Edited by: Milagros Sainz, Open University of Catalonia, Spain
Reviewed by: Itziar Fernández, National University of Distance Education, Spain; María Cecilia Fernández DArraz, Temuco Catholic University, Chile
This article was submitted to Gender, Sex and Sexualities, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
For nearly a decade, two science interventions anchored in project-based learning (PBL) principles have been shown to increase student science learning in 3rd grade and high school physical science classes. Both interventions employed a randomized control trial of several thousand students (N = 3,271 in 3rd grade and N = 4,238 in 10th, 11th, and 12th grades). Incorporating a rich background of research studies and reports, the two interventions are based on the ideas of PBL as well as the National Academies of Science’s publications, including how children learn; how science learning and instruction can be transformed; and the performance expectations for science learning articulated in the Next Generation of Science Standards. Results show significant positive increases in student academic, social, and emotional learning in both elementary and secondary school. These findings can be traced, in part, to carefully crafted experiential participatory activities and high-quality instructional materials which act as strong facilitators for knowledge acquisition and use. Reviewing the innovations undertaken by these two interventions, this article describes the importance of studying social and emotional factors ‘
香京julia种子在线播放
National and international assessments indicate that US students’ academic performance in science is barely reaching average scores, especially in junior and senior high school (
These less than promising science achievement test results were evident before the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic. The latest projections, especially among those with the most limited economic and social resources, is that these students are likely to experience major academic, social, and emotional problems at school this coming year and perhaps throughout their careers and beyond (
For the past several years, two science curriculum interventions have been implemented and evaluated in elementary and secondary schools (
One key, new addition found in the ML-PBL and CESE interventions was the explicit importance placed on social and emotional learning and its relationship to science achievement (see chapter by
Several considerations in the design of the interventions were identified for understanding social and emotional learning for both elementary and secondary students. First, and most importantly, was the selection of social and emotional constructs that were appropriate for science learning in classrooms (
For purposes of measurement at the elementary level, social and emotional learning states (i.e., patterns of feelings during activities within specific time periods) were assessed when students were in their science classes. This was a one-time measure, validated through a variety of statistical procedures (see,
More recently, there has been increased attention within the psychological community to investigate the relationship between the impact of social and emotional learning on student performance in classrooms. Previously, these issues were rarely isolated to the learning context or used to direct teachers’ practices in their classrooms for the purpose of supporting all students’ academic performance and well-being (
The intentionality of inclusionary social and emotional learning opportunities in classrooms complements PBL principles (
The ML-PBL and CESE interventions included carefully crafted lessons which are planned with a series of intra- and inter-connected experiences which coherently increase in scientific knowledge and practices (
Concentrating on several of the most important social and emotional learning measures, these two interventions also underscored the importance of obtaining such information on these constructs when students are in their science classes. This led in both intervention studies to several assumptions regarding social and emotional measurement: (1) SEL is not a distinctive single psychological state, one can be engaged and feel successful and in control while also feeling a sense of stress; (2) SEL is time variant, in that a confluence of SEL states vary in intensity across the course of one’s daily life experiences; and (3) SEL is highly susceptible to contextual environmental conditions such as the instructional activities in the classroom.
Recognizing developmental differences in literacy, social and emotional awareness of self and others, and technological skills (
Beginning as a design-based study for 3rd grade, the ML-PBL intervention underwent several rounds of revisions and testing over the course of 4 years, including teacher experiments, classroom pilots, a field-test, and most recently an efficacy study to determine whether the ML-PBL intervention enhanced students’ science academic, social, and emotional learning. A randomized control trial was conducted in 46 Michigan schools (23 treatment and 23 control) which included four regions in the state. The final analytic sampled included a total of 2,371 students. The treatment condition included curriculum materials and professional learning experiences for teachers. To assess if there was a significant difference in academic science learning, a three-level hierarchical linear model (HLM) was conducted. This method was used to account for nesting of students within classrooms within schools. Results showed that the treatment students outperformed the control students by a .277 standard deviation on an objective summative test which is a substantial treatment effect (
The above work also investigated specific research questions related to social and emotional learning, specifically, whether the treatment support more positive responses on measures in self-reflection, collaboration, and responsibility for their own and others’ work. It is important to underscore that few studies measure elementary school students social and emotional learning in their science classes (
As mentioned above, few studies have been able to examine the impact of engagement on elementary science learning. We chose to further examine the relationship between engagement and achievement as research has shown positive relationships between students’ determination to be engaged in the classroom and science achievement (
During the beginning and first year of the Covid-19 pandemic, the ML-PBL team were able to observe students in their science classes
The research questions for this new development study include:
Can 3rd graders reliably produce measures of interest, skill, and challenge ‘
When studied with repeated measures, do interest, skill, and challenge load onto a single construct of engagement?
Using the same constructs of interest, skill, and challenge as fundamental dimensions of engagement, during the pandemic, the team developed a new methodology and series of items for 3rd graders that relied on data collected situated in specific lessons within each unit. Keeping with the idea of measuring social and emotional learning ‘
For each unit during three different time periods, students were asked questions pertaining to specific measures of interest, skill, and challenge (see page 7 for fuller description). The three different time points were chosen based on the goals of each lesson, allowing us to collect more data from lessons that focused specifically on driving questions, investigation, building a model, or creating a final artifact. These items are situated directly in the context of each lesson. Six focal lessons, which contained the following features: driving question, modeling, investigation, and development of a final artifact, were sampled. For example, in the beginning of the toy unit after observing a toy rocket and how it moves, students were asked for interest, “I like asking questions about how the air rocket moves;” for skills, “I can ask questions about how toy rockets move the way they do;” and for challenge, “I had to think a lot to ask new questions about how rockets move.” With respect to collaboration, the students were asked, “When I worked with my classmates, we came up with different questions about the way the toy rocket moved;” and for ownership, “The questions I asked about the air rocket’s motion were important to me and my classmates.”
The data collection procedures used for measuring this engagement measure followed the original collection of the SEL survey, but with greater frequency. Teacher administered the four-question OLM survey to third grade students immediately following the lesson. The first three questions were based on engagement: interest, skill, and challenge. The fourth varied by form (A, B, or C) and rotated between collaboration, persistence, agency, time and outcome by lesson. A 4-point Likert scale was used (strongly disagree, disagree, agree, strongly agree) with students circling icons of thumbs up and thumbs down. In the pilot of the SEL measures at the elementary school level, at three different times during each of the four units, the teachers hand out paper copies of the engagement questions to the students in their class. The teachers then read aloud each of the questions, one at a time. After each question is read, students circle the corresponding thumb icon on their paper. In the cases where students circled more than one response, in the median score of responses was recorded.
The sample for this analysis came from 25 3rd grade classrooms in Michigan and included 596 students with a total of 3,369 responses for an average of 6 repeated measures per student.
Their responses to the engagement questions across the four ML-PBL units were analyzed. For the reliability of this survey, a Cronbach’s alpha was used to estimate the reliability.
For understanding whether the interest, skill, and challenge loaded onto a construct of engagement, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted. Factor loadings for each item onto this construct were estimated.
The descriptive statistics from the survey, including the items of interest, skill, challenge, and an additional question, are reported in
Sample descriptives.
N | Mean | St. Dev | Min | Max | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Interest | 3,369 | 3.29 | 0.87 | 0 | 4 |
Skill | 3,330 | 3.25 | 0.84 | 0 | 4 |
Challenge | 3,367 | 2.79 | 1.08 | 0 | 4 |
Q4 | 3,362 | 3.18 | 1.01 | 0 | 4 |
A confirmatory factor analysis confirmed a unidimensional model with the following factor loadings for: interest (0.77); skill (0.41); and challenge (0.26). The overall reliability of the engagement measure is a Cronbach’s Alpha of 0.53. The overall reliability and item level reliabilities are reported in
Reliability using Cronbach’s Alpha.
Item-test | Item-rest | Avg. interitem cov | Alpha | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Interest | 0.67 | 0.39 | 0.17 | 0.4 |
Skill | 0.62 | 0.34 | 0.2 | 0.44 |
Challenge | 0.61 | 0.21 | 0.25 | 0.56 |
Q4 | 0.69 | 0.35 | 0.17 | 0.42 |
Test scale | 0.2 | 0.53 |
Additional analyses are being undertaken to study variation in engagement by lesson activities and individual level variables.
The secondary school intervention, “Crafting Engaging Science Environments,” (CESE) is a high school chemistry and physics PBL intervention similar to but independent of the elementary intervention. Both interventions meet the NGSS performance expectations and incorporate NRC three-dimensional learning and principles of PBL. CESE was administered to a diverse group of over 4,238 students in chemistry and physics classes in 70 high schools. The design like the elementary study was an efficacy study that involved a randomized control trial in California and Michigan. This intervention also included curriculum materials and professional learning for the teachers. Results were estimated using a two-level HLM with the outcome being the student level performance on the physical science items from the Michigan State Science Assessment and the main predictor of interest being treatment at the school level. For this estimation, a pretest and student demographics were included as covariates. Results show that treatment students, on average, performed 0.20 standard deviations higher than control students on an independently developed summative science assessment (
A major part of the study was investigating why secondary students, as shown in national and international studies fail to be engaged in their science classes which likely affects their interest in science learning, achievement, and science career ambitions (
The PBL framework, which stresses solving personally meaningful questions and encouraging instructional activities that require collaboration and are intellectually challenging, was ideally suited to test the constructs of engagement and their impact on academic science achievement. The work is situated in the work of
In contrast to those who have conceptualize engagement as a general trend, this model of engagement identifies engagement as domain specific in duration and in intensity, which fits more closely with current definitions of situational interest in science learning (see
The PBL curriculum, as discussed above, begins with a driving question when students are in specific situations and faced with a problem or phenomenon that is relevant and meaningful to their lives, such as: “how can I build a safer car?” To build that car, students need to have the necessary knowledge and skills to create a solution. Irrespective of the students’ skill level, finding a reasonable solution should be a challenge, one that sparks determination. When students are fully engaged in a learning task, this is defined as an optimal learning moment (OLM). These moments do not just happen, but need to be artfully constructed and coherent, which is yet another fundamental aspect of PBL which inspires the acquisition of new knowledge, the use of imagination, and stretching problem-solving abilities.
Optimal learning moments can be verified and understood by other related subjective experiences occurring at nearly the same time. For example, it is expected that when involved in these activities’ students feel successful, confident, active, happy, and enjoyment with the activity (
During the field test of the CESE intervention, an ‘
While these ESM results were promising, there were several limitations. This was a pilot not a randomized trial where students in a treatment and control group could be compared. Rather it was the case that measures of engagement and feelings regarding challenge were measured using a single case design, where each classroom acts as its own experimental control (vacillating from treatment periods to times in the classroom when it was “business as usual”). These repeated periods were assessed to determine if the treatment influenced students’ engagement. Although, the pilot study results showed that more engaged students had higher grades it could not be directly attributable the CESE intervention. However, the positive nature of the results prompted the team to use the ESM in the future efficacy study (2018–2019) in selected treatment and control classrooms (
Preliminary results on the measures of engagement show that when considering levels of interest skill, and challenge, student engagement levels increase and are accompanied by other positive social and emotional affects, as well as decline in feeling of boredom and confusion. These findings show that concepts such as engagement, creativity, and problem-solving are situationally specific and share nearly equal variance when contrasted with person-level characteristics. In other words, even if a student is not interested in a topic or whose previous science achievement scores are below average, a carefully created situation can alter their negative predilections toward science, bringing considerable strength to the “nurture” side of learning especially when breaking from traditional types of assessment memorization and instead using imagination, problem-solving, and taking different points of view into consideration when engaged in scientific practices. However, these are preliminary results and an important question is the level of challenge and what impact it has on motivating higher engagement and learning for all students in specific contexts. (see,
Most recently, a deeper examination of “challenge experiences” in science class has been conducted (
The first starts with a
The second analysis uses data from a sample of students from the field test and efficacy study to understand the use of ESM and student’s variation in emotions across years. These analyses employ a series of repeated measures estimation of students situational perceived challenge, stress, anxiety, determination, giving up, and confusion to understand how the relationship between challenge and giving up and confusion is mitigated by stress and anxiety. We assume that challenge is important in driving learning; however, if challenge is correlated to giving up and confusion, this would lead to a negative relationship between challenge and learning. This leads to a question of whether anxiety and stress may be stronger mediating factors in the relationship between challenge and giving up and confusion.
The research questions for study 2 were:
How does perceived challenge vary by individual students?
How does the relationship between perceived challenge and positive and negative emotions vary by individual students?
What is the relationship between students’ perceived challenge coupled with stress and anxiety and determination, giving up, and confusion?
During the field test of the CESE (2013–2018), a total of 867 students were reported with the ESM. For the efficacy study (2018–19), a total 545 students were reported with the ESM for a total of 1,412 students combined. The phones were programmed to alert the students randomly 6–8 times per day (at least 3–4 times when they had science lessons) over an assigned period. An initial ESM prompt would occur in the beginning, mid- and late point of a study session automatically set up by researchers using the PACO app. Students were asked to respond to an identical questionnaire (nearly 30 items) within a 15 min window. Two reminders would occur 10 and 15 min after the initial prompt. On average, it takes about 90 s to complete items. Each day all participants received eight to 10 beeps on their smartphones which gave them 40 total response opportunities during a study period. We preprogrammed the beep schedule randomly and guarantee a minimum of 1–3 beeps occurring in science classes, resulting in 5 to 15 beeps per person in this study. In total, the data comprised 3,234 responses. The average valid beeps per student is 6 in science classes. We conducted two separate analyses, one which only analyzed the students in the efficacy study and a second analysis from both the field and efficacy studies.
The first analyses reported is from the efficacy study which contained a diverse population of students living in both Michigan and California with an overrepresentation of students for whom English is not their first language, as one of our sites was a mile from the Mexican border. Among the efficacy students’ sample, 315 (58%) had valid student background information, including Race/Ethnicity, gender, challenge experiences and science pretest scores. This student background survey was collected at the beginning of the year
Descriptive statistics of student sample.
Freq. | % | |
---|---|---|
Male | 151 | 48.55 |
Female | 160 | 51.45 |
Grade 10 | 81 | 31.89 |
Grade 11 | 146 | 57.48 |
Grade 12 | 27 | 10.63 |
White, (non-hispanic) | 200 | 60.4 |
Hispanic | 57 | 17.2 |
Black | 28 | 8.5 |
Asian | 18 | 5.4 |
Other | 11 | 3.3 |
Multiracial | 17 | 5.1 |
Total valid student info | 331 | |
Demographic info Missing | 214 | |
Mean | SD | |
Percentile ranking of pre-test | 62.69 | 22.03 |
Challenge | 2.31 | 0.74 |
The second analysis used the entirety of the sample from both the field and efficacy tests (demographic information is unavailable for this combined sample; however, the sampling scheme for the field and efficacy tests targeted schools with significant numbers of low-income and minority students). The entire sample was used in the second analysis which uses aggregate statistical modeling to understand the validity of these relationships across many years (2013–2019).
To measure engagement, studies typically employ surveys which are rarely conducted ‘
CESE ESM instrument.
Q1 Where were you when you were signaled? |
Q2 What science class were you in? |
Q3 Which best describes what you were doing in science when signaled? |
Q4 What were you doing when signaled? |
Q5 What were you learning about in science when signaled? |
Q6 Who were you with? |
Q7 Were you doing the main activity because you… |
Q8 Was what you were doing… |
Q9 Were you interested in what you were doing? |
Q10 Did you feel skilled at what you were doing? |
Q11 Did you feel challenged by what you were doing? |
Q12 Did you feel like giving up? |
Q13 How much were you concentrating? |
Q14 Do you enjoy what you are doing? |
Q15 Did you feel like you were in control of what you were doing? |
Q16 Were you succeeding? |
Q17 Was this activity important for you? |
Q18 How important is this activity in relation to your future goals/plans? |
Q19 Were you living up to the expectations of others? |
Q20 Were you living up to your expectations? |
Q21 I was so absorbed in what I was doing that time flew. |
Q22 How determined were you to accomplish the task? |
Q23 Were you feeling…Happy |
Q24 Were you feeling… Excited |
Q25 Were you feeling… Anxious |
Q26 Were you feeling… Competitive |
Q27 Were you feeling… Lonely |
Q28 Were you feeling… Stressed |
Q29 Were you feeling… Proud |
Q30 Were you feeling… Cooperative |
Q31 Were you feeling… Bored |
Q32 Were you feeling… Self-confident |
Q33 Were you feeling… Confused |
Q34 Were you feeling… Active |
In both analyses, students were beeped several times a day (7 times) during a week both inside and out of school and classes, with several more signals in science classes. Each classroom was randomly chosen for a specific week(s) during the intervention for data collection. Each data entry has a time stamp to indicate when the responses was collected. This approach is different from single survey as it records a set of repeated specific social and emotional measures interacting with specific activities, such as, doing a hands-on experiment in science class as compared to playing a video game. These responses are uploaded to a secured server which sends information to a cloud and are then quickly transformed into clean datasets and ready for analysis. Confidentiality is maintained by student anonymized identification numbers (It is important to note that all of our data collection and analyses underwent Institutional review board approval and received exempt status).
Screen shot of PACO app (
A more in-depth examination of ESM is shown in the case study analysis. A second analysis which relies on a hierarchical linear model (HLM) was used for the aggregate study of the field and efficacy tests. Beginning with the in-depth case study, three students were selected among varying levels of school achievement to analyze their variability in emotionality with graphic visualization. First a description is given for how an individual student experienced challenges across activities, locations, and companionship in their 4 days. Second, the three students’ emotional responses within each person’s positive and negative states when challenged is shown in
Dennis (Low-performing student) Experienced “Challenge” in situ across Context. Report Z-score Over 4 days. Pink color marks the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color marks the moments when a student is out of the school.
Megan (Average student) Experienced “Challenge” in situ across Context. Report Z-score Over 4 days. Pink color marks the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color marks the moments when a student is out of the school.
Collins (Above Average student) Experienced “Challenge” in situ across Context. Report Z-score Over 4 days. Pink color marks the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color marks the moments when a student is out of the school.
To understand the situational and individual differences for the students in the case study, their ESM responses were obtained throughout the day, including an oversample of beeps in their science classes (
Three student case study.
School performance | Gender | Average Perceived challenge (individual) all context | Average Perceived challenge (individual) in science classroom | n of moment | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dennis | Low-performing student | Male | 3.14 | 3.25 | 22 |
Megan | Average performance | Female | 2.51 | 2.25 | 20 |
Collins | Above average | Male | 2.29 | 2.3 | 24 |
A standardized z-score (mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1) of perceived challenge was calculated and took into account individual differences while also allowing for comparison across individuals on a common scale. The z-scored perceived challenge also provides an advantage for exploring the emotional response in different contexts. The students’ z-scores of challenge are compared across different settings and activities in these case studies. Additionally, the students’ positive emotional states, which are measured by “happy, enjoy, excited, success and competitive,” and negative emotional states, which are measured by “angry, stress, confused, give-up and anxious,” are compared across different levels of the students’ challenge levels using a different visualization. An average score of five emotional responses was used to represent the positive and negative states for the three cases. The five positive and negative emotional states were chosen based on earlier work (
The ESM asks students questions that correlate perceived challenge experiences that may confound the relationship with other positive and negative psychological states. Therefore, it is important to understand the influence confounding variables may have on students perceived social and emotional well-being. More specifically, in the case of perceived challenge, this new work has begun to examine the confounding effects of stress and anxiety on the relationship between challenge and two important negative psychological states, confusion and giving up, and one positive state of determination (
Then, using a repeated measures HLM, the relationship between challenge and confusion, giving up, and determination was explored first without covariates and then with stress and anxiety as covariates. The following two equations were estimated.
Model 1:
Model 2:
Where δ10 is the relationship between challenge and the outcome in the two models, v_0j is the student level random intercept and epsilon_ij is the beep level error term. The
In
Relative to the science classroom context, the out-of-school context (colored in light blue), Megan and Collins are less challenged especially when compared to Dennis. Overall, these three case studies show that the context of when students feel challenged can vary considerably by individuals and activities.
Recognizing the individual variability of experiencing challenge across contexts, the next question is whether emotional responses related to challenge differ by student. When challenged, is this experience more positive for Collins and Megan than Dennis, or do they all report similar feelings?
Dennis (Low-performing student) Experienced Challenge in Relation to Positive and Negative Psychological States. Report raw scores in positive and negative emotions. The dark blue color marks the lowest challenge moments (=1), whereas the light blue color marks the highest challenge moments (=4) in the science classroom.
Megan (Average student) Experienced Challenge in Relation to Positive and Negative Psychological States. Report raw scores in positive and negative emotions. The dark blue color marks the lowest challenge moments (=1), whereas the light blue color marks the highest challenge moments (=4) in the science classroom.
Collins (Above Average student) Experienced Challenge in Relation to Positive and Negative Psychological States (Low-performing student). Report raw scores in positive and negative emotions. The dark blue color marks the lowest challenge moments (=1), whereas the light blue color marks the highest challenge moments (=4) in the science classroom.
Among our three student cases, Dennis had fewer positive psychological states and reported more challenging tasks during his science class. His perception of high challenge (light blue bar) is less correlated with positive psychological states and more correlated with negative ones. Additionally, Dennis’ psychological states fluctuated more than the two other students. These fluctuations are more apparent when experiencing positive psychological states (e.g., feeling successful, confident) than his negative psychological states of confusion.
Megan, on the other hand, had a declining trend of positive psychological states over the 4 days. When she experienced high challenge tasks in the science classroom, her negative psychological states increased. However, for Collins, the above average student, his positive psychological states were more correlated with higher challenge. Additionally, during moments of rising challenge, Collins experienced other positive emotions. Overall, the relationship between challenge and positive and negative psychological states seem to vary across students and days (
Among all students in the ESM sample, challenge was closely related to negative emotions for some students, while for others, it was closely related to positive emotions, and for others, there was no relationship. However, these results do not indicate how stress and anxiety might be influencing the relationship of challenge with other positive and negative emotions. Instead of focusing on all positive and negative emotions, a few key variables were explored more deeply: confusion, giving up, and determination, which were all positively correlated with challenge as seen in
Pairwise correlations of challenge, stress, anxiety, giving up, determination, and confusion.
Stress | Anxious | Challenge | Give up | Determined | Confused | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stress | 1.000 | |||||
Anxious | 0.460 | 1.000 | ||||
Challenge | 0.310 | 0.270 | 1.000 | |||
Give up | 0.430 | 0.310 | 0.380 | 1.000 | ||
Determined | −0.028 | 0.093 | 0.220 | −0.099 | 1.000 | |
Confused | 0.54 | 0.400 | 0.420 | 0.490 | −0.022 | 1.000 |
However, importantly, confusion and giving up were also positively related to stress and anxiety, while determination was not. Therefore, the question arose was whether stress and anxiety may be accounting for this positive relationship between challenge and confusion and challenge and giving up.
From the repeated measures HLM, the positive relationship between challenge and confusion and challenge and giving up significantly decreased in absolute value, when including stress and anxiety as covariates (
The present findings extend previous research in at least two ways: First, these results provide a moment-level look at context differences in response to daily challenges in school, incorporating both the intra-individual as well as the inter-personal level across study one and study two. Both offer insights for each other to complete the puzzle of challenging experiences in students’ daily lives in school. These finding of significant context differences in intra-individual variability of experiencing challenge and other positive and negative states, to some degree, suggest that the relationship between perceived challenge, optimal learning moment, and psychological reactions is complex. Examining these relationships among these different emotions also offers that classroom learning, as we might have expected, is not a simple correlation with a specific experience but needs to be seen in context, over time, and in relationship to other events.
To further consider individual and contextual factors simultaneously, a designed statistical model like Simultaneous Equation Modeling (SEM) or Dynamic Structural Equation Modeling (DSEM) is essential to move this line of research forward. Second, complementing previous optimal learning moment literature on the states of the flow (
With respect to specific limitations of study 1, there are few ‘
Overall, more studies are needed to use these techniques to build a corpus of work so that a comparison across studies can be examined to understand the reliability and validity of these techniques and their results. Despite our limitations of not having more in-depth analyses of personal and environment influences on social and emotional learning, our work provides another lens for understanding how levels of engagement and motivation are related to achievement, especially today when COVID’s effects on these important relationships need further exploration. Additionally, more studies are needed on emotionality in classrooms that take into account student cultures, family histories, race/ethnicity, and gender.
Even though classrooms are busy fluid learning environments, results show it is possible to measure social and emotional experiences, but they vary considerably by context. Some students find certain types of activities more interesting than others, and their skill levels vary meeting similar challenging problems with a diverse set of reactions from boredom and confusion to determination and sense of success and accomplishment. These variations by context indicate that isolating a specific measure of emotionality may overlook the factors at work that could be deterrents to motivation and persistence in STEM. It seems critical that researchers attempting to increase motivation for students to become engaged in learning experiences need to focus on the environment and emotions which operate at the same moments within the same context. And most importantly when considering engagement, recognizing that students vary in their skill levels, and this may be affecting the pursuit of learning new skills and attempting challenging STEM problems.
The greatest challenge for researchers who wish to transform STEM learning environments is determining the important types of social and emotionality constructs that make the most sense given the subject matter and experiences when students are expected to be engaged. This work has deliberately focused on science classrooms, where the underlying instructional and curricular activities are crafted in accordance with recent reports for transformative pedagogical practices. The toolkit of social and emotional measures being considered are those that seem the most reasonable given the goals of the lessons and the phenomena and problems to be solved. However, in trying to disentangle the behavioral, cognitive, and emotionality of engagement, considering interest, skill, and challenge are imperative, as well as other social and emotional factors that also occur when valuing teamwork and collaboration for having students learn and work with others and reach a place of ownership of ideas and products.
During the in-depth study of engagement (i.e., interest, skill, and challenge), several new factors related to learning have occurred. Interest, as others have also recognized, is critical; however, it must be constructed around ideas that the students find purposeful to their own lives. Memorizing the elements of the periodic table without knowing the purpose behind understanding the properties of the atom is a non- starter to a student. However, why we need to understand the relationship of certain elements to each other and their impact on chemical reactions experienced in everyday life can become more meaningful to a student.
Students have different skill levels and when choosing group experiences being attentive to the likely variation in the classroom is indispensable. The importance of bringing everyone into the problem-solving activity and making it a reasonable challenge for all students is likely to affect their personal as well as the groups’ continued work on a project or problem. The idea here is not to construct activities that have the lowest level of skills but rather to offer various flexible routes to problem solving for all the students. Nonetheless, it is the case in PBL that there are certain disciplinary core ideas that are regarded as critical and that has to the starting point of the lessons. What students need is an awareness of their own confidence to face a challenge and how that can fit into the space of figuring out a phenomenon or solving a problem.
Moving students to learn something they do not know changes the nature of learning from memorization to using ideas. This type of learning poses another set of ideas, in that students are taking on something that they do not know but they could find out. This process exposes their vulnerabilities in of not knowing—for which they need to learn to be more comfortable with. This is particularly problematic for females especially in adolescence, where taking risks and exposing one’s vulnerabilities is typically a positive aspect of the socialization process they encounter (
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Michigan State University IRB. Written informed consent from the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
BS: conception and design of work, drafting the article, review of data analysis and interpretation, and critical revision. I-CC: data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article, and critical revisions of the article. LB: data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article, and critical revisions of the article. KB: data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article, and critical revisions of the article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
This study is supported by the National Science Foundation (OISE-1545684; PIs Barbara Schneider and Joseph Krajcik); George Lucas Educational Foundation—Lucas Education Research (PI Joseph Krajcik); and the John A. Hannah Chair in the College of Education at Michigan State University. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation and the George Lucas Educational Foundation.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
1