Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.960327 Psychology Conceptual Analysis Intervention initiatives to raise young people’s interest and participation in STEM SchneiderBarbara ChenI-Chien BradfordLydia* BartzKayla College of Education, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States

Edited by: Milagros Sainz, Open University of Catalonia, Spain

Reviewed by: Itziar Fernández, National University of Distance Education, Spain; María Cecilia Fernández DArraz, Temuco Catholic University, Chile

*Correspondence: Lydia Bradford, bradf134@msu.edu

This article was submitted to Gender, Sex and Sexualities, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

18 11 2022 2022 13 960327 02 06 2022 27 09 2022 Copyright © 2022 Schneider, Chen, Bradford and Bartz. 2022 Schneider, Chen, Bradford and Bartz

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

For nearly a decade, two science interventions anchored in project-based learning (PBL) principles have been shown to increase student science learning in 3rd grade and high school physical science classes. Both interventions employed a randomized control trial of several thousand students (N = 3,271 in 3rd grade and N = 4,238 in 10th, 11th, and 12th grades). Incorporating a rich background of research studies and reports, the two interventions are based on the ideas of PBL as well as the National Academies of Science’s publications, including how children learn; how science learning and instruction can be transformed; and the performance expectations for science learning articulated in the Next Generation of Science Standards. Results show significant positive increases in student academic, social, and emotional learning in both elementary and secondary school. These findings can be traced, in part, to carefully crafted experiential participatory activities and high-quality instructional materials which act as strong facilitators for knowledge acquisition and use. Reviewing the innovations undertaken by these two interventions, this article describes the importance of studying social and emotional factors ‘in situ’, using the Experience Sampling Method (ESM), that can motivate and engage students in science learning in both elementary and secondary school. Using these ‘in situ’ data collection (N = 596 students in 3rd and N = 1412 students in 10th, 11th, and 12th grades) along with case studies and repeated measures analysis gave deep insights into emotional and social development for young children and adolescents. These methods should continue to be considered when trying to understand key factors of improving engagement in science.

engagement social and emotional learning science learning interventions project-based learning National Science Foundation10.13039/501100008982

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      National and international assessments indicate that US students’ academic performance in science is barely reaching average scores, especially in junior and senior high school (National Assessment of Educational Progress, 2021; Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2020). More disconcerting is that among certain segments of the US student population performance scores continue to lag behind most students in the general population. Additionally, the stagnant or marginal declining scores of 4th graders on NAEP in 2019 and no changes in the scores of 12th graders affirms the view of researchers, business community, and public stakeholders that US students are unprepared to meet the technological changes of today and likely to have difficulty finding stable employment as adults (see Hammerstein et al., 2021).

      These less than promising science achievement test results were evident before the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic. The latest projections, especially among those with the most limited economic and social resources, is that these students are likely to experience major academic, social, and emotional problems at school this coming year and perhaps throughout their careers and beyond (Dorn et al., 2020). The pandemic has raised multiple questions about the long-term effects on student lives and their resilience, having experienced an unprecedented global health crisis. One possible solution for ameliorating these long-term effects is implementing new promising interventions with innovative instructional strategies and materials which show results of increasing science achievement as well as the social and emotional needs of children and adolescents.

      For the past several years, two science curriculum interventions have been implemented and evaluated in elementary and secondary schools (Schneider et al., 2022; Krajcik et al., in press). These two grade level interventions share a theoretical design-based rationale, based on project-based learning (PBL) principles, and provide solutions to several serious questions that have been raised about the quality of science instruction in the US. The elementary school intervention, Multiple Literacies in Project-based Learning (ML-PBL), is an efficacy study of 3rd graders in Michigan where students were given four science units in which learning goals were developed consistent with the Next Generation of Science Standards and the instructional experiences were based on the components of three-dimensional learning (disciplinary core ideas, science and engineering practices, and cross-cutting concepts; National Research Council, 2012; Krajcik et al., 2021). The high school study, Crafting Engaging Science Environments (CESE), developed three units in chemistry and three in physics, and was similarly created using the PBL principles, the NGSS performance expectations, and the National Research Council’s definition of three-dimensional learning (Schneider et al., 2022). These units, at the elementary and secondary level, were all designed with experiences to promote students asking questions, collaborating with one another, constructing evidence and artifacts, and engaging in scientific and engineering practices.

      One key, new addition found in the ML-PBL and CESE interventions was the explicit importance placed on social and emotional learning and its relationship to science achievement (see chapter by Krajcik and Schneider, 2021 for ML-PBL; see Schneider et al., 2020 for CESE). PBL has implicitly emphasized social and emotional learning with its activities, materials, and assessments that have been deliberately designed to create equitable environments (Miller and Krajcik, 2019). In PBL classrooms all students are encouraged and supported to participate in asking questions, collaborate and work in teams, and share personal science experiences both in- and out-of-the classroom. However, in these two interventions, these ideas were further articulated theoretically and applied with specific methods and items developed to measure the impact of social and emotional factors on science achievement at the elementary and secondary levels. At the elementary level, students were asked questions about their interest, skills, and challenge in specific science activities (Bartz et al., 2022) and these same measures were asked to the secondary students with age-appropriate language (Schneider et al., 2016; Bradford and Bartz, 2022). Additionally, during teacher professional learning sessions, special activities were designed to guide teachers in fostering greater participation and inclusivity among all students (Krajcik and Schneider, 2021; Schneider et al., 2022).

      Several considerations in the design of the interventions were identified for understanding social and emotional learning for both elementary and secondary students. First, and most importantly, was the selection of social and emotional constructs that were appropriate for science learning in classrooms (Baines et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2019). Care was taken not to include the entire corpus of psychological measures of emotionality but rather concepts that could be observed (i.e., self-reflection, ownership, and collaboration) and assessed during science lessons. Second, because the focus was on promoting engagement in science learning, fundamental concepts identified in earlier studies of engagement were used to measure interest, skills, and challenge when involved in learning activities (Salmela-Aro et al., 2016; Schneider et al., 2016; Moeller et al., 2017).

      For purposes of measurement at the elementary level, social and emotional learning states (i.e., patterns of feelings during activities within specific time periods) were assessed when students were in their science classes. This was a one-time measure, validated through a variety of statistical procedures (see, Krajcik et al., 2021). At the high school level, these constructs were measured ‘in situ’ when students who were participating in PBL experiences multiple times during the semester, were randomly notified and asked to answer a survey on their emotionality with the Experience Sampling Method (ESM). The ESM is a type of time diary which uses repeated measures randomly obtained through an intermediate notification system (such as on a phone). The results from these two important additions to the PBL design showed significant positive impacts on science learning, motivation, and engagement. This chapter describes why social and emotional learning is an essential component for academic learning, how we incorporated them in these two different efficacy studies, and how we plan to evaluate their impact on science learning.

      Why we need to care about social and emotional learning

      More recently, there has been increased attention within the psychological community to investigate the relationship between the impact of social and emotional learning on student performance in classrooms. Previously, these issues were rarely isolated to the learning context or used to direct teachers’ practices in their classrooms for the purpose of supporting all students’ academic performance and well-being (Durlak et al., 2015; Jagers et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2019). This increased interest in contextualized social and emotional learning support has now been expanded in multiple frameworks to include sensitivity to differences in students’ cultures, equity practices to encourage student participation in classroom experiences, and opportunities for enhanced collaborative and team activities [more specifically as discussed in Lee et al., 2019 and National Academies of Sciences, 2021].

      The intentionality of inclusionary social and emotional learning opportunities in classrooms complements PBL principles (Peele-Eady and Moje, 2020; Rosado-May et al., 2020) and the execution in the design of ML-PBL and CESE interventions. One of the most critical aspects of PBL is beginning with a “driving question,” a real-world problem, where students are encouraged and supported to ask meaningful questions that personalize the lesson to their own lives. Based on the driving question, subsequent experiences are enacted whereby students work together finding solutions to these problems over the course of a unit. The significance of the driving question is critical for motivating interest from the perspective of the students’ lives, shaped by their familial and community economic, social, and cultural resources, and forging them on a path of personalized scientific inquiry and discovery. One cannot overlook the fundamental value of beginning science lessons from the standpoint of appealing to the personal interest of the students for “why” pursuing a recognizable puzzling phenomenon in their natural world may have importance to them (Renninger and Hidi, 2020). Results show that personalized meaningful interest in a topic motivates sustained interest in other science learning experiences providing that they are reasonable for the students’ skill sets and are challenging solvable problems. By incorporating these ideas, students are more likely to persist and learn phenomena they may have previously considered unsolvable (Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider, 2000).

      The ML-PBL and CESE interventions included carefully crafted lessons which are planned with a series of intra- and inter-connected experiences which coherently increase in scientific knowledge and practices (Fortus and Krajcik, 2012). Lessons are constructed so students complete them with their classmates or individually extend their competencies in planning investigations, building models, and writing scientific explanations, all of which offer support for learning how to formulate evidential claims to problems. Activities typically focus on “hands on” experiences, most often in groups, bringing together students of initially varying ability to have the opportunity to acquire actual scientific skills. These learning experiences are quite different from traditional science instruction which tends to rely on science content that students have to memorize, frequently measured individually with summative tests, and which frustrates many students contributing to the loss of interest in science (National Research Council, 2012; National Academies of Sciences, 2021). Rather, these sequential learning experiences are designed to challenge students to work on problems to which they do not know the answer and to encourage them to continue trying to solve them. These activities, which push students to seek the answer to challenging questions, while doing something important to them, have been shown to be related to feelings of determination (Bradford and Bartz, 2022).

      Concentrating on several of the most important social and emotional learning measures, these two interventions also underscored the importance of obtaining such information on these constructs when students are in their science classes. This led in both intervention studies to several assumptions regarding social and emotional measurement: (1) SEL is not a distinctive single psychological state, one can be engaged and feel successful and in control while also feeling a sense of stress; (2) SEL is time variant, in that a confluence of SEL states vary in intensity across the course of one’s daily life experiences; and (3) SEL is highly susceptible to contextual environmental conditions such as the instructional activities in the classroom.

      Recognizing developmental differences in literacy, social and emotional awareness of self and others, and technological skills (Lerner and Steinberg, 2009), the selection of SEL measures and the methodology used in the elementary and secondary intervention studies varied in form, rapidity, and replication. However, what they shared is an overlap of SEL states that examined interest, sense of self-appraisal of one’s involvement in specific activities, value of one’s accomplishments, and collaboration with one another. The elementary design was to measure SEL during their science classes. The secondary school study examined moment to moment ‘in situ’ experiences of when students were both within and outside their science classes which allowed for the measuring of variations in engagement, its construct validity, and its relationship to academic performance.

      Study 1

      Beginning as a design-based study for 3rd grade, the ML-PBL intervention underwent several rounds of revisions and testing over the course of 4 years, including teacher experiments, classroom pilots, a field-test, and most recently an efficacy study to determine whether the ML-PBL intervention enhanced students’ science academic, social, and emotional learning. A randomized control trial was conducted in 46 Michigan schools (23 treatment and 23 control) which included four regions in the state. The final analytic sampled included a total of 2,371 students. The treatment condition included curriculum materials and professional learning experiences for teachers. To assess if there was a significant difference in academic science learning, a three-level hierarchical linear model (HLM) was conducted. This method was used to account for nesting of students within classrooms within schools. Results showed that the treatment students outperformed the control students by a .277 standard deviation on an objective summative test which is a substantial treatment effect (Krajcik et al., in press). This could be interpreted as a ten-point increase on a hundred-point scale or based upon a chosen percentile ranking in which the treatment could move the student from below proficient to proficient (Kraft, 2020).

      The above work also investigated specific research questions related to social and emotional learning, specifically, whether the treatment support more positive responses on measures in self-reflection, collaboration, and responsibility for their own and others’ work. It is important to underscore that few studies measure elementary school students social and emotional learning in their science classes (National Research Council, 1999). Given these constraints, the team consulted relevant limited science studies of young children and more broadly: psychological research studies on SEL; developmentally appropriate questions for 3rd graders; and items from other national assessments (e.g., the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study [ECLS-K] 2016; Durlak et al., 2015; Baines et al., 2017; Jagers et al., 2018). Recognizing differences in literacy skills among students, a drawn thumbs-up (agree), thumbs-down (disagree), and closed fist (neutral) were used to measure agreement. Students circled their feelings on a paper/pencil form administered in spring semester. Prior to the efficacy study, the SEL instrument was designed, field-tested, and revised. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were performed and supported three key latent constructs: self-reflection, ownership, and collaboration (see Krajcik et al., 2021). Additionally, the reliabilities of these constructs were estimated to be 0.78 for self-reflection, 0.81 for ownership, and 0.74 for collaboration. Results from the efficacy study of the ML-PBL intervention showed that the treatment students were estimated to have 0.544 higher factor scores in reflection, 0.434 higher factor scores in ownership, and 0.416 higher factor scores in collaboration than the control students (Krajcik et al., 2021). These results indicate that it is possible to obtain validated measures of young children’s SEL responses for selected constructs. And in this instance, constructs that are specifically designed to be contextually relevant for particular SELs that the intervention was expected to impact.

      As mentioned above, few studies have been able to examine the impact of engagement on elementary science learning. We chose to further examine the relationship between engagement and achievement as research has shown positive relationships between students’ determination to be engaged in the classroom and science achievement (Grabau and Ma, 2017). How students are feeling at the time of the lesson or activity can play a major role in how well they learn or understand key concepts. To explore student responses to project-based and three-dimensional learning, we developed optimal learning surveys that allowed us to measure student engagement in a repeated measures design. These surveys obtained student responses ‘in situ’ within the science classroom, capturing students’ perspectives on specific lessons as they happen (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson, 2014). Results of this new development study are described below.

      During the beginning and first year of the Covid-19 pandemic, the ML-PBL team were able to observe students in their science classes via video, in-person, or a mixture of the two to collect data on the students’ engagement and teacher implementation. One of major observations from the videos was the variation of students’ engagement in their science lessons. Having identified in earlier studies of secondary students a set of constructs (i.e., interest, skill, and challenge) that showed increases in engagement and impacted science learning (see Schneider et al., 2016, 2020), the research team decided to pilot whether these same engagement constructs could be found in elementary science classes and whether they might also positively influence students’ science learning.

      Research questions

      The research questions for this new development study include:

      Can 3rd graders reliably produce measures of interest, skill, and challenge ‘in situ’?

      When studied with repeated measures, do interest, skill, and challenge load onto a single construct of engagement?

      Method

      Using the same constructs of interest, skill, and challenge as fundamental dimensions of engagement, during the pandemic, the team developed a new methodology and series of items for 3rd graders that relied on data collected situated in specific lessons within each unit. Keeping with the idea of measuring social and emotional learning ‘in situ,’ specific items were contextualized to be consistent with the lesson learning goals and how teachers may have been adapting them in the four units (see Bartz et al., 2022).

      Instruments/measures

      For each unit during three different time periods, students were asked questions pertaining to specific measures of interest, skill, and challenge (see page 7 for fuller description). The three different time points were chosen based on the goals of each lesson, allowing us to collect more data from lessons that focused specifically on driving questions, investigation, building a model, or creating a final artifact. These items are situated directly in the context of each lesson. Six focal lessons, which contained the following features: driving question, modeling, investigation, and development of a final artifact, were sampled. For example, in the beginning of the toy unit after observing a toy rocket and how it moves, students were asked for interest, “I like asking questions about how the air rocket moves;” for skills, “I can ask questions about how toy rockets move the way they do;” and for challenge, “I had to think a lot to ask new questions about how rockets move.” With respect to collaboration, the students were asked, “When I worked with my classmates, we came up with different questions about the way the toy rocket moved;” and for ownership, “The questions I asked about the air rocket’s motion were important to me and my classmates.”

      The data collection procedures used for measuring this engagement measure followed the original collection of the SEL survey, but with greater frequency. Teacher administered the four-question OLM survey to third grade students immediately following the lesson. The first three questions were based on engagement: interest, skill, and challenge. The fourth varied by form (A, B, or C) and rotated between collaboration, persistence, agency, time and outcome by lesson. A 4-point Likert scale was used (strongly disagree, disagree, agree, strongly agree) with students circling icons of thumbs up and thumbs down. In the pilot of the SEL measures at the elementary school level, at three different times during each of the four units, the teachers hand out paper copies of the engagement questions to the students in their class. The teachers then read aloud each of the questions, one at a time. After each question is read, students circle the corresponding thumb icon on their paper. In the cases where students circled more than one response, in the median score of responses was recorded.

      Sample

      The sample for this analysis came from 25 3rd grade classrooms in Michigan and included 596 students with a total of 3,369 responses for an average of 6 repeated measures per student.

      Analysis

      Their responses to the engagement questions across the four ML-PBL units were analyzed. For the reliability of this survey, a Cronbach’s alpha was used to estimate the reliability.

      alpha = K K + 1 1 V i V t .

      For understanding whether the interest, skill, and challenge loaded onto a construct of engagement, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted. Factor loadings for each item onto this construct were estimated.

      Results

      The descriptive statistics from the survey, including the items of interest, skill, challenge, and an additional question, are reported in Table 1.

      Sample descriptives.

      N Mean St. Dev Min Max
      Interest 3,369 3.29 0.87 0 4
      Skill 3,330 3.25 0.84 0 4
      Challenge 3,367 2.79 1.08 0 4
      Q4 3,362 3.18 1.01 0 4

      A confirmatory factor analysis confirmed a unidimensional model with the following factor loadings for: interest (0.77); skill (0.41); and challenge (0.26). The overall reliability of the engagement measure is a Cronbach’s Alpha of 0.53. The overall reliability and item level reliabilities are reported in Table 2.

      Reliability using Cronbach’s Alpha.

      Item-test Item-rest Avg. interitem cov Alpha
      Interest 0.67 0.39 0.17 0.4
      Skill 0.62 0.34 0.2 0.44
      Challenge 0.61 0.21 0.25 0.56
      Q4 0.69 0.35 0.17 0.42
      Test scale 0.2 0.53

      Additional analyses are being undertaken to study variation in engagement by lesson activities and individual level variables.

      Study 2

      The secondary school intervention, “Crafting Engaging Science Environments,” (CESE) is a high school chemistry and physics PBL intervention similar to but independent of the elementary intervention. Both interventions meet the NGSS performance expectations and incorporate NRC three-dimensional learning and principles of PBL. CESE was administered to a diverse group of over 4,238 students in chemistry and physics classes in 70 high schools. The design like the elementary study was an efficacy study that involved a randomized control trial in California and Michigan. This intervention also included curriculum materials and professional learning for the teachers. Results were estimated using a two-level HLM with the outcome being the student level performance on the physical science items from the Michigan State Science Assessment and the main predictor of interest being treatment at the school level. For this estimation, a pretest and student demographics were included as covariates. Results show that treatment students, on average, performed 0.20 standard deviations higher than control students on an independently developed summative science assessment (Schneider et al., 2022). These results, like the ML-PBL, are quite large especially considering the advanced subject matter of the units and that they only extended over a 12-to-16-week period. Mediation analyses show an indirect path between teacher- and student-reported participation in modeling practices and science achievement. Exploratory analyses, using a two-level mixed logit model also indicate positive treatment effects for enhancing college ambitions. Overall, results show that improving secondary school science learning is achievable with a coherent system comprising teacher and student learning experiences, professional learning, and formative unit assessments that support students in “doing” science.

      A major part of the study was investigating why secondary students, as shown in national and international studies fail to be engaged in their science classes which likely affects their interest in science learning, achievement, and science career ambitions (National Assessment of Educational Progress, 2021; Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2020). This question of how to enhance engagement in science was a major concern of the secondary school science study. Several major hypotheses about studying engagement were assumed at the onset of the study as discussed above that students’ social and emotional experiences at school are fluid throughout their daily lives (Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider, 2000). First, as discussed above students’ social and emotional experiences at school are fluid throughout their daily lives. It is not expected that students would be fully engaged in all their classes full-time any more than it is expected that adults would be consistently fully engaged in all activities at work or at home. Moreover, because of what is known about adolescent development, trying to create activities that keep teenagers fully engaged requires quite a high bar of motivation (Immordino-Yang, 2015). Irrespective of the barriers and challenges, the problem to be addressed in this study was creating environments that were engaging. The nature of science requires inquiry-based discovery (National Research Council, 2012; National Academis of Sciences, 2018); therefore, students may be more receptive to doing science than memorizing facts or plugging in equations.

      The PBL framework, which stresses solving personally meaningful questions and encouraging instructional activities that require collaboration and are intellectually challenging, was ideally suited to test the constructs of engagement and their impact on academic science achievement. The work is situated in the work of Fredricks and McClolskey (2012) that identifies engagement as having cognitive, behavioral, and subjective components. Extending their definition, the new conception of engagement begins by identifying special behavioral activities that are temporal in quality, spark personalized interest, require competence of a set of knowledge and experiential science practices, and undertake challenging problems.

      In contrast to those who have conceptualize engagement as a general trend, this model of engagement identifies engagement as domain specific in duration and in intensity, which fits more closely with current definitions of situational interest in science learning (see Lavonen et al., 2005; Krapp and Prenzel, 2011). This situational approach is different from other scholars who are interested in identifying universal traits (Deaux and La France, 1998; Cuddy et al., 2008). These engagement experiences are defined as optimal learning moments, which also builds upon the idea of “flow” defined by Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi (1988) as situation specific instances when an individual is so deeply involved in a specific task-related activity that time flies by (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Hektner et al., 2007).

      The PBL curriculum, as discussed above, begins with a driving question when students are in specific situations and faced with a problem or phenomenon that is relevant and meaningful to their lives, such as: “how can I build a safer car?” To build that car, students need to have the necessary knowledge and skills to create a solution. Irrespective of the students’ skill level, finding a reasonable solution should be a challenge, one that sparks determination. When students are fully engaged in a learning task, this is defined as an optimal learning moment (OLM). These moments do not just happen, but need to be artfully constructed and coherent, which is yet another fundamental aspect of PBL which inspires the acquisition of new knowledge, the use of imagination, and stretching problem-solving abilities.

      Optimal learning moments can be verified and understood by other related subjective experiences occurring at nearly the same time. For example, it is expected that when involved in these activities’ students feel successful, confident, active, happy, and enjoyment with the activity (Shernoff et al., 2003; Shumow and Schmidt, 2014). Learning accelerants are those experiences of feeling anxious or stressed, which activate learning (Csikszentmihalyi and Schneider, 2000). Finally, the contrast to positive subjective experiences, termed learning detractors, is when students involved in an activity feel confused or bored and are therefore less likely to be actively engaged or experience an OLM (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Schneider et al., 2016).

      During the field test of the CESE intervention, an ‘in situ’ study of social and emotional relationships to science achievement was conducted with the ESM. The data included 8,273 responses from 244 students in 15 classes taught by 14 teachers in Michigan. Only half of the variance in determination and giving up were at the student level, meaning that both feelings are not altogether stable student traits and most importantly environment and context matters (Schneider et al., 2020). Students were more likely to report giving up when tasks became more challenging, but at the same time, when classroom activities were reported as more challenging than average, students were more likely to persevere, suggesting that determination is partially situationally dependent and shaped by what is occurring in the types of activities presently involved in either with others or oneself.

      While these ESM results were promising, there were several limitations. This was a pilot not a randomized trial where students in a treatment and control group could be compared. Rather it was the case that measures of engagement and feelings regarding challenge were measured using a single case design, where each classroom acts as its own experimental control (vacillating from treatment periods to times in the classroom when it was “business as usual”). These repeated periods were assessed to determine if the treatment influenced students’ engagement. Although, the pilot study results showed that more engaged students had higher grades it could not be directly attributable the CESE intervention. However, the positive nature of the results prompted the team to use the ESM in the future efficacy study (2018–2019) in selected treatment and control classrooms (Schneider et al., 2020).

      Preliminary results on the measures of engagement show that when considering levels of interest skill, and challenge, student engagement levels increase and are accompanied by other positive social and emotional affects, as well as decline in feeling of boredom and confusion. These findings show that concepts such as engagement, creativity, and problem-solving are situationally specific and share nearly equal variance when contrasted with person-level characteristics. In other words, even if a student is not interested in a topic or whose previous science achievement scores are below average, a carefully created situation can alter their negative predilections toward science, bringing considerable strength to the “nurture” side of learning especially when breaking from traditional types of assessment memorization and instead using imagination, problem-solving, and taking different points of view into consideration when engaged in scientific practices. However, these are preliminary results and an important question is the level of challenge and what impact it has on motivating higher engagement and learning for all students in specific contexts. (see, Schneider et al., 2020, for a deeper discussion of these ideas and how they were conceived and measured in the earlier field study).

      Current study

      Most recently, a deeper examination of “challenge experiences” in science class has been conducted (Bradford and Bartz, 2022; Chen et al., 2022). Until now, challenge has not been a major state in the psychological literature, and less attention has been placed on perceived “challenge” experiences in science classroom environments. Challenge experience can be highly motivating and encourage deeper engagement in a classroom task. However, there is less research regarding the importance of perceived challenge for high schoolers, how it varies ‘in situ’, and how students react to challenging experiences. To fill the gap, this study has two different analyses.

      The first starts with a case study approach to illustrate a particular pattern of perceived challenge by visualizing three student cases in 4 days of their school life. The visualization focuses on a precise moment in time and provides corresponding details on where students were, what they were doing, and who they were with. From there, the graphic visualization considers students’ school life for 4 days and how this pattern of perceived challenge experiences is general or unique to individuals who vary in their background science knowledge. After visualizing three students’ life in school, we use another graphic layout to visualize students’ reactions to perceived challenges in their positive and negative states. The purpose of this visualization work is to lead the researchers to discover patterns of emotionality shared by several members of the student sample for 4 days.

      The second analysis uses data from a sample of students from the field test and efficacy study to understand the use of ESM and student’s variation in emotions across years. These analyses employ a series of repeated measures estimation of students situational perceived challenge, stress, anxiety, determination, giving up, and confusion to understand how the relationship between challenge and giving up and confusion is mitigated by stress and anxiety. We assume that challenge is important in driving learning; however, if challenge is correlated to giving up and confusion, this would lead to a negative relationship between challenge and learning. This leads to a question of whether anxiety and stress may be stronger mediating factors in the relationship between challenge and giving up and confusion.

      Research questions

      The research questions for study 2 were:

      How does perceived challenge vary by individual students?

      How does the relationship between perceived challenge and positive and negative emotions vary by individual students?

      What is the relationship between students’ perceived challenge coupled with stress and anxiety and determination, giving up, and confusion?

      Sample

      During the field test of the CESE (2013–2018), a total of 867 students were reported with the ESM. For the efficacy study (2018–19), a total 545 students were reported with the ESM for a total of 1,412 students combined. The phones were programmed to alert the students randomly 6–8 times per day (at least 3–4 times when they had science lessons) over an assigned period. An initial ESM prompt would occur in the beginning, mid- and late point of a study session automatically set up by researchers using the PACO app. Students were asked to respond to an identical questionnaire (nearly 30 items) within a 15 min window. Two reminders would occur 10 and 15 min after the initial prompt. On average, it takes about 90 s to complete items. Each day all participants received eight to 10 beeps on their smartphones which gave them 40 total response opportunities during a study period. We preprogrammed the beep schedule randomly and guarantee a minimum of 1–3 beeps occurring in science classes, resulting in 5 to 15 beeps per person in this study. In total, the data comprised 3,234 responses. The average valid beeps per student is 6 in science classes. We conducted two separate analyses, one which only analyzed the students in the efficacy study and a second analysis from both the field and efficacy studies.

      The first analyses reported is from the efficacy study which contained a diverse population of students living in both Michigan and California with an overrepresentation of students for whom English is not their first language, as one of our sites was a mile from the Mexican border. Among the efficacy students’ sample, 315 (58%) had valid student background information, including Race/Ethnicity, gender, challenge experiences and science pretest scores. This student background survey was collected at the beginning of the year via a Qualtrics Survey. Table 3 reports the descriptive statistics for the students. Of the 315 students who provided valid ESM responses, the racial composition of the groups was 60% White, 8% Black, 17% Hispanic, 5% Asian and 5% multi-racial.

      Descriptive statistics of student sample.

      Freq. %
      Male 151 48.55
      Female 160 51.45
      Grade 10 81 31.89
      Grade 11 146 57.48
      Grade 12 27 10.63
      White, (non-hispanic) 200 60.4
      Hispanic 57 17.2
      Black 28 8.5
      Asian 18 5.4
      Other 11 3.3
      Multiracial 17 5.1
      Total valid student info 331
      Demographic info Missing 214
      Mean SD
      Percentile ranking of pre-test 62.69 22.03
      Challenge 2.31 0.74

      The second analysis used the entirety of the sample from both the field and efficacy tests (demographic information is unavailable for this combined sample; however, the sampling scheme for the field and efficacy tests targeted schools with significant numbers of low-income and minority students). The entire sample was used in the second analysis which uses aggregate statistical modeling to understand the validity of these relationships across many years (2013–2019).

      Methods Instruments and measures

      To measure engagement, studies typically employ surveys which are rarely conducted ‘in situ’ or when they are happening, which of course fails to capture how students are feeling from one moment to the next. Measuring how students feel across moments allows us to identify when they feel successful at what they are doing and its relationship to what they are learning. The ESM records what students are doing, what they are thinking about, and what they report feeling in the moment forming an archival repository of daily experiences. This focus on the situational and contextual aspects of what happens in-and -out-of-the classroom lessens the opportunity for recall bias and socially desirable answers and has been validated in previous studies (Hektner et al., 2007). The ESM SEL survey items and their response are reported in Table 4. There were approximately 30 items.

      CESE ESM instrument.

      ESM questions in CESE
      Q1 Where were you when you were signaled?
      Q2 What science class were you in?
      Q3 Which best describes what you were doing in science when signaled?
      Q4 What were you doing when signaled?
      Q5 What were you learning about in science when signaled?
      Q6 Who were you with?
      Q7 Were you doing the main activity because you…
      Q8 Was what you were doing…
      Q9 Were you interested in what you were doing?
      Q10 Did you feel skilled at what you were doing?
      Q11 Did you feel challenged by what you were doing?
      Q12 Did you feel like giving up?
      Q13 How much were you concentrating?
      Q14 Do you enjoy what you are doing?
      Q15 Did you feel like you were in control of what you were doing?
      Q16 Were you succeeding?
      Q17 Was this activity important for you?
      Q18 How important is this activity in relation to your future goals/plans?
      Q19 Were you living up to the expectations of others?
      Q20 Were you living up to your expectations?
      Q21 I was so absorbed in what I was doing that time flew.
      Q22 How determined were you to accomplish the task?
      Q23 Were you feeling…Happy
      Q24 Were you feeling… Excited
      Q25 Were you feeling… Anxious
      Q26 Were you feeling… Competitive
      Q27 Were you feeling… Lonely
      Q28 Were you feeling… Stressed
      Q29 Were you feeling… Proud
      Q30 Were you feeling… Cooperative
      Q31 Were you feeling… Bored
      Q32 Were you feeling… Self-confident
      Q33 Were you feeling… Confused
      Q34 Were you feeling… Active

      In both analyses, students were beeped several times a day (7 times) during a week both inside and out of school and classes, with several more signals in science classes. Each classroom was randomly chosen for a specific week(s) during the intervention for data collection. Each data entry has a time stamp to indicate when the responses was collected. This approach is different from single survey as it records a set of repeated specific social and emotional measures interacting with specific activities, such as, doing a hands-on experiment in science class as compared to playing a video game. These responses are uploaded to a secured server which sends information to a cloud and are then quickly transformed into clean datasets and ready for analysis. Confidentiality is maintained by student anonymized identification numbers (It is important to note that all of our data collection and analyses underwent Institutional review board approval and received exempt status).

      Figure 1 below shows a screen shot of one of the questions used in the secondary school intervention in both the field test and efficacy study. The actual software program was developed by Robert Evans, a google engineer, who named the program Paco,1 after his dog which barks to let students know it is time to answer the questions. Although there are multiple questions, the students can move through them quickly. Since they are programmed for smartphones, beeping schedules can be easily programmed over the course of days of a week or multiple weeks during specific time periods. Figure 1 shows the questions asked regarding interest, skill, and challenge.

      Screen shot of PACO app (Evans, 2016). Republished with permission.

      Analysis

      A more in-depth examination of ESM is shown in the case study analysis. A second analysis which relies on a hierarchical linear model (HLM) was used for the aggregate study of the field and efficacy tests. Beginning with the in-depth case study, three students were selected among varying levels of school achievement to analyze their variability in emotionality with graphic visualization. First a description is given for how an individual student experienced challenges across activities, locations, and companionship in their 4 days. Second, the three students’ emotional responses within each person’s positive and negative states when challenged is shown in Figures 24. Finally in the second analysis, we explore the relationship of challenge on spurring continued determination or on confusion and giving up with or without changes in other states of emotionality through the HLM.

      Dennis (Low-performing student) Experienced “Challenge” in situ across Context. Report Z-score Over 4 days. Pink color marks the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color marks the moments when a student is out of the school.

      Megan (Average student) Experienced “Challenge” in situ across Context. Report Z-score Over 4 days. Pink color marks the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color marks the moments when a student is out of the school.

      Collins (Above Average student) Experienced “Challenge” in situ across Context. Report Z-score Over 4 days. Pink color marks the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color marks the moments when a student is out of the school.

      Three case studies

      To understand the situational and individual differences for the students in the case study, their ESM responses were obtained throughout the day, including an oversample of beeps in their science classes (Chen et al., 2022). Table 5 shows the three students’ background, the level of prior test scores, and the average perceived challenge across all contexts and in the science classroom only. To see how this visualization works, consider the rating of perceived challenge by those three students. The three students are: Dennis, a low-academic performing student based on his prior test scores; Megan, an academically average student; and Collins, an above average on his prior test scores. During the 4 days of the study, Dennis has an average challenge response of 3.14 across all contexts and the average challenge response of 3.25 in the science classroom. Megan has an average challenge response of 2.51 across all contexts and the average challenge response of 2.25 in her science classroom. Collins has an average challenge response of 2.29 across all contexts and the average challenge response of 2.30 in his science classroom. These three students are in the same science class at their school. Examining these individual case studies allows for the comparisons among the three students, their different social and emotional experiences throughout the day, and their relationship to challenge in different contexts.

      Three student case study.

      School performance Gender Average Perceived challenge (individual) all context Average Perceived challenge (individual) in science classroom n of moment
      Dennis Low-performing student Male 3.14 3.25 22
      Megan Average performance Female 2.51 2.25 20
      Collins Above average Male 2.29 2.3 24

      A standardized z-score (mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1) of perceived challenge was calculated and took into account individual differences while also allowing for comparison across individuals on a common scale. The z-scored perceived challenge also provides an advantage for exploring the emotional response in different contexts. The students’ z-scores of challenge are compared across different settings and activities in these case studies. Additionally, the students’ positive emotional states, which are measured by “happy, enjoy, excited, success and competitive,” and negative emotional states, which are measured by “angry, stress, confused, give-up and anxious,” are compared across different levels of the students’ challenge levels using a different visualization. An average score of five emotional responses was used to represent the positive and negative states for the three cases. The five positive and negative emotional states were chosen based on earlier work (Hektner et al., 2007, pp: 110–123). These analyses are depicted through graphs to illustrate these varying states of challenge with the students’ other positive and negative emotional states. These analyses give insight at the individual level; however, to understand aggregate relationships, we move to statistical models with the entire sample of ESM students from the field test and efficacy study.

      From case studies to a statistical model

      The ESM asks students questions that correlate perceived challenge experiences that may confound the relationship with other positive and negative psychological states. Therefore, it is important to understand the influence confounding variables may have on students perceived social and emotional well-being. More specifically, in the case of perceived challenge, this new work has begun to examine the confounding effects of stress and anxiety on the relationship between challenge and two important negative psychological states, confusion and giving up, and one positive state of determination (Bradford and Bartz, 2022). First, the correlations for the variables were calculated to understand the relationship between challenge, stress, anxiety, confusion, giving up and determination.

      Then, using a repeated measures HLM, the relationship between challenge and confusion, giving up, and determination was explored first without covariates and then with stress and anxiety as covariates. The following two equations were estimated.

      Model 1:

      outcom e i j = δ 00 + δ 10 challenge + ν 0 j + ϵ i j

      Model 2:

      outcom e i j = δ 00 + δ 10 challenge + δ 20 stress + δ 30 anxiety + ν 0 j + ϵ i j

      Where δ10 is the relationship between challenge and the outcome in the two models, v_0j is the student level random intercept and epsilon_ij is the beep level error term. The δ 10 from both models 1 and 2 were compared using the Hausmann test to determine if the inclusion of stress and anxiety significantly changed the relationship between challenge and the outcome.

      Results Three case studies

      In Figure 2, we plot the z-score of Dennis’ perceived challenge over 22 moments. We also plot the z-score of Megan’ perceived challenge over 20 moments in Figure 3 and the z-score of Collins’ perceived challenge over 24 moments in Figure 4. The three plots center in the middle line of the z-score as 0, which is each individual student’s average challenge score. Dennis has a smaller range of perceived challenge than Megan and Collins, and he favors to report high perceived challenge among 4 days of the study. If we are interested in the context of science classroom, we can compare patterns when the three students are taking quiz in the same context. For example, we use the pink color to mark the moments in science classroom, and the light blue color mark the moments when a student is out of the school. Dennis and Megan perceived higher challenges, particularly when taking a quiz (z-score ranged from 0.5 to 1.0). Collins feels less challenged when taking a quiz but experiences a higher challenge in group discussion or when using a computer. We can conclude that Dennis and Megan objectively have higher perceived challenge than Collins when taking a quiz among the 4 days they were sampled.

      Relative to the science classroom context, the out-of-school context (colored in light blue), Megan and Collins are less challenged especially when compared to Dennis. Overall, these three case studies show that the context of when students feel challenged can vary considerably by individuals and activities.

      Recognizing the individual variability of experiencing challenge across contexts, the next question is whether emotional responses related to challenge differ by student. When challenged, is this experience more positive for Collins and Megan than Dennis, or do they all report similar feelings? Figures 57 show other positive and negative emotional states of these three students as well as their level of challenge during their science classes.

      Dennis (Low-performing student) Experienced Challenge in Relation to Positive and Negative Psychological States. Report raw scores in positive and negative emotions. The dark blue color marks the lowest challenge moments (=1), whereas the light blue color marks the highest challenge moments (=4) in the science classroom.

      Megan (Average student) Experienced Challenge in Relation to Positive and Negative Psychological States. Report raw scores in positive and negative emotions. The dark blue color marks the lowest challenge moments (=1), whereas the light blue color marks the highest challenge moments (=4) in the science classroom.

      Collins (Above Average student) Experienced Challenge in Relation to Positive and Negative Psychological States (Low-performing student). Report raw scores in positive and negative emotions. The dark blue color marks the lowest challenge moments (=1), whereas the light blue color marks the highest challenge moments (=4) in the science classroom.

      Among our three student cases, Dennis had fewer positive psychological states and reported more challenging tasks during his science class. His perception of high challenge (light blue bar) is less correlated with positive psychological states and more correlated with negative ones. Additionally, Dennis’ psychological states fluctuated more than the two other students. These fluctuations are more apparent when experiencing positive psychological states (e.g., feeling successful, confident) than his negative psychological states of confusion.

      Megan, on the other hand, had a declining trend of positive psychological states over the 4 days. When she experienced high challenge tasks in the science classroom, her negative psychological states increased. However, for Collins, the above average student, his positive psychological states were more correlated with higher challenge. Additionally, during moments of rising challenge, Collins experienced other positive emotions. Overall, the relationship between challenge and positive and negative psychological states seem to vary across students and days (Chen et al., 2022).

      Statistical results from the entire sample

      Among all students in the ESM sample, challenge was closely related to negative emotions for some students, while for others, it was closely related to positive emotions, and for others, there was no relationship. However, these results do not indicate how stress and anxiety might be influencing the relationship of challenge with other positive and negative emotions. Instead of focusing on all positive and negative emotions, a few key variables were explored more deeply: confusion, giving up, and determination, which were all positively correlated with challenge as seen in Table 6, which includes the entire sample of students, 1,412 students from the field test and efficacy study.

      Pairwise correlations of challenge, stress, anxiety, giving up, determination, and confusion.

      Stress Anxious Challenge Give up Determined Confused
      Stress 1.000
      Anxious 0.460 1.000
      Challenge 0.310 0.270 1.000
      Give up 0.430 0.310 0.380 1.000
      Determined −0.028 0.093 0.220 −0.099 1.000
      Confused 0.54 0.400 0.420 0.490 −0.022 1.000

      However, importantly, confusion and giving up were also positively related to stress and anxiety, while determination was not. Therefore, the question arose was whether stress and anxiety may be accounting for this positive relationship between challenge and confusion and challenge and giving up.

      From the repeated measures HLM, the positive relationship between challenge and confusion and challenge and giving up significantly decreased in absolute value, when including stress and anxiety as covariates ( δ 0.1 , p value < 0.001 ), while the relationship between challenge and determination remained relatively the same (Bradford and Bartz, 2022).

      Discussion

      The present findings extend previous research in at least two ways: First, these results provide a moment-level look at context differences in response to daily challenges in school, incorporating both the intra-individual as well as the inter-personal level across study one and study two. Both offer insights for each other to complete the puzzle of challenging experiences in students’ daily lives in school. These finding of significant context differences in intra-individual variability of experiencing challenge and other positive and negative states, to some degree, suggest that the relationship between perceived challenge, optimal learning moment, and psychological reactions is complex. Examining these relationships among these different emotions also offers that classroom learning, as we might have expected, is not a simple correlation with a specific experience but needs to be seen in context, over time, and in relationship to other events.

      To further consider individual and contextual factors simultaneously, a designed statistical model like Simultaneous Equation Modeling (SEM) or Dynamic Structural Equation Modeling (DSEM) is essential to move this line of research forward. Second, complementing previous optimal learning moment literature on the states of the flow (Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Schneider et al., 2016), students’ determination could be one psychological state that may keep students working on the challenging tasks in science. The results from adding stress and anxiety as covariates indicates that as one explores individual and contextual differences in their experiences, one should also consider the confounding effects that may occur when these individuals are experiencing many different emotional states at once. Additionally, these results may suggest that stress and anxiety may not be as important of an activation for challenge. These results offer some possibilities for discovering methods to increase students’ optimal learning moments in science.

      Limitations of the study

      With respect to specific limitations of study 1, there are few ‘in situ’ surveys for elementary level students for which we could compare our results. We have plans to use collected videos of classrooms to collaborate our findings which could increase the validity of these instruments. With respect to study 2, there may be other emerging technologies that could capture more changes in emotionality than the ESM, such as combining individual responses with video technology to capture facial, cognitive, and biomarkers, to which we could compare our results.

      Overall, more studies are needed to use these techniques to build a corpus of work so that a comparison across studies can be examined to understand the reliability and validity of these techniques and their results. Despite our limitations of not having more in-depth analyses of personal and environment influences on social and emotional learning, our work provides another lens for understanding how levels of engagement and motivation are related to achievement, especially today when COVID’s effects on these important relationships need further exploration. Additionally, more studies are needed on emotionality in classrooms that take into account student cultures, family histories, race/ethnicity, and gender.

      Implications for raising interest in STEM

      Even though classrooms are busy fluid learning environments, results show it is possible to measure social and emotional experiences, but they vary considerably by context. Some students find certain types of activities more interesting than others, and their skill levels vary meeting similar challenging problems with a diverse set of reactions from boredom and confusion to determination and sense of success and accomplishment. These variations by context indicate that isolating a specific measure of emotionality may overlook the factors at work that could be deterrents to motivation and persistence in STEM. It seems critical that researchers attempting to increase motivation for students to become engaged in learning experiences need to focus on the environment and emotions which operate at the same moments within the same context. And most importantly when considering engagement, recognizing that students vary in their skill levels, and this may be affecting the pursuit of learning new skills and attempting challenging STEM problems.

      The greatest challenge for researchers who wish to transform STEM learning environments is determining the important types of social and emotionality constructs that make the most sense given the subject matter and experiences when students are expected to be engaged. This work has deliberately focused on science classrooms, where the underlying instructional and curricular activities are crafted in accordance with recent reports for transformative pedagogical practices. The toolkit of social and emotional measures being considered are those that seem the most reasonable given the goals of the lessons and the phenomena and problems to be solved. However, in trying to disentangle the behavioral, cognitive, and emotionality of engagement, considering interest, skill, and challenge are imperative, as well as other social and emotional factors that also occur when valuing teamwork and collaboration for having students learn and work with others and reach a place of ownership of ideas and products.

      During the in-depth study of engagement (i.e., interest, skill, and challenge), several new factors related to learning have occurred. Interest, as others have also recognized, is critical; however, it must be constructed around ideas that the students find purposeful to their own lives. Memorizing the elements of the periodic table without knowing the purpose behind understanding the properties of the atom is a non- starter to a student. However, why we need to understand the relationship of certain elements to each other and their impact on chemical reactions experienced in everyday life can become more meaningful to a student.

      Students have different skill levels and when choosing group experiences being attentive to the likely variation in the classroom is indispensable. The importance of bringing everyone into the problem-solving activity and making it a reasonable challenge for all students is likely to affect their personal as well as the groups’ continued work on a project or problem. The idea here is not to construct activities that have the lowest level of skills but rather to offer various flexible routes to problem solving for all the students. Nonetheless, it is the case in PBL that there are certain disciplinary core ideas that are regarded as critical and that has to the starting point of the lessons. What students need is an awareness of their own confidence to face a challenge and how that can fit into the space of figuring out a phenomenon or solving a problem.

      Moving students to learn something they do not know changes the nature of learning from memorization to using ideas. This type of learning poses another set of ideas, in that students are taking on something that they do not know but they could find out. This process exposes their vulnerabilities in of not knowing—for which they need to learn to be more comfortable with. This is particularly problematic for females especially in adolescence, where taking risks and exposing one’s vulnerabilities is typically a positive aspect of the socialization process they encounter (Reniers et al., 2016). What is needed here is to underscore the value in taking intellectual risks in problem solving learning activities and the determination to continue working until a solution is found. Coming out of one’s comfort zone intellectually particularly in science where discovery and new innovations are fundamental must be nurtured not just with content but the social and emotional factors that can inspire motivated students to solve. Understanding these relationships among these different emotions suggest that classroom learning as we might have expected is not a simple one to one correlation with a specific experience but need to be seen in context, over time and in relationship to other events. This underscores the difficulty and limitations of new curricular packages designed to measure and relate emotionality to achievement and certain positive behavioral actions.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Michigan State University IRB. Written informed consent from the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

      Author contributions

      BS: conception and design of work, drafting the article, review of data analysis and interpretation, and critical revision. I-CC: data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article, and critical revisions of the article. LB: data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article, and critical revisions of the article. KB: data analysis and interpretation, drafting the article, and critical revisions of the article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      Funding

      This study is supported by the National Science Foundation (OISE-1545684; PIs Barbara Schneider and Joseph Krajcik); George Lucas Educational Foundation—Lucas Education Research (PI Joseph Krajcik); and the John A. Hannah Chair in the College of Education at Michigan State University. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation and the George Lucas Educational Foundation.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Baines A. DeBarger A. De Vivo K. Warner N. (2017). “Why is social and emotional learning essential to project-based learning?,” LER position paper 2 United States: George Lucas Educational Foundation. Bartz K. Miller C. Bateman K. (2022). Measuring Engagement in 3rd Grade Science Classes. Working Paper. Michigan: Create for STEM. Bradford L. Bartz K. (2022). Intersections of challenge, stress and anxiety: When is the challenge too much? [Conference Presentation]. AERA Annual Conference 2022, San Diego, CA, United States. Chen I. Bradford L. Bartz K. Krajcik J. (2022). Exploring the stability and fluctuation of experiencing “challenges” in the high school science classroom [Conference Presentation]. AERA Annual Conference 2022, San Diego, CA, United States. Csikszentmihalyi M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York: Harper Perennial. Csikszentmihalyi M. Csikszentmihalyi I. (1988). Optimal Experience. New York: Cambridge University Press. Csikszentmihalyi M. Larson R. (2014). “Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method” in Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology. ed. Peele-Eady (Dordrecht: Springer), 3554. Csikszentmihalyi M. Schneider B. (2000). Becoming Adult: How Teenagers Prepare For the World of Work. New York: Basic Books. Cuddy A. J. C. Fiske S. T. Glick P. (2008). “Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: the stereotype content model and the BIAS map,” in Advances in experimental social psychology. ed. Zanna M. P., Vol. 40. (Oxford: Academic Press), 61149. Deaux K. La France M. (1998). “Gender” in Handbook of Social Psychology. eds. Gilert D. T. Fiske S. T. Lindzey G., vol. Vol. 1. 4th Edn. (New York: McGraw-Hill), 788827. Dorn E. Hancock B. Sarakatsannis Viruleg E. (2020). COVID-19 and student learning in the United States: The hurt could last a lifetime. Chicago, Illinois: McKinsey & Company Public sector Practice. Durlak J. A. Domitrovich C. E. Weissberg R. P. Gullotta T. P. (Eds.). (2015). Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research and practice. New York: Guilford Publications. Evans B. (2016). Paco—applying computational methods to scale qualitative methods. EPIC. 2016, 348368. Fortus D. Krajcik J. (2012). Curriculum coherence and learning progressions. Secon International Handbook of Science Education. Berlin: Springer 783798. Fredricks J. McClolskey W. (2012). “The measurement of student engagement: a comparative analysis of various methods and student self-report instruments,” in Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. eds. Christenson S. Reschly A. Wylie C. (MA: Springer), 737. Grabau L. J. Ma X. (2017). Science engagement and science achievement in the context of science instruction: a multilevel analysis of US students and schools. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 39, 10451068. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2017.1313468 Hammerstein S. König C. Dreisörner T. Frey A. (2021). Effects of COVID-19-related school closures on student achievement-a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 12:289. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.746289, PMID: 34603162 Hektner J. Schmidt J. Csikszentmihalyi M. (2007). Experience sampling method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage Publications. Immordino-Yang M. (2015). Emotions, learning and the brain: Exploring the educational implications of affective neuroscience. New York: W.W. Norton. Jagers R. J. Rivas-Drake D. Borowski T., (2018). Equity & social and emotional learning: a cultural analysis. Frameworks Briefs (November). Available at: https://casel.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/04/equity-and-SEL-.pdf Kraft M. (2020). Interpreting effect sizes of education interventions. Educ. Res. 49, 241253. doi: 10.3102/0013189X20912798 Krajcik J. Schneider B. (Eds.) (2021). Science education through multiple literacies: Project-based learning in elementary school. MA: Harvard Education Press. Krajcik J. Schneider B. Miller E. Chen I. Bradford L. Baker Q. (in press). Assessing the effect of the project-based learning on science learning in elementary schools. Am. Educ. Res. J. Krajcik J. Schneider B. Miller E. Chen I. Bradford L. Bartz K. (2021). Assessing the effect of the project-based learning on science learning in elementary schools. Final Technical Report to Lucas Education Research Krapp A. Prenzel M. (2011). Research on interest in science: theories, methods, and findings. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 33, 2750. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2010.518645 Lavonen J. Juuti K. Uitto A. Meisalo V. Byman R. (2005). Attractiveness of science education in the Finnish comprehensive school. Helsinki, Finland: Technology Industries of Finland. Lee C. Nasir N. Pea R. de Royston M. (2019). Introduction Reconceptualizing learning: a critical ask for knowledge building and technology. In Nasir N. S. Lee C. D. Peas R. de Royston M. M. (Eds.) Handbook of the cultural foundations of learning. (United Kingdom: Rutledge), 1735. Lerner R. M. Steinberg L. (Eds.). (2009).Handbook of adolescent Pshycology. (3rd ed.). United States: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Miller E. C. Krajcik J. S. (2019). Promoting deep learning through project-based learning: a design problem. Disciplinary and Interdisciplinary Science Education Research 1, 110. doi: 10.1186/s43031-019-0009-6 Moeller J. Spicer J. Salmela-Aro K. Schneider B. (2017). “Advances in the research on situation-specific and contextual aspects of student engagement,” in Pathways to adulthood, educational opportunities, motivation, and attainment in times of social change. eds. Schoon I. Silbereisen R. K. (London: UCL IOE Press), 119136. National Academies of Sciences. (2021). Science and engineering in preschool through elementary grades: The brilliance of children and strengths of educator’s. United States: National Academies Press. National Academis of Sciences. (2018). How People Learn II: Learners, Context, and Cultures. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. National Assessment of Educational Progress (2021). Results From the 2019 Science Assessment. U.S. Department of Education and Institute of Education Sciences. National Research Council . (1999). How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School. United States: National Academies Press. National Research Council. (2012). A framework for K–12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. United States: National Academies Press. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development . (2020). Science performance (PISA)—Indicator. Available at: https://data.oecd.org/pisa/science-performance-pisa.htm Peele-Eady T. B. Moje E. B. (2020). “Communities as contexts for learning,” in Handbook of the Cultural Foundations of Learning. eds. N. S. Nasir, C. D. Lee, R. Peas and M. M. de Royston (United Kingdom: Routledge), 230246. Reniers R. L. E. P. Murphy L. Lin A. Bartolomé S. P. Wood S. J. (2016). Risk perception and risk-taking behaviour during adolescence: the influence of personality and gender. PLoS One 11:e0153842. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153842, PMID: 27100081 Renninger K. A. Hidi S. E. (2020). To level the playing field, develop interest. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 1018. doi: 10.1177/2372732219864705 Rosado-May F. J. Urrieta L. Jr. Dayton A. Rogoff B. (2020). “Innovation as a key feature of indigenous ways of learning: individuals and communities generating knowledge 1” in Handbook of the cultural foundations of learning. eds N. S. Nasir, C. D. Lee, R. Peas and M. M. de Royston (United Kingdom: Routledge), 7996. Salmela-Aro K. Moeller J. Schneider B. Spicer J. Lavonen J. (2016). Integrating the light and dark sides of student engagement with person-oriented and situation-specific approaches. Learn. Instr. 43, 6170. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.001 Schneider B. Krajcik J. Lavonen J. Salmela-Aro K. (2020). Learning science: The value of crafting engagement in science environments. New Haven: Yale University Press. Also published in Chinese by, Educational Science Publishing House Limited, 2021. Schneider B. Krajcik J. Lavonen J. Salmela-Aro K. Broda M. Spicer J. (2016). Investigating optimal learning moments in U.S. and Finnish science classes. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 53, 400421. doi: 10.1002/tea.21306 Schneider B. Krajcik J. Lavonen J. Salmela-Aro K. Klager C. Bradford L. (2022). Improving science achievement—is it possible? Educ. Res. 51, 109121. doi: 10.3102/0013189X211067742 Shernoff D. Csikszentmihalyi M. Schneider B. Shernoff E. (2003). Student engagement in high school classrooms from the perspective of flow theory. Sch. Psychol. Q. 18, 158176. doi: 10.1521/scpq.18.2.158.21860 Shumow L. Schmidt J. A. (2014). Enhancing Adolescents’ Motivation for Science: Research-Based Strategies for Teaching Male and Female Students. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin, A Sage Company.

      1https://www.pacoapp.com/

      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016fkchain.com.cn
      www.rmchain.com.cn
      www.tinuan.com.cn
      www.okfchjk.org.cn
      www.ubcyub.com.cn
      qeoiof.com.cn
      www.udtw.com.cn
      qyzenw.com.cn
      mkiusi.com.cn
      www.wuxibar.org.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p