. For example, BI has been posited to predict displays of social withdrawal (solitude) in the preschool setting. In turn, the model suggests, and research has supported the notion that social withdrawal among familiar peers predicts deficits in perspective–taking and interpersonal problem-solving skills (e.g., Rubin and Krasnor, 1986; Stewart and Rubin, 1995). These latter deficits, as evidenced in the elementary and middle school years, have been posited, in the conceptual model, and supported in extant research, to predict peer rejection, the consequent development of negative self-appraisals of one’s social competence and relationships, and ultimately, rejection sensitivity and social anxiety during early adolescence (Rubin et al., 1991; Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano, 2021). Indeed, this latter outcome has been supported by research demonstrating that young children who are characterized as being highly inhibited are at increased risk for the later development of social anxiety disorder, which in and of itself is associated with a host of functional impairments throughout adolescence and adulthood (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2009; Muris et al., 2011; Clauss and Blackford, 2012).

      Traditionally, researchers have observed children and their parents in the laboratory setting to identify children high in BI. Furthermore, as noted above, behavioral continuities of BI have been assessed, almost exclusively, within contexts comprising unfamiliar peers (see Rubin et al., 2018 for a relevant review). For example, Kagan (1989) developed a paradigm in which caregivers and their children are placed in an unfamiliar room to engage in unstructured play. While the dyad is playing, an unfamiliar adult enters the room to allow researchers the opportunity to observe children’s reactions, including their hesitancy to interact with the novel adult and toys, frequency of social approach behaviors, and proximity to and interactions with their caregiver (Kagan, 1989; Stifter et al., 1989). In studies of preschool, kindergarten, and elementary school-age children, the consequences of toddler BI have often been assessed by observing children in quartets of unfamiliar peers (e.g., Henderson et al., 2004; Degnan et al., 2008). When laboratory observations are not used, researchers employ a variety of parent- and teacher-report measures to capture BI and conceptually similar constructs in young children (e.g., shyness, social withdrawal, social anxiety), such as the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire (Bishop et al., 2003), the Preschool Anxiety Scale (Spence et al., 2001), and the Colorado Child Temperament Inventory (Rowe and Plomin, 1977). However, researchers have noted discrepancies between parent and teacher ratings of BI children, such that both parent and teacher ratings only moderately converge with observational ratings of children’s behavior in the laboratory (Ballespí et al., 2012), thus highlighting the importance of multi-informant and multi-modal measurement. Moreover, given the pivotal role that positive and negative peer interactions play in the aforementioned developmental cascade model toward child social outcomes (Rubin et al., 2009; Rubin and Chronis-Tuscano, 2021), it is imperative to not only observe inhibited children’s reactions in the face of novelty, but also within the peer/social contexts in which their social difficulties may actually ensue.

      In one of the few studies to examine associations between laboratory assessments of BI and school-based assessments of social withdrawal, Tarullo et al. (2011) found that compared to preschool-age children high in exuberance (e.g., high activity levels, stimulation-seeking, and risk-seeking), laboratory-identified inhibited preschoolers were observed to experience fewer positive peer interactions, engage in more watching/wandering behavior, interact more with the teacher, and display less positive affect and more anxious/vigilant and sad affect within the familiar setting of the classroom (Tarullo et al., 2011). Although this seminal study highlighted inhibited preschoolers’ unique social experiences within the familiar peer setting, the authors noted that only a relatively small number of children were actually identified as temperamentally inhibited. This disclosure may limit the generalizability and conclusions that can be drawn from the findings. Moreover, the children who were inhibited were compared to a sample of children with highly exuberant temperaments. This latter group may well display markedly different social behaviors/interactions than an unselected sample of children. As such, a replication and extension of these findings is warranted in which a larger sample of inhibited children is compared with an unselected sample of same-age peers within a naturalistic setting.

      Also warranted is an examination of the extent to which one of the most frequently used measures of BI, the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire (BIQ; Bishop et al., 2003), can distinguish between those preschoolers who are identified as highly inhibited from those who represent a “typical” sample vis-a-vis their observed behavior in a school setting populated by familiar peers. Thus, we sought to compare observed and teacher-reported behaviors of children who are behaviorally inhibited with those who are typically developing in their preschool classrooms. We did so by recruiting a reasonably large sample of preschoolers who had been identified, by parent assessments on the BIQ, as highly inhibited and compared their classroom behaviors with an unselected sample of same-age children. Based on prior findings (e.g., Tarullo et al., 2011), we hypothesized that the children identified as highly inhibited would display more solitude as well as less group play compared to their typically developing peers. As previous studies have highlighted low activity levels in children identified as shy and anxious (Poole and Schmidt, 2018), we expected that children identified as inhibited would display less rough-and-tumble play (i.e., playful physical contact, roughhousing, or pretend fighting with peers; Rubin, 1982) compared to their typically developing classmates. Furthermore, we hypothesized that children identified as inhibited would make fewer social initiations to, and receive fewer social initiations from, their classmates and teachers as early inhibition has been shown to predict less social competence and prosociality with peers (Bohlin et al., 2005). We also assessed teacher-reports of child behavior. We hypothesized that preschool teachers would assess the highly inhibited preschoolers as being more solitary, anxious, and excluded by their peers and less prosocial and aggressive relative to their typically developing age-mates.

      Materials and methods Procedure

      Two samples of children were recruited for the study. The first sample comprised a group of children identified as highly behaviorally inhibited (see Sample 1 description below); the second sample comprised a group of children who were matched in age to Sample 1 (see Sample 2 description below). All children fell between the ages of 45–64 months (n = 275). They were recruited through community organizations (e.g., schools, daycare centers, pediatrician offices) in the surrounding Washington, DC metropolitan area. Exclusionary criteria included current engagement in anxiety-focused treatment, a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder or a score at or below the clinical cutoff on the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Eaves et al., 2006), or a diagnosis (or suspected diagnosis) of selective mutism. Additionally, current enrollment in a preschool/daycare program was required for study participation.

      A telephone screen to assess eligibility was completed with parents who expressed interest in participating in the study. A primary parent was identified to complete demographic information and assessments online via Qualtrics software. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents. After obtaining informed consent from families, school administrators and teachers were contacted for permission to complete school-based observations of children enrolled in the study. Teachers of participating children also completed questionnaires to assess children’s behavior in the school setting. Trained study personnel conducted 30-min observations of each child during free play in the school setting on each of two separate days. Study materials and procedures were approved by the research team’s university Institutional Review Board. Parents and teachers were compensated for the completion of questionnaires. Data were collected between 2015 to 2020.

      Participants Sample 1 – behaviorally inhibited sample

      One hundred thirty children (n = 130; M = 54 months, SD = 5.73) comprised Sample 1. The sample included 68 girls (52.3%) and 62 boys (47.7%). Children from this sample were recruited as part of a larger randomized controlled trial examining early intervention programs for children high in BI (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2022; ClinicalTrials.gov registration: NCT02308826). The current study uses baseline data from this preregistered intervention study. For inclusion in this sample, children had to score in the 85th percentile or above on the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire (Bishop et al., 2003).

      Sample 2 – typically developing sample

      One hundred forty-five children (n = 145; M = 53 months, SD = 5.33) were included in Sample 2. The sample comprised 76 girls (52.4%) and 69 boys (47.6%). Children in this sample were recruited for the purpose of comparing the BI sample to typically developing children unselected for BI.

      Measures Demographic variables

      Prior to completing the school observations, parents provided demographic information (e.g., sex, age, race, and ethnicity) for themselves and their child (Table 1). Parents also indicated the total time that their child spent in school each week to control for variations in types of school schedules (e.g., full-day vs. extended-day programs).

      Primary parent and child demographic characteristics.

      Variable Sample 1 (n = 130) Sample 2 (n = 145) Combined sample (N = 275)
      Primary parent
      Parent age in years, M (SD) 38.87 (5.35) 37.79 (5.33) 38.29 (5.37)
      Parent relationship to child (% mother) 87.6 89.0 88.0
      Parent marital status (%)
      Married 89.9 93.1 90.9
      Divorced/Separated 4.7 2.8 3.6
      Other 5.4 4.2 5.4
      Parent race (%)
      Asian 17.7 7.6 12.4
      Black or African American 13.1 4.8 8.7
      White 66.2 78.6 72.7
      Other 3.1 8.4 5.8
      Hispanic or Latino (%) 6.9 4.9 5.8
      Annual household income (USD, %)
      $0–$24,999 3.2 0.0 1.8
      $25,000–$49,999 3.2 0.0 1.8
      $50,000–$74,999 4.0 7.1 5.4
      $75,000–$99,999 7.1 6.1 6.7
      $100,000–$124,999 17.5 11.2 14.7
      $125,000–$149,999 8.7 18.4 13.0
      $150,000+ 56.3 57.1 56.7
      Child
      Child age in months, M (SD) 53.88 (5.73) 52.96 (5.33) 53.37 (5.53)
      Child sex (% female) 52.3 52.4 52.4
      Child race
      Asian 13.1 4.9 8.8
      Black or African American 11.5 5.6 8.4
      White 50.8 72.2 62.0
      Other 24.6 17.4 20.8
      Child time in school, M (SD)* 29.16 (13.44) 26.81 (13.89) 27.97 (13.71)
      Time of school observation (% fall) 41.5 37.2 39.3

      *Refers to the number of hours child spent in school per week. Sample 1 = Behaviorally inhibited sample; Sample 2 = Typically developing sample.

      Behavioral inhibition questionnaire

      The Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire (BIQ; Bishop et al., 2003) was used as the primary measure of parent-reported child BI. The BIQ is a psychometrically sound (Kim et al., 2011) 30-item caregiver-rating scale that assesses children’s responses to novel situations across six domains: adults, peers, performance demands, novel settings, physical challenges, and parental separation. Parents rated child behavior on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 1 (Hardly ever) to 7 (Almost always). The BIQ provides a score indicative of social inhibition (comprising the adults, peers, and performance demands subscales) and a score that captures BI in novel and unfamiliar situations (comprising the novel settings, physical challenges, and parental separation subscales). A total BI score can be calculated by summing all items. Scores that fall above 132 (i.e., the top 15% of scores) are within the highly inhibited range based on conceptual models of BI (Kagan et al., 2007). Higher scores reflect more concerns related to BI. Within the current study, reports of internal consistency were high (overall sample α = 0.96, Sample 1 α =0.87, Sample 2 α = 0.95).

      Play observation scale

      A short form of the Play Observation Scale (POS; Rubin, 1982) was used to assess children’s social and non-social behaviors in the classroom setting. Observations began in mid-October so that children had the opportunity to acclimate to the school setting. The POS includes two elements to capture the behaviors of the child being observed (the target child): Time-sampled codes (i.e., mutually exclusive behaviors that are captured within 10-s intervals) and event-sampled codes (i.e., non-mutually exclusive behaviors that were coded each time they occurred). Time-sampled codes included five categories of child’s behavior: reticence (unoccupied and observing/onlooking others from afar); solitary behavior (playing at least three feet away from other children); parallel play (independent play within three feet of other children); group activity (engaging in the same activity as peers or conversing with at least one other child); and teacher interaction (conversing or engaging with a teacher or other adult in the classroom). Also coded were five event-sampled behaviors including anxious behavior (e.g., crying, whining, nail biting, automanipulative activity), positive affect (e.g., laughing), social initiations made to peers, social initiations received from peers, social initiations made to teachers, and social initiations received from teachers. To account for minor differences in the time spent observing each child, a proportion was created by dividing the number of 10-s time samples that behavior was coded by the total number of 10-s time samples that each child was observed. Senior research personnel trained staff to reliably collect the in-school POS data. Observers were required to reach high interrater reliability consisting of κ greater than 0.80 with senior research personnel on training videos. Following the completion of training using pre-recorded videos of children’s play in the laboratory, research personnel were required to reach a κ equal to or greater than 0.80 during a live observation session at a local childcare facility affiliated with the university. To control for time of year, observations were dichotomously coded based on the timing during the academic year to either be a “fall” observation (i.e., occurring during the months October through December) or a “spring” observation (i.e., occurring during the months of February through June).

      Child behavior scale

      The Child Behavior Scale (CBS; Ladd and Profilet, 1996) is a teacher questionnaire used to assess social interaction in the school context. Teachers rated children’s behavior on 35 items using a 3-point scale (1: Does not apply, 2: Applies sometimes, and 3: Certainly applies). The CBS comprises six subscales that capture peer aggression, prosocial behavior with peers (e.g., helps other children, empathetic, cooperative with peers, shows concern for moral issues), asocial behavior in the company of peers (e.g., prefers to play alone, keeps peers at a distance, withdraws from peer activities), exclusion by peers (e.g., not much liked by children, ignored by peers, not chosen as a playmate by peers, ridiculed by peers), anxious-fearful behavior, and hyperactive-distractible behavior. Within the current study, reports of internal consistency were acceptable (Cronbach’s α for subscales from the overall sample ranged from α = 0.77–0.90, Sample 1 ranged from α = 0.77–0.92, Sample 2 ranged from α = 0.77–0.87).

      Preschool play behavior scale

      The Preschool Play Behavior Scale (PPBS; Coplan and Rubin, 2001) is an 18-item teacher-report measure designed to capture the reticent behavior (e.g., takes role of onlooker/spectator; wanders around aimlessly; watches/listens to other children without trying to join in; remains alone/unoccupied or staring into space), solitary-passive behavior (e.g., plays alone examining an object/toy; plays alone building things or with other toys; plays alone drawing/painting or doing puzzles; plays alone exploring toys/objects trying to figure out how they work), solitary-active behavior (e.g., engages in pretend play by self; plays make-believe, but not with other children), social play (e.g., talks to other children during play; engages in groups with other children (not just beside them); engages in active conversations with other children), and rough-play (e.g., rough-and-tumble play; engages in playful fighting with other children) of preschool-aged children in the classroom setting. Teachers were instructed to rate child behavior during free play periods from a scale of 1 (Never) to 5 (Very often). Within the current study, reports of internal consistency were acceptable (Cronbach’s α for subscales from the overall sample ranged from 0.78–0.91, Sample 1 ranged from 0.68–0.95, Sample 2 ranged from 0.64–0.94).

      Data analytic plan

      Hypotheses were tested using structural equation modeling (SEM) in the lavaan package in R (Rosseel, 2012; R Core Team, 2013). A series of regression analyses were run to examine differences in child behavior between the behaviorally inhibited children and the typically developing group for each of the outcome variables. The direct effect of condition on teacher- and parent-reported measures as well as observed behaviors was examined. Robust full-information maximum likelihood was used to handle missing data (Enders, 2001).Theoretically relevant demographic factors [e.g., child sex, child age, total time spent in school, and time of year of the school observations (i.e., fall or spring)] were included as covariates in all analyses.

      Results Descriptive statistics

      Preliminary analyses were conducted using SPSS version 26. Descriptive statistics by sample for all variables of interest are found in Tables 1, 2. Patterns of missingness were examined prior to running analyses. Missingness ranged from 1–19% for teacher-reported variables, and there were no missing data for observed variables.

      Descriptive statistics of variables of interest.

      Variable Sample 1, M (SD) (n = 130) Sample 2, M (SD) (n = 145)
      BIQ Total 153.36 (19.95) 98.13 (28.62)
      BIQ Social Inhibition Composite 5.61 (0.80) 3.53 (1.11)
      BIQ Adults Subscale 5.65 (1.35) 3.60 (1.48)
      BIQ Peers Subscale 5.71 (0.95) 3.63 (1.23)
      BIQ Performance Subscale 5.43 (1.14) 3.28 (1.24)
      BIQ Novelty Composite 5.12 (0.84) 3.32 (1.09)
      BIQ Novel Subscale 4.69 (0.83) 3.08 (0.98)
      BIQ Physical Challenges Subscale 3.36 (1.43) 2.51 (1.21)
      XBIQ Separation Subscale 5.14 (1.45) 3.18 (1.45)
      CBS Peer Aggression Subscale 1.11 (0.27) 1.20 (0.27)
      CBS Prosocial Behavior Subscale 2.18 (0.51) 2.35 (0.42)
      CBS Asocial Behavior Subscale 1.63 (0.60) 1.42 (0.41)
      CBS Exclusion Subscale 1.20 (0.34) 1.19 (0.29)
      CBS Anxious-Fearful Subscale 1.43 (0.47) 1.42 (0.47)
      CBS Hyperactive-Distractible Subscale 1.25 (0.44) 1.49 (0.56)
      POS Time-Sampled Behavior
      Reticence 0.22 (0.16) 0.14 (0.10)
      Solitary Behavior 0.23 (0.15) 0.19 (0.13)
      Parallel Play 0.22 (0.16) 0.13 (0.10)
      Group Activity 0.31 (0.21) 0.42 (0.21)
      Teacher Interaction 0.08 (0.08) 0.11 (0.12)
      POS Event-Sampled Behavior
      Anxious behavior 0.01 (0.03) 0.01 (0.04)
      Positive affect 0.04 (0.04) 0.03 (0.03)
      Social initiations to peers 0.04 (0.03) 0.04 (0.02)
      Social initiations from peers 0.01 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02)
      Social initiations to teachers 0.03 (0.03) 0.03 (0.02)
      Social initiations from teachers 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.01)
      PPBS Reticent Behavior Subscale 2.45 (0.89) 2.19 (0.67)
      PPBS Solitary-Passive Behavior Subscale 3.08 (0.78) 2.90 (0.61)
      PPBS Solitary-Active Behavior Subscale 2.58 (0.97) 2.55 (0.69)
      PPBS Social Play Subscale 3.46 (1.05) 4.07 (0.79)
      PPBS Rough Play Subscale 1.90 (1.00) 2.13 (1.04)

      BIQ, Behavioral inhibition questionnaire; CBS, Child behavior scale; POS, Play observation scale; PPBS, Preschool play behavior scale. Sample 1 = Behaviorally inhibited sample; Sample 2 = Typically developing sample.

      Observational data Reticence

      In the model examining differences in observed child reticence, it was found that the sample of parent-reported BI children exhibited significantly more reticent behavior in their classrooms than children in the typically developing sample (b = 0.081, z = 4.923, p < 0.001). Predictors explained 9.6% of the variance. Child time in school was a significant covariate (b = −0.001, z = −2.147, p < 0.001), suggesting more time in school was related to less reticent behavior for both groups.

      Solitary play

      In the model examining differences in solitary play between the BI and typical samples, children who were inhibited exhibited significantly more solitary play in their classrooms than typically developing children (b = 0.050, z = 3.028, p = 0.002). Predictors explained 6.5% of the variance. Child age was a significant covariate (b = −0.003, z = −2.067, p = 0.039); older children engaged in less solitary play across both groups.

      Parallel play

      There were no significant group differences found in observed child parallel play (b = 0.019, z = 1.455, p = 0.146). Predictors explained 5.1% of the variance. Child sex was a significant covariate (b = − 0.031, z = − 2.452, p = 0.014), with boys engaging in significantly less parallel play than girls across groups. Total time spent in school (b = − 0.001, z = 2.948, p = 0.009) was also a significant covariate, with those spending more time in school displaying less parallel play across groups.

      Group activity

      It was found that children who were behaviorally inhibited exhibited significantly less group play than children who were not inhibited (b = −0.123, z = −5.214, p < 0.001). Predictors explained 13.4% of the variance. Child age (b = 0.008, z = 3.572, p < 0.001) and total time spent in school (b = 0.002, z = 2.948, p = 0.003) were significant covariates, with older children and those spending more time in school engaging in more group play across groups.

      Teacher interaction

      Children who were highly inhibited exhibited significantly less teacher interaction than typical children (b = −0.028, z = −2.360, p = 0.018). Predictors explained 4.7% of the variance. Child age was a significant covariate (b = −0.003, z = −1.987, p = 0.047); younger children from both groups engaged in more interaction with their teachers.

      Event-sampled behaviors

      Unexpectedly, in the models examining group differences in event-sampled behaviors, sample was not a significant predictor of observed anxious behavior (p = 0.288), positive affect (p = 0.642), social initiations made to peers (p = 0.349), social initiations received from peers (p = 0.126), or social initiations received from teachers (p = 0.177).

      However, it was found that children who were inhibited exhibited significantly fewer social initiations to teachers than typical children (b = −0.029, z = −2.151, p = 0.031). The predictors explained 4% of the variance in social initiations made to teachers.

      Teacher-report comparisons Preschool play behavior scale

      On the PPBS, teachers rated children who were inhibited as engaging in more reticent behavior (b = 0.290, SE = 0.098, B = 0.183, p = 0.003) and more solitary activity that involved constructive activity with objects (e.g., puzzle construction; artwork; b = 0.183, SE = 0.086, B = 0.132, p = 0.033), than typical children. There were non-significant group differences in teacher ratings of child solitary active play (e.g., running aimlessly around the playroom; b = 0.045, SE = 0.108, B = 0.027, p = 0.679). Further, teachers rated typical children as engaging in more rough-and-tumble play (b = −0.262, SE = 0.116, B = −0.127, p = 0.024) and more social play involving cooperation between and conversations among peers (b = −0.639, SE = 0.115, B = −0.329, p < 0.001), compared to children who were highly inhibited.

      Child behavior scale

      On the CBS, there were significant differences between groups on several domains. Teachers rated typical children as being more aggressive (b = −0.097, SE = 0.034, B = −0.178, p = 0.004) and more prosocial (b = −0.172, SE = 0.061, B = −0.179, p = 0.005) than children who were inhibited. Additionally, teachers rated children who were inhibited as being more asocial than typical children (b = 0.185, SE = 0.063, B = 0.183, p = 0.003). However, there were no significant group differences in teacher ratings of child anxiety (b = 0.010, SE = 0.062, B = 0.011, p = 0.870) or teacher ratings of child exclusion (b = 0.019, SE = 0.040, B = 0.030, p = 0.640).

      Discussion

      Although researchers have long been reporting behavioral differences between extremely inhibited and typical children when these groups are observed in an unfamiliar setting (e.g., the laboratory) and in the company of unfamiliar peers (see Rubin et al., 2018 for a review), few studies have extended this research to the naturalistic and familiar context of the school. Moving beyond observations in laboratory settings to understand how inhibited children function in familiar contexts is essential if one is to establish support for the conceptual conjecture that BI in early childhood is linked to the display of solitude in familiar settings that, in turn, predicts subsequent difficulties in the peer group (rejection; victimization), negative thoughts and feelings about the self, and ultimately to anxiety (and more specifically, social anxiety; Rubin et al., 2009). Although the ability to regulate emotions and behavior in novel situations and contexts is crucial to optimal child development, continued adaptability in everyday social contexts is highly significant to overall child functioning. Thus, the primary aim of the current study was to examine links between a reliable and valid index of parent-reported BI (Bishop et al., 2003; Broeren and Muris, 2010; Kim et al., 2011) and observed and teacher-reported child behaviors in the preschool setting with familiar peers.

      In accordance with our hypotheses and previous research, our findings revealed that children identified as highly inhibited on the parent-report BIQ were observed to engage in more reticent behavior and solitary play and in less social interaction with peers and teachers than a comparison group of typical (non-BI) age-mates. Indeed, both observational data and teacher-reports indicated that inhibited children evidenced significantly more reticence and solitude (i.e., unoccupied, observing/onlooking others from afar; solitary play) in the school setting compared to their same-aged typical peers. These findings are in accord with those reported by Tarullo et al. (2011) in a study of a much smaller sample of extremely inhibited children which was compared with a group of highly exuberant preschoolers. When conceptualized within the broader literature pertaining to laboratory-based observations of inhibited children’s behaviors, our findings indicated not only that inhibited preschoolers demonstrated significantly more unoccupied/onlooker behaviors (i.e., reticence; Coplan et al., 1994), but also more solitary activity compared to their typically developing peers.

      It is notable that children in the inhibited sample did not engage in less parallel play (i.e., independent play within three feet of other children) compared to their typically developing counterparts. Researchers have suggested that parallel play may be a necessary step that allows inhibited children to progress from watching others from afar or choosing to express solitude to eventually approaching others in an effort to engage in social interaction (Bakeman and Brownlee, 1980; Asendorpf, 1991). Perhaps a different observational taxonomy and the use of such statistical methods as sequential analyses may allow researchers to examine whether those inhibited children who gradually come to engage others in social interaction do, indeed, display a sequential process of observing others from afar, to approaching others and quiescently marking territory in close proximity to specific peers, to requesting that they join the activities of the desired peers.

      In general, the teacher reports supported that which was observed. Thus, teachers indicated that children identified as behaviorally inhibited evidenced significantly more reticence and solitary passive (e.g., quiescent object exploration and construction) activity. No group differences emerged with regard to teacher-reported solitary active play. Given that the latter form of solitude is rather infrequently displayed during preschool free play (e.g., Rubin, 1982) the non-significant between-group difference is unsurprising.

      Contrary to our hypotheses, children in the behaviorally inhibited sample did not make or receive fewer bids for social interactions than their typically developing peers. Despite these non-significant differences, it may have been that the preschoolers identified as inhibited were approaching their age-mates in a less than competent manner, thereby negating the possibility of engaging with peers in cooperative, group-oriented play. Unfortunately, our observational coding taxonomy did not distinguish between positive and negative social overtures to (and from) peers. However, in previous studies, researchers have established that inhibited and socially withdrawn preschoolers are less socially competent than their typically developing same-age peers (e.g., Rubin et al., 1991; Bohlin et al., 2005). For example, inhibited and withdrawn children have been observed to be less successful than their typically developing age-mates in being able to meet their social goals (Rubin and Krasnor, 1986; Stewart and Rubin, 1995). Furthermore, inhibited and withdrawn preschoolers have been found to be less able than their more sociable age-mates to generate competent and flexible strategies to join others in play or to establish friendships (Rubin and Krasnor, 1986). Perhaps these latter difficulties may explain why the BI children in the present sample were unable to successfully initiate sustained social interaction or to capitalize on opportunities offered by peers to engage in social play.

      Lastly, it may have been possible that the bids for social interaction received by inhibited children were not for the purpose of initiating coordinated and positive social play. Thus, despite the lack of between group differences in teacher ratings of peer exclusion, it is possible that more subtle negative peer interactions are not as noticeable to teachers at this developmental stage. Notably, preschool teachers are more likely to notice physical aggression and defiance as forms of bullying, but often overlook bullying that occurs in verbal and relational forms (Tepetaş et al., 2010). Furthermore, as the children in the current study were in their first years of school, they may not have reached the point at which solitary behavior is considered, by peers, to be abnormal (Younger et al., 1993). Thus, the BI preschoolers who expressed reticent and solitary behavior in the classroom may not have been viewed as being “easy targets” for peer victimization, exclusion, and rejection (Ladd, 2006; Rubin et al., 2009) as is the case for older, elementary school-aged socially withdrawn children. To further pinpoint the emergence of this developmental transactional process, researchers would do well to examine the content and quality of inhibited and socially withdrawn children’s peer interactions across time.

      As expected, inhibited children were reported, by teachers, to engage in significantly less rough-and-tumble play compared to their typically developing peers. Significantly, rough-and-tumble play can be distinguished from acts of aggression in that the former is not considered to involve a goal to harm the play partner (Pellegrini, 2002). Indeed, there is a growing body of research regarding the benefits of “adventurous play” for children. More specifically, play in which children have the opportunity to take developmentally appropriate risks in a playful manner has been linked with reduced social anxiety later on in childhood (Majdandžić et al., 2018). Thus, there are clear potential benefits for supporting inhibited children’s adventurous play in an effort to mitigate their already elevated risk for later social anxiety. Teachers play a critical role in increasing children’s access to adventurous play, and several school-based interventions have been developed with the goal of facilitating opportunities for risk and challenge in children’s play (see Nesbit et al., 2021 for a systematic review). Nevertheless, further research is needed to identify the most effective ways of supporting children’s adventurous play and eliminating school-related barriers to implementing related interventions.

      With regard to teacher interactions, children in the behaviorally inhibited sample spent significantly less time in play or in conversation with their teachers compared to their typically developing peers. While teachers initiated social interactions with children in both samples at similar frequencies, inhibited children made significantly fewer initiations to their teachers. Over time, if inhibited children lack the repertoire of social skills to support effective communication with their teachers, they may face challenges advocating for their needs to be met in the classroom. Difficulties vocalizing their needs to teachers may also make inhibited children more susceptible to peer victimization across the school years (Rubin et al., 2009). Indeed, researchers have shown that inhibited children lack closeness in their relationships with their teachers, even when they are engaged in fewer personal conflicts in the classroom setting (Rudasill et al., 2006; Thijs and Koomen, 2009). Given the significant role of positive teacher-child relationships in supporting both social and academic success (Rudasill et al., 2006), it is of prime importance to improve behaviorally inhibited children’s ability to connect with, and benefit from, their relationships with their teachers.

      Our findings have several implications for prevention/intervention efforts for inhibited young children. First, the significant differences between inhibited and typically developing children highlight tangible opportunities for early intervention efforts. When inhibited children engage in less social interaction within their first years of school, they naturally encounter fewer opportunities to gain knowledge of social relationships and utilize social skills (Rubin et al., 2009). To disrupt this negative developmental process from unfolding, intervention programs would benefit from targeting inhibited children’s social skills, with the goal to generalize the learned skills to the school setting, and ultimately increase positive peer and teacher interactions. Along with age-appropriate play and social skills, it may be particularly beneficial for such programs to equip inhibited children with assertive communication skills to ensure that their needs are not overlooked. Importantly, engagement in reticent or solitary-passive play may not, in and of itself, warrant intervention to mitigate the risk for developing anxiety; other factors that may underlie the expression of these behaviors must be taken into account (Coplan and Rubin, 2001). Such other child factors include the ability to regulate emotion and the ability to understand the perspectives and feelings of others. These factors must be assessed to determine whether, or which type of intervention is necessary to best support children’s social and emotional development. Furthermore, teachers can play an important role in scaffolding inhibited children’s social development in the classroom. In the current sample, inhibited children engaged in less group activity with peers and less teacher interaction compared to their typically developing. Teachers and other educational staff may benefit from evidence-based strategies to engage inhibited children in both adult and peer interactions. To facilitate inhibited children’s social skill development and offer naturalistic opportunities for sustained social interaction, teachers may benefit from intervention/prevention efforts that incorporate social skills and associated group-based activities into the regular classroom curriculum (Coplan and Rudasill, 2016).

      The current study expands on previous work in several ways. First, we discovered, for the first time, that the oft-used BIQ allows a distinction to be made between the classroom free-play behaviors of young, inhibited children and their uninhibited counterparts. Knowing that the parent-reported BIQ can distinguish between inhibited and uninhibited children’s behaviors in both unfamiliar and familiar settings will be useful in the screening of children in need of intervention (e.g., The Turtle ProgramChronis-Tuscano et al., 2022; The Cool Little Kids ProgramRapee et al., 2005).

      Relatedly, by comparing children with elevated BI with a sample of typically developing children in the school setting, we were able to gain insight into inhibited children’s behaviors in the context of their familiar peers, rather than in an unfamiliar laboratory setting comprising groups of unacquainted children. Although BI is characterized by wariness in the context of novelty, examining children in their natural settings provides opportunities to better understand the ways in which inhibited youth’s socioemotional development can be optimally supported across settings.

      Furthermore, multiple informants’ reports (teacher and parent) were utilized in the current study to characterize the children’s behaviors, along with objective school-based observations. A multi-informant approach is essential, as child behaviors have repeatedly been shown to vary across environments and caregivers (De Los Reyes et al., 2013).

      It is important to acknowledge the limitations of the current study. First, many of the children from both samples attended highly resourced preschools. As these settings are not representative of all preschools, it will be important for future researchers to take the classroom context and curriculum into account to ensure that various school formats are incorporated into the sample. Second, studies would benefit from including a measure of classroom emotional climate, which has been shown to buffer against socially withdrawn youths’ experiences of peer rejection and victimization (Gazelle, 2006). Third, a measure of children’s language development was not included as part of the current study. While it is possible that language delays may account for fewer social initiations to peers and teachers (e.g., Coplan and Weeks, 2009), word approximations, single words, phrases, and sentences are all sufficient for a code of social initiation to others as part of the Play Observation Scale (Rubin, 1982). Moreover, children are able to engage in collaborative group play (i.e., with a common goal or purpose) without verbal communication to receive a code of “group play” on the Play Observation Scale. Nevertheless, in the future, researchers should incorporate measures of language skills when evaluating behavioral inhibition to disentangle the constructs of verbal communication and sociality. Finally, children in the current study were measured at one timepoint in their school classrooms. As peer experiences in the classroom setting may impact behavior across time (Almas et al., 2011), in the future, would do well to assess child behavior at multiple timepoints.

      In sum, the goal of the current study was to compare the in-school behaviors of two distinct groups of preschoolers – one comprising typical children and the other comprising children identified as dispositionally behaviorally inhibited. The study was designed to establish whether BI was associated with the display of solitude in the company of familiar peers. Indices of BI were drawn from parents and trained observers. Findings from the present study suggested that children high in BI differed from typically developing children in the extent to which they were observed to engage in social reticence and solitude in the school setting. While inhibited children engaged in more reticent and solitary behaviors and less group-based interactive play, they received similar amounts of social initiations from their classmates. The findings provide evidence for the social challenges inhibited children face in familiar peer contexts, and indicate that young, inhibited children may have difficulties capitalizing on their peers’ advances to foster social connection. These findings have several implications for early intervention and prevention efforts, as children high in BI may require additional support from parents and teachers to develop social skills through peer interaction.

      Data availability statement

      The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical and privacy restrictions. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the corresponding author.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board, University of Maryland-College Park. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

      Author contributions

      LD, DN, KS, and KR contributed to the conceptualization and design of the study. LD, DN, and KS organized the database. DN and KS performed the statistical analyses. LD, DN, and KR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. NW, SP, HF, CD, and AC-T reviewed and edited this manuscript. LD, DN, KS, SP, HF, and CD were responsible for project administration. KR and AC-T designed the original study and acquired the funding for this project. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      Funding

      This project was funded by a grant from NIH R01MH103253-01 awarded to AC-T and KR.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      The authors would like to thank the participating families, teachers, and school administrators that made this study possible. The authors would also like to thank the study personnel and research assistants from the University of Maryland Laboratory for the Study of Child and Family Relationships for their years of hard work and dedication to this project.

      References Almas A. N. Phillips D. A. Henderson H. A. Hane A. H. Degnan K. A. Moas O. L. . (2011). The relations between infant negative reactivity, non-maternal childcare, and children’s interactions with familiar and unfamiliar peers. Soc. Dev. 20, 718740. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00605.x, PMID: 22563147 Asendorpf J. B. (1991). Development of inhibited children’s coping with unfamiliarity. Child Dev. 62, 14601474. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01618.x, PMID: 1786728 Bakeman R. Brownlee J. R. (1980). The strategic use of parallel play: a sequential analysis. Child Dev. 51, 873878. doi: 10.2307/1129476 Ballespí S. Jané M. C. Riba M. D. (2012). Who should report abnormal behavior at preschool age? The case of behavioral inhibition. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 43, 4869. doi: 10.1007/s10578-011-0250-5, PMID: 21915723 Bishop G. Spence S. H. McDonald C. (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition? Child Dev. 74, 18991917. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00645.x, PMID: 14669903 Bohlin G. Hagekull B. Andersson K. (2005). Behavioral inhibition as a precursor of peer social competence in early school age: the interplay with attachment and nonparental care. Merrill-Palmer Q. 51, 119. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23096047. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2005.0001 Broeren S. Muris P. (2010). A psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition questionnaire in a non-clinical sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 41, 214229. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0162-9, PMID: 19921537 Brooker R. J. Kiel E. J. Buss K. A. (2016). Early social fear predicts kindergarteners' socially anxious behaviors: direct associations, moderation by inhibitory control, and differences from nonsocial fear. Emotion 16, 9971010. doi: 10.1037/emo0000135, PMID: 27213729 Buss K. A. Davis E. L. Kiel E. J. Brooker R. J. Beekman C. Early M. C. (2013). Dysregulated fear predicts social wariness and social anxiety symptoms during kindergarten. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 42, 603616. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.769170, PMID: 23458273 Chronis-Tuscano A. Degnan K. A. Pine D. S. Perez-Edgar K. Henderson H. A. Diaz Y. . (2009). Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48, 928935. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09df, PMID: 19625982 Chronis-Tuscano A. Novick D. R. Danko C. M. Smith K. Wagner N. J. Wang C. H. . (2022). Early intervention for inhibited young children: a randomized controlled trial comparing the turtle program and cool little kids. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 63, 273281. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13475, PMID: 34184792 Clauss J. A. Blackford J. U. (2012). Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: a meta-analytic study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 10661075.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.002, PMID: 23021481 Coplan R. J. Rubin K. H. (2001). Exploring and assessing nonsocial play in the preschool: the development and validation of the preschool play behavior scale. Soc. Dev. 7, 7291. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00052 Coplan R. J. Rubin K. H. Fox N. A. Calkins S. D. Stewart S. L. (1994). Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: distinguishing among reticence, and passive, and active solitude in young children. Child Dev. 65, 129137. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00739.x, PMID: 8131643 Coplan R.J. Rudasill K.M. (2016). Quiet at school. New York: Teachers’ College Press. Coplan R. J. Weeks M. (2009). Shy and soft-spoken: shyness, pragmatic language, and socio-emotional adjustment in early childhood. Infant Child Dev. 18, 238254. doi: 10.1002/icd.622 De Los Reyes A. Henry D. B. Tolan P. H. Wakschlag L. S. (2013). Linking informant discrepancies to observed variations in young children’s disruptive behavior. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 637652. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9307-3, PMID: 19247829 Degnan K. A. Fox N. A. (2007). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: multiple levels of a resilience process. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 729746. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000363, PMID: 17705900 Degnan K. A. Fox N. A. Henderson H. A. Rubin K. H. Nichols K. (2008). Predicting social wariness in middle childhood: the moderating roles of childcare history, maternal personality and maternal behavior. Soc. Dev. 17, 471487. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00437.x, PMID: 20463856 Eaves L. C. Wingert H. D. Ho H. H. Mickelson E. C. R. (2006). Screening for autism spectrum disorders with the social communication questionnaire. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 27, S95S103. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200604002-00007 Enders C. K. (2001). The performance of the full information maximum likelihood estimator in multiple regression models with missing data. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 61, 713740. doi: 10.1177/0013164401615001 Fox N. A. Henderson H. A. Marshall P. J. Nichols K. E. Ghera M. M. (2005). Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 235262. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532, PMID: 15709935 Fox N. A. Henderson H. A. Rubin K. H. Calkins S. D. Schmidt L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Dev. 72, 121. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00262, PMID: 11280472 Garcia Coll C. Kagan J. Reznick J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Dev. 55, 10051019. doi: 10.2307/1130152 Gazelle H. (2006). Class climate moderates peer relations and emotional adjustment in children with an early history of anxious solitude: a child × environment model. Dev. Psychol. 42, 11791192. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1179, PMID: 17087551 Henderson H. A. Marshall P. J. Fox N. A. Rubin K. H. (2004). Psychophysiological and behavioral evidence for varying forms and functions of nonsocial behaviors in preschoolers. Child Dev. 75, 251263. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00667.x, PMID: 15015688 Kagan J. (1989). “The concept of behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar” in Perspectives on behavioral inhibition. ed. Reznick J. S. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press), 123. Kagan J. Reznick J. S. Snidman N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Dev. 58, 14591473. doi: 10.2307/1130685 Kagan J. Reznick J. S. Snidman N. Gibbons J. Johnson M. O. (1988). Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Dev. 59, 15801589. doi: 10.2307/1130672, PMID: 3208569 Kagan J. Snidman N. Kahn V. Towsley S. Steinberg L. Fox N. (2007). The preservation of two infant temperaments into adolescence. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 72, 195. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2007.00436.x, PMID: 17661895 Kiel E. J. Buss K. A. (2011). Prospective relations among fearful temperament, protective parenting, and social withdrawal: the role of maternal accuracy in a moderated mediation framework. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 953966. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9516-4, PMID: 21537895 Kiel E. J. Premo J. E. Buss K. A. (2015). Maternal encouragement to approach novelty: a curvilinear relation to change in anxiety for inhibited toddlers. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 44, 433444. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0038-3, PMID: 26050798 Kim J. Klein D. N. Olino T. M. Dyson M. W. Dougherty L. R. Durbin C. E. (2011). Psychometric properties of the Behavioral inhibition questionnaire in preschool children. J. Pers. Assess. 93, 545555. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2011.608756, PMID: 21999378 Ladd G. W. (2006). Peer rejection, aggressive or withdrawn behavior, and psychological maladjustment from ages 5 to 12: an examination of four predictive models. Child Dev. 77, 822846. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00905.x Ladd G. W. Profilet S. M. (1996). The child behavior scale: a teacher-report measure of young children’s aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Dev. Psychol. 32, 10081024. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.6.1008 Majdandžić M. de Vente W. Colonnesi C. Bögels S. M. (2018). Fathers’ challenging parenting behavior predicts less subsequent anxiety symptoms in early childhood. Behav. Res. Ther. 109, 1828. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.07.007, PMID: 30077804 Muris P. van Brakel A. M. Arntz A. Schouten E. (2011). Behavioral inhibition as a risk factor for the development of childhood anxiety disorders: a longitudinal study. J. Child Fam. Stud. 20, 157170. doi: 10.1007/s10826-010-9365-8, PMID: 21475710 Nelson L. J. Rubin K. H. Fox N. A. (2005). Social withdrawal, observed peer acceptance, and the development of self-perceptions in children ages 4 to 7 years. Early Child. Res. Q. 20, 185200. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2005.04.007 Nesbit R. J. Bagnall C. L. Harvey K. Dodd H. F. (2021). Perceived barriers and facilitators of adventurous play in schools: a qualitative systematic review. Children 8:681. doi: 10.3390/children8080681, PMID: 34438572 Pellegrini A. D. (2002). “Rough-and-tumble play from childhood through adolescence: development and possible functions” in Blackwell handbook of childhood social development. eds. Smith P. K. Hart C. H. (London: Blackwell), 438453. Penela E. C. Walker O. L. Degnan K. A. Fox N. A. Henderson H. A. (2015). Early behavioral inhibition and emotion regulation: pathways toward social competence in middle childhood. Child Dev. 86, 12271240. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12384, PMID: 26014351 Poole K. L. Schmidt L. A. (2018). Smiling through the shyness: the adaptive function of positive affect in shy children. Emotion 19, 160170. doi: 10.1037/emo0000426, PMID: 29565610 R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Rapee R. M. Kennedy S. Ingram M. Edwards S. Sweeney L. (2005). Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited preschool children. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 488497. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.488 Rosseel Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 136. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02 Rowe D. C. Plomin R. (1977). Temperament in early childhood. J. Pers. Assess. 41, 150156. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4102_5 Rubin K. H. (1982). Non-social play in preschoolers: necessarily evil? Child Dev. 53, 651657. doi: 10.2307/1129376 Rubin K. H. Barstead M. G. Smith K. A. Bowker J. C. (2018). “Peer relations and the behaviorally inhibited child” in Behavioral inhibition. eds. Pérez-Edgar K. Fox N. (Germany: Springer), 157184. Rubin K. H. Burgess K. B. Hastings P. D. (2002). Stability and social–behavioral consequences of toddlers’ inhibited temperament and parenting behaviors. Child Dev. 73, 483495. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00419, PMID: 11949904 Rubin K. H. Chronis-Tuscano A. (2021). Perspectives on social withdrawal in childhood: past, present, and prospects. Child Dev. Perspect. 15, 160167. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12417, PMID: 34434251 Rubin K. H. Coplan R. J. Bowker J. C. (2009). Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 60, 141171. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642, PMID: 18851686 Rubin K. H. Hastings P. D. Stewart S. L. Henderson H. A. Chen X. (1997). The consistency and concomitants of inhibition: some of the children, all of the time. Child Dev. 68, 467483. doi: 10.2307/1131672, PMID: 9249961 Rubin K. H. Hymel S. Mills R. S. L. Rose-Krasnor L. (1991). “Conceptualizing different pathways to and from social isolation in childhood” in Internalizing and externalizing expressions of dysfunction. eds. Cicchetti D. Toth S. (United States: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc), 91122. Rubin K. H. Krasnor L. R. (1986). “Social-cognitive and social behavioral perspectives on problem solving” in Cognitive perspectives on children's social and behavioral development. The Minnesota symposia on child psychology. ed. Perlmutter M. , vol. 18 (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 168. Rudasill K. M. Rimm-Kaufmann S. E. Justice L. M. Pence K. (2006). Temperament and language skills as predictors of teacher-child relationship quality in preschool. Early Educ. Dev. 17, 271291. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.12.003 Spence S. H. Rapee R. McDonald C. Ingram M. (2001). The structure of anxiety symptoms among preschoolers. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 12931316. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00098-X Stewart S. L. Rubin K. H. (1995). The social problem-solving skills of anxious-withdrawn children. Dev. Psychopathol. 7, 323336. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006532 Stifter C. A. Fox N. A. Porges S. W. (1989). Facial expressivity and vagal tone in five and ten-month-old infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 12, 127137. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(89)90001-5 Tarullo A. R. Mliner S. Gunnar M. R. (2011). Inhibition and exuberance in preschool classrooms: associations with peer social experiences and changes in cortisol across the preschool year. Dev. Psychol. 47, 13741388. doi: 10.1037/a0024093, PMID: 21688898 Tepetaş G. Ş. Akgun E. Altun S. A. (2010). Identifying preschool teachers’ opinion about peer bullying. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2, 16751679. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.964 Thijs J. Koomen H. M. Y. (2009). Toward a further understanding of teachers’ reports of early teacher–child relationships: examining the roles of behavior appraisals and attributions. Early Child. Res. Q. 24, 186197. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.03.001 Walker O. L. Henderson H. A. Degnan K. A. Penela E. C. Fox N. A. (2014). Associations between behavioral inhibition and children’s social problem-solving behavior during social exclusion. Soc. Dev. 23, 487501. doi: 10.1111/sode.12053, PMID: 25360063 Younger A. J. Gentile C. Burgess K. B. (1993). “Children’s perceptions of social withdrawal: changes across age” in Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood. eds. Rubin K. Asendorpf J. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 215236.
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.hknbsoft.com.cn
      enyou.org.cn
      guituapp.com.cn
      www.kqxiwq.com.cn
      www.ffokkx.com.cn
      www.jzchain.com.cn
      fmlpjs.com.cn
      relayr.com.cn
      psexwn.com.cn
      www.oojoo.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p