Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.790313 Psychology Original Research Skin-to-skin SDF positioning: The key to intersubjective intimacy between mother and very preterm newborn—A pilot matched-pair case-control study Buil Aude 1 2 Sankey Carol 1 Caeymaex Laurence 2 3 Gratier Maya 4 Apter Gisèle 5 Vitte Lisa 1 5 Devouche Emmanuel 1 5 * 1Laboratoire de Psychopathologie et Processus de Santé (LPPS UR 4057), Université Paris Cité, Paris, France 2NICU Service de réanimation néonatale, Hospital Center Intercommunal De Créteil, Créteil, France 3Faculté de santé - Université Paris Est Créteil, Créteil, France 4Université Paris Nanterre, Nanterre, France 5Service de pédopsychiatrie universitaire, Hospital Group Du Havre, Le Havre, France

Edited by: Jonathan T. Delafield-Butt, University of Strathclyde, United Kingdom

Reviewed by: Eystein Våpenstad, Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences, Norway; Mikael Heimann, Linköping University, Sweden

*Correspondence: Emmanuel Devouche, devouche7@gmail.com

This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

11 10 2022 2022 13 790313 06 10 2021 22 08 2022 Copyright © 2022 Buil, Sankey, Caeymaex, Gratier, Apter, Vitte and Devouche. 2022 Buil, Sankey, Caeymaex, Gratier, Apter, Vitte and Devouche

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Background

Skin-to-skin contact (SSC) has been widely studied in NICU and several meta-analyses have looked at its benefits, for both the baby and the parent. However, very few studies have investigated SSC’ benefits for communication, in particular in the very-preterm newborn immediately after birth.

Aims

To investigate the immediate benefits of Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) positioning during SSC on the quality of mother—very-preterm newborn communication and to examine the coordination of the timing of communicative behaviors, just a few days after birth.

Subjects and study design

Monocentric prospective matched-pair case-control study. Thirty-four mothers and their very preterm infants (27 to 31 + 6 weeks GA, mean age at birth: 30 weeks GA) were assigned to one of the two SSC positioning, either the Vertical Control positioning (n = 17) or the SDF Intervention positioning (n = 17). Mother and newborn were filmed during the first 5 min of their first SSC.

Outcome measures

Infants’ states of consciousness according to the Assessment of Preterm Infants’ Behavior scale (APIB). Onset and duration of newborns’ and mothers’ vocalizations and their temporal proximity within a 1-s time-window.

Results

In comparison with the Vertical group, very preterm newborns in the SDF Intervention Group spent less time in a drowsy state and more in deep sleep. At 3.5 days of life, newborns’ vocal production in SSC did not differ significantly between the two groups. Mothers offered a denser vocal envelope in the SDF group than in the Vertical group and their vocalizations were on average significantly longer. Moreover, in a one-second time-frame, temporal proximity of mother-very preterm newborn behaviors was greater in the SDF Intervention Group.

Conclusion

Although conducted on a limited number of dyads, our study shows that SDF positioning fosters mother-very preterm newborn intimate encounter during the very first skin to skin contact after delivery. Our pioneer data sheds light on the way a mother and her very preterm vocally meet, and constitutes a pilot step in the exploration of innate intersubjectivity in the context of very preterm birth.

first skin-to-skin contact, SDF positioning mother-very preterm newborn interaction innate intersubjectivity Kangaroo Care NICU vocal production very premature birth Institut Universitaire de France10.13039/501100004795

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      In the 1970s, scientists brought evidence that, before they actually begin to speak, newborns, as well as young infants, experience intersubjective awareness. Their thoughts develop through the sympathetic and timely engagement with the expressive behaviors of a sensitive parent (Bruner, 1977; Trevarthen, 1977; Bateson, 1979; Brazelton, 1979; Brazelton et al., 1979). The centrality of interpersonal synchrony was highlighted through pioneer studies using videotape recordings of mother–infant interactions (Trevarthen, 1977; Tronick et al., 1977; Feldman et al., 1999; Jaffe et al., 2001). The present study falls within the psychobiological theory of intersubjectivity (Trevarthen, 2012, 2016). The theory of innate intersubjectivity claims that “a child is born with motives to find and use the motives of other persons in ‘conversational’ negotiation of purposes, emotions, experiences, and meaning. The efficiency of sympathetic engagement between persons signals the ability of each to ‘model’ or ‘mirror’ the motivations and purposes of companions, immediately” [Trevarthen, 1998 (in Braten) p. 16]. In this perspective, achieving a conversation at birth rests on an efficient mobilization of the neonate’s internal resources (self-regulation), as well as on the efficient sympathetic regulation of a partner. The theory of innate intersubjectivity considers newborn and partner as an intrinsic unit, the dyad. It is especially activated during intimate dyadic free play. In such face-to-face, eye-to-eye intersubjective emotional communications, infant and mother intently look at and listen to each other. Thereby, they synchronize and regulate each other’s emotional states. During these protoconversations, the partners’ emotions are expressed and actively perceived in spontaneous, reciprocal, and rhythmic-turn-taking interactions (Trevarthen, 1993). In this perspective, the dyad becomes the place where they meet, as well as where they miss each other. Every time they meet or miss each other results from an intelligent combination of their ability to perceive each other’s motives. In her tribute to Stern, Beebe (2017, p. 234) stated that “the meaning of the behavior is co-created,” recalling that the way communication is bidirectionally regulated is specific to a particular dyad. Feldman et al. (2011) developed a similar neurophysiological model, including physiological synchrony from birth: when mother and child are physically close, they synchronize their breathing and heart rate. This provides a basis for later “social synchrony,” in which partners meet and miss one another and respond during shared interactions. But sometimes birth comes earlier than expected and requires hospitalization, thereby separating mother and child and medicalizing their first relations. The present study investigates the essential question of how and to what extent the quality of communication can be supported from the start in the context of very preterm birth.

      Before term birth

      For the fetus, pregnancy is a time for chemosensory learning, which helps him adapt to his new after-birth environment (Lickliter, 2000). The uterine environment provides plentiful multisensory stimulations. The fetus can experience vestibular, somaesthetic, auditory, chemical and olfactory stimulations, simultaneously or sequentially (Pinéda et al., 2017a).

      In utero, the fetus moves intentionally, with controlled timing and sequencing, anticipating sensory confirmation of self-related effects (Piontelli, 2010). The fetus is bathed in an environment of vestibular, somatosensory, tactile, and auditory rhythmic stimuli, through the mother’s breathing, heartbeats, walking, dancing, running, speaking, singing, etc. (Provasi et al., 2021). After 20 weeks, some fetuses make self-touching gestures with their left hand, which may signal sympathetic emotional attitudes with a mother’s feelings of stress, and respond positively to her voice (Reissland and Kisilevsky, 2015). Various sounds are present in the uterus, with frequencies and intensities that are low yet detectable by the fetus (Philbin, 2017). When the mother speaks or sings, sound travels through the inside but also through the air, enabling the fetus to perceive her voice from both internal and external sources (Pino, 2016). Fetuses are able to clearly identify the mother’s voice, and could shape mouth movements when they hear someone speaking (Marx and Nagy, 2015). Toward the end of pregnancy, fetuses are particularly attuned to maternal acoustic cues (Ferrari et al., 2016) and they are able to detect, recognize, respond, and remember some characteristics of her voice (Kisilevsky et al., 2003; Voegtline et al., 2013).

      Term birth

      Babies are equipped to perceive the world they are born into. Newborns have a holistic perception of others: they perceive faces, voices, body movements, tactile stimuli, smells, and shapes. Evidence suggests that the mother’s voice is one of the major drivers of the intricate intersensory and intermodal connections that are formed and retained in memory in the days following birth (Gratier and Devouche, 2017). The mother’s voice is a salient, consistent and frequent stimulus that provides before-after birth continuity, regardless of the drastic changes linked to the exit of the uterine environment (Lickliter, 2000). From that perspective, the full-term newborn experiences a normal biological continuum between intra- and extra-uterine lives. At birth, the newborn is exposed to multimodal stimulations that form intermodal redundancies (joint and repeated apprehension of the environment with different sensory modalities; Provasi et al., 2014). This gives meaning to the newborn’s environment, making his/her ecological niche. Significant biological stimulations originating from the mother, notably on the olfactory (Varendi et al., 1997) and auditory levels (Doheny et al., 2012), lead to an improvement of physiological and behavioral stability and better adapted responses.

      A parent, or any caring elder, can pick up on the infant’s skills to regulate him- or herself as well as the partner, thus enabling the development of a new and unique relationship of intimate familiarity and responsiveness (Brazelton et al., 1979; Trevarthen, 2011; Devouche and Gratier, 2019). In this context, Dominguez et al. (2016) and Boiteau et al. (2021) highlighted a tight coordination in the timing of vocalization between 2 to 4-day-old neonates and their parent. In both studies, neonates demonstrated control over the timing of vocalizations produced with an attentive and affectionate partner, with a 1-s temporal window explaining most baby-parent vocal contingencies. However, to date, no study has explored temporal coordination between the newborn and a partner in the context of preterm birth.

      Preterm birth

      Preterms have been denied part of their prenatal chemosensory learning time, and deprived of the typical biological continuum between pre- and postnatal life. In addition, after birth, they have to face the incubator’s harsh environment that combines sensory deprivations, over-stimulations and inappropriate stimulations (Lickliter, 2000), which do not match their pre-organized sensory expectations. Preterm babies receive many sensory stimulations that are not adapted to their sensory maturation level, including more and higher levels of auditory stimulations (machine noises and rings, voices that are transformed and amplified by the incubator’s walls) and reduced vestibular stimulation, at a time when they should be benefiting from filtered auditory stimulations, and regular vestibular stimulations provided by the mother’s movements (Lickliter, 2000). They are also likely to be subjected to visual over-stimulations (bright lights) sometimes necessary in highly technical care. Furthermore, they are confronted to many unpleasant (antiseptic agents) but very few familiar or maternal smells (Marlier and Schaal, 2005). The preterm newborn is thus exposed to early sensory experiences that are atypical, both in quality and quantity, and that occur during the most critical period for the nervous system’s development (Pinéda et al., 2017b; Montagna and Nosarti, 2016).

      At birth, preterms’ motor and interactional skills are fragile, which makes it is more difficult for them than for healthy term babies, to initiate an exchange and respond actively (Muller-Nix et al., 2004). However, they can discriminate between two stimuli, associate events, and become habituated to external stimulations. The preterm infant does receive environmental input and plays an active part in the interactive system (Provasi, 2019). Nevertheless, interactions between mothers and preterm children often involve poorer maternal adaptation to the infant’s signals, leading to less maternal touch, as well as fewer vocalizations and gazes (Feldman, 2006; Forcada-Guex et al., 2011).

      Preterm birth is associated with high stress and anxiety levels in parents (Segre et al., 2014), including post-traumatic stress (Forcada-Guex et al., 2011; Anderson and Cacola, 2017). A correlation has been evidenced between maternal and paternal stress, as well as between parental stress and the quality of father–mother–infant interactions in premature (Gatta et al., 2017). In the meta-analysis of Grote et al. (2010), 39% of mothers with a preterm baby presented postpartum depressive (PPD) symptoms. De Paula Eduardo et al.’s (2019) systematic review analyses the studies that explored preterm birth as a risk factor for postpartum depression in the last 10 years, and provides evidence of higher risk for PPD among mothers of preterm infants up to 24 weeks after childbirth. Stressful or traumatizing events have been shown to slow down oxytocin release, which inhibits the mother’s empathetic abilities (Feldman, 2015). Indeed, mothers of preterm infants provide a less responsive and stimulating environment than mothers of full-term infants (Muller-Nix et al., 2004). Preterm birth could permanently disturb the interactional sphere (Zelkowitz et al., 2009). The analysis of preterms’ neurological and behavioral disturbances highlights the entanglement between biological vulnerability (brain alteration) and environmental factors, such as stress, perinatal pain, and break of the emotional connexion with the mother (Montagna and Nosarti, 2016).

      Pierrat et al. (2021, the EPIPAGE 2 study) showed that at age 5, behavioral disturbances were parents’ most frequent preoccupation in France. This study also showed that the level of prematurity is crucial to neurodevelopment: moderate or severe developmental difficulties observed in 27% of extreme preterm children, 19% of very preterm children, and 12% of moderately preterm children. Irrespective of the degree of prematurity, 35% of preterms require adapted care to prevent difficulties from impacting the child’s daily life and learning. More precisely: 50% of extreme preterms, 1/3 of very preterm children and ¼ of moderately preterm children received support in their development (speech therapy, psychomotor education, psychological support …). In the OLIMPE study, an ancillary study to EPIPAGE 2, Cambonie et al. (2017) recorded disorganized interactive behaviors on discharge from the maternity ward (50% for mothers, 33% for preterm babies), which persisted at 6 months (32% for mothers and 26% for preterms).

      Skin-to-skin contact

      Skin-to-skin contact is a consistent and reciprocal interaction, which is entirely dedicated to the parent-infant relationship. It is performed routinely around the world, starting from birth, as part of intensive care in neonatal units (Nyqvist et al., 2010). Skin-to-skin contact is offered during hospitalization, as part of family-centered care programs, as a support of the subjective experience of parenthood (Roué et al., 2017). This natural relational opportunity helps parents develop a sense of responsibility that is often on hold while the infant is in medical care. Skin-to-skin contact, along with infant-directed singing, allows mother and child to synchronize rhythmically and provides an envelope that enables the tuning of different rhythmic stimulations (Markova et al., 2019). Supporting mutual synchrony through SCC is in line with Thomson et al.’s (2013) recommendation to include some sense of coherence into the neonatal environment, by giving parents a central role in the decision-making process and supporting their engagement to care for their child.

      In the specific context of a preterm birth, SSC has immediate physiological and neurobehavioral benefits (Feldman and Eidelman, 2003): it facilitates the vulnerable newborn’s adaptation to extra-uterine life (Kristoffersen et al., 2016). Longer-term benefits include better psychomotor (Feldman et al., 2002) and cognitive development (Charpak et al., 2005; Feldman et al., 2014). Mothers and fathers of preterm newborns experience an increase of oxytocin release and a reduction in cortisol and stress responses during SSC, as well as facilitated breastfeeding (Cong et al., 2015; Mörelius et al., 2015). Mothers’ psychological benefits include reduced stress and postpartum depression risk (Athanasopoulou and Fox, 2014; Zhao and Zhang, 2020). Additional benefits have been evidenced on parents’ affective and interactive behavior at corrected term, as well as on the quality of their attachment (Tallandini and Scalembra, 2006; Moore et al., 2016; Feldman et al., 2014). Furthermore, SSC may promote parental presence, even in poor reception conditions (Raiskila et al., 2017).

      Indeed, SSC has been widely studied (Charpak et al., 2005) and several meta-analyses or meta-syntheses have investigated its implementation (Seidman et al., 2015; Chan et al., 2016) or its benefits (Moore et al., 2016; Lawn et al., 2010; Mori et al., 2010; Anderzen-Carlsson et al., 2014; Johnston et al., 2017; Conde-Agudelo et al., 2011).

      There has been a real paradigm shift toward family-centered care (Franck and O'Brien, 2019; Skene et al., 2019). In their paper presenting eight principles for patient-centered and family-centered care for newborns in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU), Roué et al. (2017) listed the main benefits of SSC, however omitting to mention potential benefits for communication. Studies investigating this aspect are scarce. In their systematic review on the effects of early communication intervention on the speech and communication skills of preterm infants in the NICU, Harding et al. (2019) identified five studies, all conducted on very or extremely preterm babies, with outcome measures collected 1 month after birth (one study) and at 3 months CA or more (four other studies). Importantly, four of these studies correlated infant and mother measures but none actually focused on communication during SSC. Only Caskey et al. (2014) investigated the timing and coordination of parent-preemie communications, through a turn-taking measure computed by the LENA system, with a 5-s time window of temporal contingency. Authors considered any block that contains both infant and adult speech as conversational and found that infants responded preferentially and more frequently to their mothers compared with their fathers. However, their measure of coordination timing remained less precise than the one used by Dominguez et al. (2016) and Boiteau et al. (2021).

      The search terms used for the Harding et al. (2019) review excluded several studies, such as Tallandini and Scalembra (2006), Feldman et al. (2014), or Buil et al. (2016) that nonetheless focused on mother-preemie communication. The study conducted by Buil et al. (2020) precisely aimed to investigate how communication could be improved during SSC, by modifying SSC positioning. Considering that mothers often complained about the impossibility to look at their baby’s face, because of the vertical positioning of their child on their chest (leading them to sometimes prefer an arm-holding cuddle), authors investigated how an innovative kangaroo Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) positioning influenced mother and infant well-being and communication during SSC, while maintaining a safe and preventive preterm posture (Buil et al., 2016). This positioning was developed in a high-tech NICU according to recent experts’ recommendations for promoting a “semi reclined positioning” (Ludington-Hoe et al., 2008; Nyqvist et al., 2010). SDF positioning was also found to reduce the risk of postnatal maternal depression and to promote and prolong SSC sessions (Buil et al., 2019). Moreover, Buil et al. (2020) tested the kangaroo SDF positioning 18 days after a very preterm birth and showed that SDF positioning improves the mother’s ability to perceive her infant’s behaviors such as vocalizations, smiles or eyes openings, as well as to respond in a timely manner. Since its creation, other teams have investigated potential benefits of skin to skin in SDF position, either looking at “the influence of a skin-to-skin lullaby on the stability of maternal behavior and on the tonic emotional manifestations of the preterm infant” (Provasi et al., 2021) or at the “vocal responsiveness of preterm infants to maternal infant-directed speaking and singing during skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) in the NICU” (Carvalho et al., 2019). However, in these studies, observations were not made immediately after birth. Therefore, the essential question of how and to what extent SDF positioning enhances the quality of communication from the start is yet to be examined. In this direction, our team has previously explored touch and maternal vocal behavior during the first skin to skin in a sample of our population. In this position, mothers display more active, securing and affectionate touch, favoring a quality early reunion, free from over-stimulations, as reflected by drowsiness and less agitation in the baby (Buil et al., 2017a). Moreover, from the very first minutes of skin to skin, mothers in SDF provide a denser and more musical sound envelope (Buil et al., 2017b).

      Given our current knowledge on the effects of temporal coordination during communication on infant development, particularly in the neonatal period and infancy, the question of mother-preterm communication is decisive. The first minutes of the very first skin to skin are the physical and emotional reunions of mother and child after birth in a potential traumatic context. Nonetheless, current literature regarding the first skin to skin in the NICU focuses mainly on its secure feasibility (Linnér et al., 2020), the moment of and impact on its implementation during hospitalization (Mörelius et al., 2012; Blomqvist et al., 2013), its physiological benefits for the preterm (Cadwell et al., 2018; Gere et al., 2021; Pandya et al., 2021) or parents’ feelings (Maastrup et al., 2018). The data collected in this pioneering study shed light on what actually happens between mothers and their babies born prematurely during their first skin to skin.

      The present study aimed to better characterize the vocal meeting of the mother and her very preterm newborn, during the first minutes of their first ever skin to skin. We also further examined the immediate benefits of SDF positioning during the first SSC on the quality of mother—very-preterm infant communication. We hypothesized that, compared to Vertical positioning and as early as 3–4 days after birth, SDF positioning would, by increasing opportunities for eye contact, improve the mothers’ ability to recognize her infant’s signals, thus enabling a more timely feedback. Finally, we aimed to examine how the communicative behaviors of mothers and very-preterm infants were coordinated in time during SSC, just a few days after birth.

      Materials and methods Participants

      The study was conducted between May 2015 and June 2016, in a level III NICU at the Centre Hospitalier Intercommunal de Créteil (France). Among the 90 very preterm babies (27 to 31 + 6 weeks’ gestation) admitted during the inclusion period, 53 met parent and child’s inclusion criteria [living in the geographical area considering the longitudinal follow-up, no multiple birth > 2, no neurological complication due to several vascular hemorrhage (IVH grade III, or IV)], no severe medical conditions, no incapacitated physical or psychological illness in the mother. Seven mothers refused to participate, and two inclusions were missed. Among the remaining 44 births, two were lost during follow-up and eight were multiple births which were not included in the present report. The final sample included 34 very preterm infants and their mothers. The first 17 dyads were offered SSC positioning, as usually practiced in the participating NICU (Vertical Control Group). The following 17 dyads were offered the Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) Intervention positioning and these were matched to the first 17 dyads on newborns’ gestational age at birth and weight at birth (see Buil et al., 2020 for the detailed method). Participants’ socio-demographic, Ob/Gyn, delivery and birth data were obtained from medical files.

      The present study is part of a longitudinal follow-up from very preterm birth until 3 months corrected age. The data presented constituted the first data collection point of this prospective monocentric matched-pair case-control study. Some data from our study of mother-very preterm communication at 18 days post-partum have already been published in a previous paper (Buil et al., 2020). Measures of the mothers’ risk of depression (made before the first SSC session), were comparable for the SDF positioning group and the Vertical positioning group, with EPDS mean scores of 13.8 and 12.9, respectively (Buil et al., 2019).

      Skin-to-skin positioning in each group

      In the Vertical Control Group, preemies were positioned chest to chest between the mother’s breasts, at the center and on the median line of the mother’s torso, in an upright position, with a breastfeeding nursing pillow (see Figures 1A,B). According to Kangaroo Mother Care guidelines, the head is turned to one side and in a slightly extended position which keeps the airway open. Moreover, the hips should be flexed and extended in a “frog” position; the arms should also be flexed (World Health Organization, 2003).

      Vertical control positioning (A,B) and Supported Diagonal Flexion (SDF) intervention positioning (C,D) during the very first SSC after very preterm birth.

      In the SDF Intervention positioning (see Figures 1C,D), the baby is naked, off-center and semi-reclined on the mother’s chest, chest to chest (Buil et al., 2016). In this intermittent kangaroo mother care, the choice of on which side of the body (right or left) the baby’s head is positioned is up to the mother. The SDF position is characterized by a slightly flexed body axis, with the limbs retracted in a preventive posture and the head in line with the body axis to prevent side-to-side toppling and to free respiratory permeability (Nyqvist et al., 2010), moderately externally rotated hips in flexion-abduction (Vaivre-Douret et al., 2004), with adducted shoulders (Ferrari et al., 2007). The baby’s head is located between the mother’s nipple and clavicle and oriented toward the mother’s face. His/her arms and legs are flexed, in a naturally adopted asymmetrical tonic neck posture (Casaer, 1979), according to the baby’s gestational age and comfort. The baby is positioned on the mother’s naked chest, a baby wrap adjusted around the two is used to support and help maintain the baby’s posture and to relieve the mother. For the study purpose, we chose The Little-Wrap-Without-A-Knot is a knitted fabric, specially made for Baby Wearing. On one end of the cloth are sewn two metal rings. The Fabric has a special density of 300 gr/m2. It is made of a two-faced Viscose certified Oeko-Tex 100. The special mechanism of this knitted fabric, allows it to slightly stretch, and to ensure a soft touch that respects the preterm baby’s skin. The Little-Wrap-Without-A-Knot is put as a loop around the mother’s body. It goes from shoulder to opposite hip and back up to the shoulder, in an asymmetric diagonal. The end of the fabric passes through the rings that creates a buckle effect. This adjustment system ensures a precise fit between the two morphologies and to maintain the desired position of the baby.

      In both positionings, the mother was comfortably seated in an adjustable armchair, her back inclined 40°, with a toe-clip, dim light and a quiet atmosphere; she had the choice to wear clothes or not. In SDF Intervention positioning, the baby wrap was placed over the mother’s clothes or on her naked chest.

      Apparatus

      Mother and neonate were filmed during their very first SSC, on average 3.7 days after very preterm birth, i.e., on average 29.4 weeks GA (see Table 1). For each dyad, vocal interaction was analyzed only during the first 5 min. Indeed, mothers were particularly active during the first minutes of SSC care, before giving way to mutual relaxation. Thus, 5 min recordings allowed sufficient data collection, but left plenty of intimacy time to the dyad. Mother-neonate dyads were filmed using a single camcorder mounted on a tripod and sound recordings were obtained using a shotgun microphone. Video and audio recordings were synchronized. The camcorder was placed in front of the dyad, in order to frame the mother’s chest and the baby’s entire body, as well as their faces.

      Infant’s’ characteristics at birth and at first skin to skin, according to type of SSC positioning.

      SDF intervention group Vertical control group
      N = 12 N = 11
      Mean (SD) N (%) Mean (SD) N (%)
      Birth data
      Gender
      Girl 8 (67%) 7 (64%)
      Weight (g) 1,079 (266) 1,126 (306)
      <1,000 5 (42%) 4 (36%)
      1,000–2000 7 (58%) 7 (64%)
      Gestational age(w) 29.4 (2.7) 29.4 (2.4)
      <28 3 (25%) 2 (18%)
      28 to <32 9 (75%) 9 (82%)
      Spontaneous breathing
      Yes 3 (25%) 3 (27%)
      Data at first skin to skin
      Age (days) 3.8 (2.0) 3.7 (2.1)
      Weight (g) 994 (203) 1,048 (290)
      <1,000 6 (50%) 4 (36%)
      1,000–2000 6 (50%) 7 (64%)
      Video and acoustic analyses

      Video analysis was performed using The Observer XT with a precision of 40 ms (25 images per second). Vocal data were analyzed with the software Audacity.

      Coding of newborns’ states of alertness

      Newborns’ state of alertness was coded with frame-by-frame video microanalysis using the software The Observer XT1 and following APIB’s state configurations (Als et al., 1982): (1) deep sleep state, (2) active sleep state, (3) drowsiness state, (4AL) low awake state, (4AH) high awake state, (4B) quiet awake state, (5) active awake state, and (6) crying state. We included an additional “undefined” category, when the infant’s state of alertness could not be coded because the newborn’s face and body could not be viewed in the video. Coding was carried out by a perinatal professional with NIDCAP certification (Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program). We recorded the time of onset and duration of each of these states of alertness.

      Coding of newborn and maternal vocalizations

      Maternal and newborn vocalizations were coded using the software Audacity.2 Based on the visualization of sonograms and audio guidance, a segmentation into two types of events was made: maternal vocalization and newborn vocalization, (Gratier et al., 2015; Dominguez et al., 2016), both defined as the production of vocal sound that was either continuous or included unvoiced segments of <300 ms. If a pause following an audible vocal sound was >300 ms, two successive vocalizations were coded. Vegetative sounds produced by infants such as burps, growls or hiccups, noise from the environment and vegetative sounds produced by mothers, such as coughs, were not coded. Our criterion was more flexible than that used with full term babies, in order to include more of the preemies’ voiced demonstrations.

      Coding of turn-taking sequences

      A turn-taking sequence was defined as a sequence of vocalizations involving at least one alternation between interactive partners, which is expressed as follows. When more than one alternation occurred, with intervening pauses between the two partners, the number of pauses coincided with the number of turns in the sequence (e.g., two turns: “newborn vocalisation-pause-mother vocalisation-pause-newborn vocalisation”; three turns: “newborn vocalisation-pause-mother vocalisation-pause-newborn vocalization-pause-mother vocalisation” and so on). A turn-taking sequence ended when the same partner produced at least two successive vocalizations, or when the pause following a vocalization exceeded 3,000 ms in accordance with previous studies (Stern, 1985; Gratier et al., 2015; Dominguez et al., 2016; Boiteau et al., 2021).

      Training and reliability

      A single coder was responsible for coding states of consciousness with the Observer software and two coders for coding the vocal exchange with the Audacity software. None of the coders were aware of the aim and hypotheses under investigation. For this reason, training was performed on randomly chosen dyads both from the SDF and Vertical positioning groups. The training of coders consisted in three steps. First, coders were trained to use the coding template on 4 dyads under the supervision of the researchers. During the second step, each trainee was invited to code 4 additional dyads alone. At the end of this step, the coding was checked and discussed with the supervisor. The third step consisted in reapplying the second step.

      Twenty five percent of the data set, chosen randomly, was double-coded. Inter-coder reliability (Pearson product–moment correlations) regarding the number of behaviors ranged from 0.82 to 0.92 depending on the behavior. Onset positions were considered identical if they occurred within 80 ms (i.e., two frames); thus, measures of behavior duration had an error tolerance of up to 160 ms. Both coders correctly identified 77.2% of all onset positions within the subset of double-coded sequences.

      Ethical considerations

      All mothers were offered to participate to the research study on a voluntary basis, within the first 2 days postpartum, and in all cases before the first SSC session. Every mother was informed of the research by a letter in the NICU. Mothers gave a written informed consent before participating. Written informed consent was obtained from the mothers for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article. An initial information-based meeting was organized prior to data collection. This research was approved by the French Local Ethics Consulting Committee for the Protection of Persons (IRB n°2015120001072).

      Statistics

      All analyses were performed using Stata for Windows (version 14; StataCorp). Sociodemographic data, previous obstetrical and delivery data and infants’ characteristics at birth and at first skin to skin contact were compared in both groups, with either a t-test or a chi-square test, depending on the measure. The number of vocalizations and their duration were analyzed through a general linear model with an adjustment on age and weight at birth. Parametric tests were not chosen considering these measures did not differ significantly from normality.

      Results

      For 11 dyads, either the baby or the mother did not vocalize during the recorded session of SSC. Analysis of the vocal exchange was therefore conducted on 23 dyads, 11 dyads in the Vertical control group and 12 in the SDF Intervention group. Table 2 presents sociodemographic characteristics, obstetrical and delivery data, and Table 1 infant’s data at birth, at first skin to skin, according to SSC positioning. No significant difference was found between the two groups.

      Sociodemographic, previous obstetrical, and delivery data according to SSC positioning.

      SDF intervention group Vertical control group
      N = 12 N = 11
      Mean (SD) N (%) Mean (SD) N (%)
      Socio-demographic data
      Mothers’ age (years) 30.9 (6.4) 30.2 (4.8)
      ≥30 9 (75%) 8 (73%)
      Living with partner
      Yes 10 (83%) 11 (100%)
      Employment status
      Employed 9 (75%) 10 (91%)
      Previous obstetrical history
      Gestity before current pregnancy 2.3 (1.0) 2.1 (1.0)
      Yes 9 (75%) 7 (64%)
      Parity before current pregnancy 0.47 (0.62) 0.88 (0.99)
      Nulliparous 6 (50%) 6 (55%)
      Early pregnancy loss 0.88 (0.93) 0.82 (1.24)
      Yes 5 (42%) 3 (27%)
      Late pregnancy loss
      Yes 0 (0%) 1 (9%)
      Previous history of preterm birth/LBW
      Yes 1 (12%) 2 (18%)
      Current pregnancy
      Hospitalization during pregnancy
      Yes 5 (42%) 11 (100%)
      High risk pregnancy
      Yes 5 (42%) 8 (73%)
      Intra uterine growth restriction 3 (25%) 3 (27%)
      Hypertension/pre-eclampsia 3 (25%) 5 (45%)
      Preterm delivery threat 1 (8%) 4 (36%)
      Current delivery
      Spontaneous delivery 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
      Labor induction 12 (100%) 11 (100%)
      Fetal heart rate abnormalities 8 (67%) 4 (36%)
      Intra uterine growth restriction 5 (42%) 3 (27%)
      Hypertension/Pre-eclampsia 4 (33%) 1 (9%)
      Delivery type
      Vaginal delivery 1 (8%) 4 (36%)
      Caesarean section 11 (92%) 7 (64%)
      State of consciousness

      Infants were mainly in a state of drowsiness (state 3), even more so in the Vertical group (58%) than in the SDF group (35% of time). Active sleep state (state 2) represented around 35% of the time in both groups. However, infants in the SDF group also spent 19% of the time in deep sleep (state 1), while those in the Vertical group spent only 3%. In SDF group, infants spent 58% of the time sleeping (states 1 and 2) compared to 36% in the Vertical group. Distributions were significantly different [Chi-square (5) = 116, p < 0.0001].

      Newborns’ vocal production

      We collected 167 vocalizations: 84 in the SDF group (i.e., on average 7.0 per newborn and 1.4 per minute) and 83 in the Vertical group (i.e., on average 7.5 per newborn and 1.5 per minute, adj p = 0.90). Vocalizations’ duration was on average longer in the Vertical group (Vertical: 817 ms, SDF: 537 ms), but not significantly after adjustment on birth age and weight (adj p = 0.78). Consequently, newborns’ vocalizations occupied less dialogue space in the SDF group than in the Vertical group (1.3% vs. 1.9% of the 5 min analyzed).

      Mothers’ vocal production

      Overall, mothers vocalized twice more in the SDF group (604, i.e., on average 45.3 per mother and 9.1 per minute) than in the Vertical group (269, i.e., on average 24.5 per mother and 4.9 per minute), but the difference was not significant (adj p = 0.068). Their vocalizations were on average significantly longer (SDF: 1166 ms, Vertical: 1068 ms, adj p = 0.002). Hence, Mothers’ vocalizations occupied more dialogue space in the SDF group than in the Vertical group (20.1% vs. 8.0% of the 5 min analyzed, p = 0.007).

      Temporal proximity of mother and newborn vocalizations

      Based on a 3-s criterion, 3 preterm newborn vocalizations out of 4 (74%) were at a temporal proximity from a maternal vocalization in the SDF intervention group against 1 out of 2 (47%) in the Vertical control group (OR = 3.2, p < 0.0001). The difference was still significant when using a 1-s criterion (57% vs. 39%, OR = 2.1, p = 0.017).

      Turn-taking coordination

      Based on a 3-s criterion, 73 mother-newborn turn-taking sequences (TTS) were identified: 44 in the SDF group and 29 in the Vertical group. In all, 75 newborn vocalizations were involved in these sequences, i.e., 45% of the total number of vocalizations collected. Almost all sequences were either one turn “baby–mother” or two turns “mother–baby–mother.” One third of these were two-turns sequences, sensitively but not significantly more in the SDF group (OR = 1.4, p = 0.56). When using a 1-s criterion, the number of TTS sequences fell to 65: 36 in the SDF group and 29 in the Vertical group: the odd ratio indicates a 3.8 times higher chance to observe a two-turns sequence rather than a one-turn sequence in the SDF intervention group than in the Vertical control group, but it does not reach significance (OR = 3.8, p = 0.059).

      Discussion

      The present study brings pioneer data on the very first skin to skin between mother and her very premature baby. Although novel, analyses conducted in the paper are pilot, the first of their kind.

      First meeting: Not necessarily vocal

      Our study focused on the very first vocal exchanges, therefore we selected dyads in which both the mother and the baby vocalized (23 out of 34 dyads). It is important to note that during the first 5 min of the first meeting after giving birth, some mothers may have not wished to speak. This very first skin-to-skin is also the very first opportunity for mothers to hold the newborn, with a place for spontaneity as it is a non-medical, non-nursing, and non-guided moment. It is a moment for intimacy, for being together, and not necessarily for speaking. Indeed, during this moment, deciding to speak or not to speak to the newborn was the mother’s choice. It seems that this choice was the expression of the mother’s preferred way of communicating, the relationship being potentially expressed through other sensory modalities: tactile (Buil et al., 2017a), kinaesthetic, visual.

      Better sleep quality

      Our results showed that within the first 5 min of the very first skin to skin, very preterm newborns in the “SDF” group spent less time in a drowsy state (state 3), and more in deep sleep (state 1), than those in the Vertical control group. This result suggests that the “SDF” positioning helps preterms stabilize in restorative sleep during skin to skin, rather than stay in a state of drowsiness, considered by some authors as a transitional state, costly in energy (Als, 1982; Brazelton and Nugent, 1995; Bullinger and Goubet, 1999; Foreman et al., 2008; Devouche and Buil, 2019). This stabilization of preterm newborns’ state could rely on both an improved postural support thanks to the SDF positioning that fosters the axial winding posture with support on the neck and retroverted pelvis (Vaivre-Douret et al., 2004), but also the active behavioral support of mothers installed in SDF positioning. These mothers would intuitively help their child into deep sleep, switching rapidly from state 2 to 1, or appease him/her. From the first minutes of this renewed closeness, the SDF positioning could increase mothers’ sensitivity to their child’s signals of disorganization (such as growling, frowning, wriggling, spreading their fingers, etc.,), allowing them to respond by speaking, nursing or caressing them before they reach full disorganization.

      Premature newborn vocal presence

      During this moment, that occurred on average at 3.5 days of life, newborns’ vocal production did not differ significantly between the two groups, even though vocalizations were on average longer in the Vertical group (before statistical adjustment). Our vocalizations sample was not large enough to allow us to investigate the links between length of vocalizations and states of awareness. However, we believe that the lengthier vocalizations recorded in the Vertical group could be linked to the state 3 that is found in the synactive theory of development (Als, 1982) and characterized by more grunting, which are also probably longer than other kinds of vocalizations. It would be interesting to investigate this further, by characterizing the quality of this vocalization.

      One aim of this study was to better characterize the vocal meeting between mother and very preterm newborn, during the first minutes of their first ever skin to skin. Our study provides pioneering data regarding the vocal presence of the very preterm newborn during the first SSC, which represents 1.4 to 1.5 vocalizations per minute for the 23 dyads in which vocalizations did occur, regardless of the positioning. The observed frequency was much higher than that recorded by Caskey et al. (2011), because data was collected at a time dedicated to the relationship, whereas Caskey and colleagues made their recording during several hours in a crib. Vocal displays were scarcer in our very preterm newborns than in healthy term newborns installed for a cuddle in the parent’s arms or close to each other [3/min in Boiteau et al., 2021 and 2.7/min in Dominguez et al., 2016], but they were present. Indeed, it is hard to know whether these vocalizations were signs of discomfort or communication, whether they were voluntary or not, and whether they were addressed or not. Their mere existence, despite infants’ immaturity, parents’ feeling of unease and the hypermedicalization of these first moments, provides an opportunity to establish communication.

      Maternal vocal envelope

      Our results showed a denser vocal presence in the SDF group than in the Vertical group (20% vs. 8%), with longer vocal interventions. This longer exposure to the mother’s voice might be important according to a recent literature review highlighting its positive impact on parenting skills (Filippa et al., 2019). This result supports the hypothesis that SSC in SDF positioning enhances and supports early vocal contact. This strategy is now recognized as a new and important Infant- and Family-Centered Developmental Care (IFCDC) strategy for the benefit of preterm infant brain development (Filippa et al., 2017; Monaci et al., 2021).

      This clearly denser maternal vocal envelope could be perceived as over-stimulating for a 3–4-day preterm newborn. The SDF positioning was not conceived to promote “more” (as in “too much”) but rather “better,” closer to what would be the first meeting with a healthy term newborn. Here, judging by the analysis of the newborn’s states of consciousness, it seems that these behaviors were not perceived as over-stimulations. This question could be further explored by investigating the links between maternal voice and states of consciousness, as it was done by Filippa et al. (2018) or Saliba et al. (2020).

      Temporal coordination

      In our pilot data collection, we tried to investigate the vocal temporal coordination between the mother and her very preterm newborn as Dominguez et al. (2016) and Boiteau et al. (2021) did with term-newborns. However, the few vocalizations recorded only allowed a timed analysis. Although no difference was found between both positionings regarding the amount of newborn vocalizations involved in turn-taking, descriptive results suggest more complex turn-taking in the SDF intervention group. Although not significant, this result is consistent with the denser vocal presence of mothers. We hypothesized that being more able to see the face and perceiving their newborn as an available partner, mothers in the SDF group were more likely to vocalize in a timely manner and thus create the very first vocal exchange with their baby.

      Furthermore, based on a 1-s criterion, 57% of the preterm newborn vocalizations were at a temporal proximity of a mother’s vocalization in the SDF intervention group against 39% in the Vertical control group. Hence, our results show that mothers in the SDF intervention group provided a more proximal vocal envelope, but far from the 95% observed by Dominguez et al. (2016) in mothers of healthy term babies. By nature, full-term infants require the intervention of competent adults, and in this sense, it is not relevant to oppose the “natural” lives of term infants with the “unnatural” lives of hospitalized infants (Gratier and Devouche, 2017). Environments are constructed around infants but more importantly, infants, from the earliest moments of their lives, participate in this construction. The present study adds to the current knowledge and understanding that preterm infants respond to their environment too. Our results plead for more reflection on how to better adapt the environment, so that in turn, it responds to the premature baby.

      Developmental perspectives

      Dyads observed in the present study were followed during the first months of life and data collected at 18 days of life, i.e., 15 days after the very first SSC have already been published in a previous paper (Buil et al., 2020). At birth, preemies in both groups did not differ in their vocal production, the main results being that mothers provide a more proximal and denser vocal envelope. Two weeks later, in the SDF Intervention Group, very preterm infants vocalized three times more, mothers vocalized, gazed at their baby’s face, and smiled more than in the Vertical Control Group, and temporal proximity of mother-infant behaviors in a one-second time frame was greater in the SDF Intervention Group (Buil et al., 2020).

      The present study and the previous one plead in favor of a positive impact of a more comfortable positioning, that allows visual contact for mother-preemie interaction from the very beginning. Indeed, when offered more opportunities to be connected, mothers tended to engage more in the communication. Being able to perceive key behaviors such as open eyes, mouth movements, or being able to understand a vocal manifestation because they have a better perception of the whole story, mothers are more responsive from the very beginning. Thus, because primary intersubjectivity had the possibility to develop and so had the dyadic space (Trevarthen, 1998), mother and preemie were more able to meet and communicate 2 weeks later.

      SDF positioning not only influences positively infants’ states of consciousness and mothers’ availability, thus fostering communication. Indeed, what seems to matter the most is the mother’s responsiveness, her ability to perceive the availability of her very preterm newborn to interact (Ainsworth et al., 2015) and to adjust her behavior at the level of body tonus and emotion (Ajuriaguerra, 1986).

      Long-term perspective: Supporting the preemie, the mother and the father, and a timely adjusted communication during SSC

      Given the persistence of difficulties in parent-infant synchrony at 3 (Feldman and Eidelman, 2007) and 6 months GA (Forcada-Guex et al., 2011) in preterm contexts, offering SSC to support communication between the preemie and his parent as soon as possible after birth is essential. Feldman et al. (2014) pointed out that any attempt to encourage mother-infant proximity could help fragile babies and their parents to develop optimal synchrony before the end of the sensitive period. Practicing SSC with SDF positioning, rather than SSC as it is widely practiced today (Vertical positioning), offsets the paucity of parent-infant communication related to preterm birth, by enhancing both partners’ production and detection of multimodal signals, as well as their temporal coordination. Our results highlight the possibility of mother—very preterm baby vocal exchanges during skin-to-skin, especially with SDF positioning, as early as 3/4 days after birth.

      The literature shows that mothers present a left side bias for carrying their baby (Donnot and Vauclair, 2005; Malatesta et al., 2019), which seems particularly stable during the first 3 months of the child’s life, regardless of culture and time (Malatesta et al., 2019). These two studies noted correlations between carrying bias and maternal empathy as well as mother’s capacity to engage emotionally in a relationship (Donnot and Vauclair, 2005; Malatesta et al., 2019). In the present study, five mothers out of 17 spontaneously chose to carry their newborn on the left side of their body during the first SSC. Hence, by allowing mothers to choose on which side their wish to carry their child each time a skin-to-skin care is proposed, SSC in SDF positioning is indeed a natural precocious way to support intuitive parenting (Papoušek and Papoušek, 2002).

      In their article on the eight principles to follow in NICU, Roué et al. (2017) highlighted the need for a free 24 h/24 parental access and a psychological parental support. In their conclusion they recalled the need for future researches in this direction. More recently, Filippa et al. (2021) published a systematic review on the benefits of involving the fathers of preterm infants in early interventions in neonatal intensive care units. Their conclusion was in line with Roué et al.’s recommendations by stressing the need to develop new, multimodal and interactive interventions that provide fathers with positive contact with their preterm infants. Indeed, few studies were conducted on early father-preterm communication. However, as Boiteau et al. (2021) showed, the father and the baby meet as of the birth, why not the father and the (very) premature newborn? Benefits of SDF positioning could thus be appreciated through daily practice with fathers in NICU.

      SDF positioning could promote brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions (Kinreich et al., 2017). SSC is described as the only developmental care dedicated to dyadic interaction (Buil, 2019), but this appears all the more obvious when both partners manage to meet (even briefly), by making eye contact, smiling and vocalizing. Even though long-term benefits of SDF positioning are yet to be investigated, our results, although based on pilot data, pleads in favor of a modification of current skin-to-skin contact practices with very preterm babies.

      Limitations

      This pilot study presents some limitations. Additionally, to the number of dyads included (N = 34) which in itself limits the range of our results, our study was conducted in one unique level 3 NICU. A random allocation of dyads in SSC positioning would have been more optimal and would have strengthened our study design, but it would have created a risk of practice contamination. The safe implementation of SDF positioning in NICU needs to be further examined by conducting a multicentric study based on randomized sampling and including a larger group of extremely preterm infants and/or unstable very preterm infants requiring endotracheal ventilation.

      Conclusion

      Very little is known about what happens between the very preterm and the parent during skin-to-skin contact, despite it being the only care entirely dedicated to the parent–infant relationship in NICUs. Seeing very preterm newborns open their eyes, attempt a smile or hearing their vocalizations are rare and short-lived moments. Yet they are essential moments, because they reveal the preterm as a fully-fledged individual, who communicates, despite immaturity and uncertain vital and developmental prognoses. They are also essential when infant and parents meet for they contribute to the creation of the first bonds.

      Our study showed that whatever the positioning considered, and although we only analyzed the first 5 min of SSC in a sample of 23 dyads, very preterm newborns were able to vocalize. Indeed, during SSC, i.e., in a moment dedicated to intimacy, the very preterm newborn was vocally present and provided opportunities to establish communication. Moreover, SDF positioning supported mothers’ vocal responsiveness to the vocal presence of their newborn, thus fostering the beginning of adapted and coordinated vocal responses, and feeding intersubjective intimacy.

      Providing early communicational pathways for parents and very preterm infants, as early as possible, enables initial interactive experiences, providing a less atypical, less dysregulated, and more consistent environment. Our pioneer data sheds light on the way a mother and her very preterm vocally meet, and constitutes a pilot step in the exploration of innate intersubjectivity in the context of very preterm birth. Future studies are needed to explore other ways by which the very preterm connects to his/her partner, thus creating a dyadic intersubjective space, i.e., vision, tonic postural dialogue, rhythm, touch, and olfaction.

      Data availability statement

      The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because it contains sociodemographic and medical information that cannot be shared consistently with GDPR requirements. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to devouche7@gmail.com.

      Ethics statement

      All mothers were offered to participate to the research study on a voluntary basis, within the first two days postpartum, and in all cases before the first SSC session. Every mother was informed of the research by a letter in the NICU. Mothers gave a written informed consent before participating. Written informed consent was obtained from the mothers for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article. An initial information-based meeting was organised prior to data collection. This research was approved by the French Local Ethics Consulting Committee for the Protection of Persons (IRB n°2015120001072).

      Author contributions

      AB and ED contributed to the conception and design of the study and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AB collected the data. ED performed the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      Funding

      This work was supported by four research grants by Institut Universitaire de France, Fondation Mustela, Fondation pour la Recherche en Psychomotricité et Maladies de Civilisation, and Fondation des Treilles. Baby wraps were provided by L’école à porter—JPMBB.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      We are grateful to the Chairs and staff of the Neonatal Medicine Department who facilitated access to the participants in this study. We extend our special thanks to all the families who agreed to take part in this research project.

      References Ainsworth M. D. S. Blehar M. C. Waters E. Wall S. N. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the strange Situation. London: PsychologyPress; (2015). Ajuriaguerra J. D. (1986). Organización neuropsicológica de algunas funciones: de los movimientos espontáneos al diálogo tónico-postural ya las formas precoces de comunicación. Revista Psicomotricidad. 23, 1734. Als H. (1982). Toward a synactive theory of development: promise for the assessment and support of infant individuality. Infant Ment. Health J. 3, 229243. Als H. Lester B. M. Tronick E. Z. Brazelton T. B. (1982). “Toward a research instrument for the assessment of preterm infants’ behavior (APIB)” in Theory and Research in Behavioral pediatrics (Springer, Boston, MA), 35132. Anderson C. Cacola P. (2017). Implications of preterm birth for maternal mental health and infant development. MCN: The American journal of maternal/child. Nursing 42, 108114. doi: 10.1097/NMC.0000000000000311 Anderzen-Carlsson A. Lamy Z. C. Tingvall M. Eriksson M. (2014). Parental experiences of providing skin-to-skin care to their newborn infant—part 2: a qualitative meta-synthesis. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well-Being 9:24907. doi: 10.3402/qhw.v9.24907, PMID: 25319747 Athanasopoulou E. Fox J. R. (2014). Effects of kangaroo mother care on maternal mood and interaction patterns between parents and their preterm, low birth weight infants: a systematic review. Infant Ment. Health J. 35, 245262. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21444, PMID: 25798479 Bateson M. C. (1979). “The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development,” in Before speech: The Beginning of human Communication. ed. Bullowa M. (London: Cambridge University Press), 6377. Beebe B. (2017). Daniel Stern: microanalysis and the empirical infant research foundations. Psychoanal. Inq. 37, 228241. Blomqvist Y. T. Ewald U. Gradin M. Nyqvist K. H. Rubertsson C. (2013). Initiation and extent of skin-to-skin care at two Swedish neonatal intensive care units. Acta Paediatr. 102, 2228. doi: 10.1111/apa.12056, PMID: 23072448 Boiteau C. Kokkinaki T. Sankey C. Buil A. Gratier M. Devouche E. (2021). Father–newborn vocal interaction: A contribution to the theory of innate intersubjectivity. Infant Child Dev.:e2259. doi: 10.1002/icd.2259 Brazelton T. B. Yogman M. W. Als H. Tronick E. (1979). “The infant as a focus for family reciprocity,” in The child and his Family. eds. Lewis M. Rosenblum L. A. (London: Plenum Press), 2943. Brazelton T. B. (1979). “Evidence of Communication during Neonatal Behavioral Assessment,” in Beforespeech: The beginning of human communication. ed. Bullowa M. (London: Cambridge University Press), 7999. Brazelton T. B. Nugent J. K. (1995). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. 3rd. Bruner J. S. (1977). “Early social interaction and language acquisition,” in Studies in mother-infant Interaction: The Loch Lomond Symposium. ed. Schaffer H. R. (London: Academic Press), 271289. Buil A. (2019). “Early psychomotor therapy: support for vulnerable babies and their families,” in Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology. eds. Apter G. Devouche E. Gratier M. (Springer) Buil A. Caeymaex L. Mero S. Sankey C. Apter G. Devouche E. (2019). Kangaroo supported diagonal flexion positioning: positive impact on maternal stress and postpartum depression risk and on skin-to-skin practice with very preterm infants. J. Neonatal Nurs. 25, 8692. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2018.10.006 Buil A. Carchon I. Apter G. Laborne F. X. Granier M. Devouche E. (2016). Kangaroo supported diagonal flexion positioning: new insights into skin-to-skin contact for communication between mothers and very preterm infants. Arch. Pediatr. 23, 913920. doi: 10.1016/j.arcped.2016.04.023, PMID: 27388909 Buil A. Da Costa S. M. Granger A. Devouche E. (2017a). Changer l’installation du soin peau-à-peau en néonatalogie pour une communication précoce de qualité. Enfance 4, 471476. doi: 10.3917/enf1.174.0471 Buil A. Fillon-Devys D. Granger A. Roger K. Thomas N. Apter G. . (2017b). Impact de l’installation en Flexion diagonale soutenue sur le maternage tactile spontané lors de la première séance de peau-à-peau en réanimation néonatale. Neuropsychiatr. Enfance Adolesc. 65, 289298. Buil A. Sankey C. Caeymaex L. Apter G. Gratier M. Devouche E. (2020). Fostering mother-very preterm infant communication during skin-to-skin contact through a modified positioning. Early Hum. Dev. 141:104939. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2019.104939, PMID: 29216238 Bullinger A. Goubet N. (1999). Le bébé prématuré, acteur de son développement. Enfance 52, 2732. doi: 10.3406/enfan.1999.3127 Cadwell K. Brimdyr K. Phillips R. (2018). Mapping, measuring, and analyzing the process of skin-to-skin contact and early breastfeeding in the first hour after birth. Breastfeed. Med. 13, 485492. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2018.0048, PMID: 30036081 Cambonie G. Muller J. Ehlinger V. Roy J. Guédeney A. Lebeaux C. . (2017). OLIMPE study writing group. Mother-infant interaction assessment at discharge and at 6 months in a French cohort of infants born very preterm: The OLIMPE study. PLoS One 12:e0188942. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188942, PMID: 29216238 Carvalho M. E. S. Justo J. M. Gratier M. Tomé T. Pereira E. Rodrigues H. (2019). Vocal responsiveness of preterm infants to maternal infant-directed speaking and singing during skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) in the NICU. Infant Behav. Dev. 57:101332 Casaer P. Postural Behaviour in Newborn Infants Clinics in Developmental Medicine. London: W. Spastic International Medical Publications; (1979). Caskey M. Stephen B. Tucker R. Vohr B. (2011). Importance of parent talk on the development of preterm infant vocalizations. Pediatrics 128, 910916. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0609, PMID: 22007020 Caskey M. Stephens B. Tucker R. Vohr B. (2014). Adult talk in the NICU with preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics 133, 578584. Chan G. J. Labar A. S. Wall S. Atun R. (2016). Kangaroo mother care: a systematic review of barriers and enablers. Bull. World Health Organ. 94:130. Charpak N. Gabriel Ruiz J. Zupan J. Cattaneo A. Figueroa Z. Tessier R. . (2005). Kangaroo mother care: 25 years after. Acta Paediatr. 94, 514522. Conde-Agudelo A. Belizán J. M. Díaz-Rossello J. L. (2011). Kangaroo mother care to reduce morbidity and mortality in low birthweight infants. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 16. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002771.pub4 Cong X. Ludington-Hoe S. M. Hussain N. Cusson R. M. Walsh S. Vazquez V. . (2015). Parental oxytocin responses during skin-to-skin contact in pre-term infants. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 401406. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.04.012, PMID: 25988992 De Paula Eduardo J. A. F. de Rezende M. G. Menezes P. R. Del-Ben C. M. (2019). Preterm birth as a risk factor for postpartum depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 259, 392403. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.069, PMID: 31470184 Devouche E. Buil A. (2019). “De l’état éveillé au sommeil. Comprendre les états de conscience du bébé,” in J. Le développement du bébé de la vie fœtale à la marche: Sensoriel-Psychomoteur-Cognitif-Affectif-Social. eds. Devouche E. Provasi (Elsevier Health Sciences) Devouche E. Gratier M. (2019). “The beginning of parent-infant communication” in Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology. Volume I: Infancy. eds. Apter G. Devouche E. Gratier M. (Cham: Springer), 2134. Doheny L. Morey J. A. Ringer S. A. Lahav A. (2012). Reduced frequency of apnea and bradycardia episodes caused by exposure to biological maternal sounds. Pediatr. Int. 54, e1e3. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2012.03575.x, PMID: 22507169 Dominguez S. Devouche E. Apter G. Gratier M. (2016). The roots of turn-taking in the neonatal period. Infant Child Dev. 25, 240255. Donnot J. Vauclair J. (2005). Biais de latéralité dans la façon de porter un très jeune enfant: une revue de la question. Neuropsychiatr. Enfance Adolesc. 53, 413425. doi: 10.1016/j.neurenf.2005.09.019 Feldman R. (2006). From biological rhythms to social rhythms: physiological precursors of mother-infant synchrony. Dev. Psychol. 42, 175188. Feldman R. (2015). Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 10071023. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000656, PMID: 26439059 Feldman R. Eidelman A. I. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 45, 274281. Feldman R. Eidelman A. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant–mother and infant–father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 290302. Feldman R. Eidelman A. I. Sirota L. Weller A. (2002). Comparaison of skin-to-skin (kangaroo) and traditional care: parenting outcomes et preterm infant developement. Pediatrics 110, 1626. Feldman R. Greenbaum C. W. Yirmiya N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Dev. Psychol. 35, 223231. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.223, PMID: 9923477 Feldman R. Magori-Cohen R. Galili G. Singer M. Louzon Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 569577. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.008, PMID: 21767879 Feldman R. Rosentha Z. Eidelman A. I. (2014). Maternal-preterm skin-to-skin contact enhances child physiologic organization and cognitive control across the first 10 years of life. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 5664. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.012, PMID: 24094511 Ferrari F. Bertoncelli N. Gallo C. Roversi M. F. Guerra M. P. Ranzi A. . (2007). Posture and movement in healthy preterm infants in supine position in and outside the nest. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 92, 386390. Ferrari G. A. Nicolini Y. Demuru E. Tosato C. Hussain M. Scesa E. . (2016). Ultrasonographic investigation ofhumanfetus responses to maternal communicative and non-communicative stimuli. Front. Psychol. 7:354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00354 Filippa M. Gratier M. Devouche E. Grandjean D. (2018). Changes in infant-directed speech and song are related to preterm infant facial expression in the neonatal intensive care unit. Interact. Stud. 19, 427444. Filippa M. Kuhn P. Westrup B. Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development. Cham: Springer; (2017). Filippa M. Lordier L. De Almeida J. S. Monaci M. G. Darque A. A. Grandjean D. . (2019). Early vocal contact and music in the NICU: new insights into preventive interventions. Pediatr. Res. 87, 249264. doi: 10.1038/s41390-019-0490-9, PMID: 31266053 Filippa M. Saliba S. Esseily R. Gratier M. Grandjean D. Kuhn P. (2021). Systematic review shows the benefits of involving the fathers of preterm infants in early interventions in neonatal intensive care units. Acta Paediatr. 110, 25092520. doi: 10.1111/apa.15961, PMID: 34053115 Forcada-Guex M. Borghini A. Pierrehumbert B. Ansermet F. Muller-Nix C. (2011). Prematurity, maternal posttraumatic stress and consequences on the mother–infant relationship. Early Hum. Dev. 87, 2126. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.09.006, PMID: 20951514 Foreman S. W. Thomas K. A. Blackburn S. T. (2008). Individual and gender differences matter in preterm infant state development. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 37, 657665. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2008.00292.x, PMID: 19012716 Franck L. S. O'Brien K. (2019). The evolution of family-centered care: From supporting parent-delivered interventions to a model of family integrated care. Birth Defects Res. 111, 10441059. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1521, PMID: 31115181 Gatta M. Miscioscia M. Svanellini L. Peraro C. Simonelli A. (2017). A psychological perspective on preterm children: the influence of contextual factors on quality of family interactions. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017. doi: 10.1155/2017/9152627 Gere S. Berhane Y. Worku A. (2021). Chest-to-Back skin-to-skin contact to regulate body temperature for low birth weight and/or preterm babies: A crossover randomized controlled clinical trial. Int. J. Pediatr. 2021 Gratier M. Devouche E. (2017). “The development of infant participation in communication,” in Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development. eds. Manuela F. Kuhn P. Westrup P. (Cham: Springer), 5569. Gratier M. Devouche E. Guellai B. Infanti R. Yilmaz E. Parlato-Oliveira E. (2015). Early development of turn-taking invocal interaction between mothers and infants. Front. Psychol. 6:1167. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167, PMID: 26388790 Grote N. K. Bridge J. A. Gavin A. R. Melville J. L. Iyengar S. Katon W. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 10121024. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.111, PMID: 20921117 Harding C. Levin A. Crossley S. L. Murphy R. Van den Engel-Hoek L. (2019). Effects of early communication intervention on speech and communication skills of preterm infants in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU): A systematic review. J. Neonatal Nurs. 25, 177188. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2019.04.004 Jaffe J. Beebe B. Feldstein S. Crown C. L. Jasnow M. D. Rochat P. . (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 66, 17. doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00137 Johnston C. Campbell-Yeo M. Disher T. Benoit B. Fernandes A. Streiner D. (2017). Skin‐to‐skin care for procedural pain in neonates. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008435.pub2 Kinreich S. Djalovski A. Kraus L. Louzoun Y. Feldman R. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions. Sci. Rep. 7:17060. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5, PMID: 29213107 Kisilevsky B. S. Hains S. M. J. Lee K. Xie X. Huang H. Ye H.-H. . (2003). Effects of experience on fetalvoicerecognition. Psychol. Sci. 14, 220224. Kristoffersen L. Støen R. Rygh H. Sognnæs M. Follestad T. Mohn H. S. . (2016). Early skin-to-skin contact or incubator for very preterm infants: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 17:593. doi: 10.1186/s13063-016-1730-5, PMID: 27955652 Lawn J. E. Mwansa-Kambafwile J. Horta B. L. Barros F. C. Cousens S. (2010). ‘Kangaroo mother care’ to prevent neonatal deaths due to preterm birth complications. Int. J. Epidemiol. 39, i144i154. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq031, PMID: 20348117 Lickliter R. (2000). Atypical perinatal sensory stimulation and early perceptual development: insights from developmental psychobiology. J. Perinatol. 20, S45S54. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7200450 Linnér A. Klemming S. Sundberg B. Lilliesköld S. Westrup B. Jonas W. . (2020). Immediate skin-to-skin contact is feasible for very preterm infants but thermal control remains a challenge. Acta Paediatr. 109, 697704. doi: 10.1111/apa.15062, PMID: 31618466 Ludington-Hoe S. M. Morgan K. Abouelfettoh A. (2008). A clinical guideline for implementation of kangaroo care with preterm infants of 30 or more weeks' postmenstrual age. Adv. Neonatal Care 8, S3S23. doi: 10.1097/01.ANC.0000324330.25734.b6 Maastrup R. Weis J. Engsig A. B. Johannsen K. L. Zoffmann V. (2018). Now she has become my daughter: parents’ early experiences of skin-to-skin contact with extremely preterm infants. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 32, 545553. doi: 10.1111/scs.12478, PMID: 28851054 Malatesta G. Marzoli D. Rapino M. Tommasi L. (2019). The left-cradling bias and its relationship with empathy and depression. Sci. Rep. 9, 19. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42539-6 Markova G. Nguyen T. Hoehl S. (2019). Neurobehavioral interpersonal synchrony in early development: The role of interactional rhythms. Front. Psychol. 10:2078. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02078 Marlier L. Schaal B. (2005). Human newborns prefer human milk: conspecific milk odor is attractive without postnatal exposure. Child Dev. 76, 155168. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00836.x, PMID: 15693764 Marx V. Nagy E. (2015). Fetalbehavioral responses to maternal voice and touch. PLoS One 10:e0129118. doi: 10.1371/journal.Pone.0129118, PMID: 26053388 Monaci M. G. Gratier M. Trevarthen C. Grandjean D. Kuhn P. Filippa M. (2021). Parental perception of vocal contact with preterm infants: communicative musicality in the neonatal intensive care unit. Children 8:513. doi: 10.3390/children8060513, PMID: 34204321 Montagna A. Nosarti C. (2016). Socio-emotional development following very preterm birth: pathways to psychopathology. Front. Psychol. 7:80. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00080 Moore E. R. Bergman N. Anderson G. C. Medley N. (2016). Early skin‐to‐skin contact for mothers and their healthy newborn infants. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003519.pub4 Mörelius E. Angelhoff C. Eriksson J. Olhager E. (2012). Time of initiation of skin-to-skin contact in extremely preterm infants in Sweden. Acta Paediatr. 101, 1418. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02398.x, PMID: 21732975 Mörelius E. Örtenstrand A. Theodorsson E. Frostell A. (2015). A randomised trial of continuous skin-to-skin contact after preterm birth and the effects on salivary cortisol, parental stress, depression, and breastfeeding. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 6370. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.12.005, PMID: 25545453 Mori R. Khanna R. Pledge D. Nakayama T. (2010). Meta-analysis of physiological effects of skin-to-skin contact for newborns and mothers. Pediatr. Int. 52, 161170. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2009.02909.x, PMID: 19519670 Muller-Nix C. Forcada-Guex M. Pierrehumbert B. Jaunin L. Borghini A. Ansermet F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother–child interactions. Early Hum. Dev. 79, 145158. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002 Nyqvist K. H. Anderson G. C. Bergman N. Cattaneo A. Charpak N. Davanzo R. . (2010). Towards universal kangaroo mother care: recommendations and report from the first European conference and seventh international workshop on kangaroo mother care. Acta Paediatr. 99, 820826. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01787.x, PMID: 20219044 Pandya D. Kartikeswar G. A. P. Patwardhan G. Kadam S. Pandit A. Patole S. (2021). Effect of early kangaroo mother care on time to full feeds in preterm infants-A prospective cohort study. Early Hum. Dev. 154:105312. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105312, PMID: 33517173 Papoušek H. Papoušek M. (2002). “Intuitive parenting,” in Handbook of Parenting Volume 2 Biology and Ecology of Parenting. ed. Bornstein M. H. (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 183203. Philbin K. M. (2017). “The sound environments and auditory perceptions of the fetus and preterm newborn” in Early Vocal Contact and Preterm Infant Brain Development: Bridging the Gaps Between Research and Practice. eds. Filippa M. Westrup B. Kuhn P. (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 91111. Pierrat V. Marchand-Martin L. Marret S. Arnaud C. Benhammou V. Cambonie G. . (2021). Neurodevelopmental outcomes at age 5 among children born preterm: EPIPAGE-2 cohort study. BMJ 373. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n741, PMID: 33910920 Pinéda R. Durant P. Mathur A. Inder T. Wallendorf M. Schlaggar B. L. (2017a). Auditory exposure in the neonatal intensive care unit: room type and other predictors. J. Pediatr. 183, 5666.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.12.072, PMID: 28189301 Pinéda R. Guth R. Herring A. Reynolds L. Oberle S. Smith J. (2017b). Enhancing sensory experiences for very preterm infants in the NICU: an integrative review. J. Perinatol. 37, 323332. doi: 10.1038/jp.2016.179, PMID: 28189301 Pino O. (2016). Fetal memory: The effects of prenatal auditory experience on human development. BAOJ Med. Nurs. 2:20. Piontelli A. (2010). Development of Normal fetal Movements: The first 25 weeks of Gestation. Milan: Springer. Provasi J. (2019). “Parent-preterm interaction,” in Early Interaction and Developmental Psychopathology, Volume I: Infancy. eds. Apter G. Devouche E. Gratier M. (Cham: Springer), 123149. Provasi J. Anderson D. I. Barbu-Roth M. (2014). Rhythm perception, production, and synchronization during the perinatal period. Front. Psychol. 5:1048. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01048 Provasi J. Blanc L. Carchon I. (2021). The importance of rhythmic stimulation for preterm infants in the NICU. Children 8:660. doi: 10.3390/children8080660Academic, PMID: 34438551 Raiskila S. Axelin A. Toome L. Caballero S. Tandberg B. S. Montirosso R. . (2017). Parents' presence and parent–infant closeness in 11 neonatal intensive care units in six European countries vary between and within the countries. Acta Paediatr. 106, 878888. doi: 10.1111/apa.13798, PMID: 28235152 Reissland N. Kisilevsky B. (Eds.). (2015). Fetal Development: Research on brain and Behavior, Environmental Influences, Andemerging Technologies. London: Springer Verlag. Roué J. M. Kuhn P. Maestro M. L. Maastrup R. A. Mitanchez D. Westrup B. . (2017). Eight principles for patient-centred and family-centred care for newborns in the neonatal intensive care unit. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 102, F364F368. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2016-312180 Saliba S. Esseily R. Filippa M. Gratier M. Grandjean D. (2020). Changes in the vocal qualities of mothers and fathers are related to preterm infant’s behavioural states. Acta Paediatr. 109, 22712277. doi: 10.1111/apa.15238, PMID: 32073679 Segre L. S. McCabe J. E. Chuffo-Siewert R. O’Hara M. W. (2014). Depression and anxiety symptoms in mothers of newborns hospitalized on the neonatal intensive care unit. Nurs. Res. 63, 320332. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0000000000000039, PMID: 25171558 Seidman G. Unnikrishnan S. Kenny E. Myslinski S. Cairns-Smith S. Mulligan B. . (2015). Barriers and enablers of kangaroo mother care practice: a systematic review. PLoS One 10:e0125643. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125643, PMID: 25993306 Skene C. Gerrish K. Price F. Pilling E. Bayliss P. Gillespie S. (2019). Developing family-centred care in a neonatal intensive care unit: an action research study. Intensive Crit. Care Nurs. 50, 5462. doi: 10.1016/j.iccn.2018.05.006, PMID: 29937077 Stern D. N.. The Interpersonal World of the infant: a view from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books; (1985). Tallandini M. A. Scalembra C. (2006). Kangaroo mother care and mother-preterm infant dyadic interaction. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 251275. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20091, PMID: 28640473 Thomson G. Moran V. H. Axelin A. Dykes F. Flacking R. (2013). Integrating a sense of coherence into the neonatal environment. BMC Paediatr. 13:84. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-13-84, PMID: 23697687 Trevarthen C. (1977). “Descriptive analysis of infant communicative behavior,” in Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction. ed. H. Schaffer (New York: Academic Press), 227270. Trevarthen C. (1993). “The self born in Intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating” in The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge. ed. Neisser U.. ed. ed (New York: Cambridge University Press), 121173. Trevarthen C. (1998). “The concept of foundations of infant intersubjectivity,” in Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny. ed. Bråten S. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 1547. Trevarthen C. (2011). What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being. Infant Child Dev. 20, 119135. doi: 10.1002/icd.689 Trevarthen C. (2012). Embodied human intersubjectivity: imaginative agency, to share meaning. J. Cogn. Semiot. 4, 656. doi: 10.1515/cogsem.2012.4.1.6 Trevarthen C. (2016). The spiritual nature of the infant self: An imaginative actor in relations of affection. J. Conscious. Stud. 23, 258282. Tronick E. D. Als H. Brazelton T. B. (1977). Mutuality in mother infant interaction. J. Commun. 27, 7479. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1977.tb01829.x Vaivre-Douret L. Ennouri K. Jrad I. Garrec C. Papiernik E. (2004). Effect of positioning on the incidence of abnormalities of muscle tone in low-risk, preterm infants. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 8, 2134. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2003.10.001 Varendi H. Porter R. H. Winberg J. (1997). Natural odour preferences of newborn infants change over time. Acta Paediatr. 86, 985990. Voegtline K. M. Costigan K. A. Pater H. DiPetro J. A. (2013). Near-term fetal response to maternal spoken voice. Infant Behav. Dev. 36, 526533. World Health Organization. Kangaroo Mother Care: A Practical Guide. Geneva: Departement of Reproductive Health and Research, WHO; (2003). Zelkowitz P. Papageorgiou A. Bardin C. Wang T. (2009). Persistent maternal anxiety affects the interaction between mothers and their very low birthweight children at 24 months. Early Hum. Dev. 85, 5158. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.06.010, PMID: 18632229 Zhao X. H. Zhang Z. H. (2020). Risk factors for postpartum depression: An evidence-based systematic review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Asian J. Psychiatr. 53:102353. Abbreviations APIB

      Assessment of preterm infants’ behavior scale

      IFCDC

      Infant- and Family-Centered Developmental Care

      GA

      Gestational age

      NICU

      Neonatal intensive care unit

      SDF

      Supported diagonal flexion

      SSC

      Skin-to-skin contact

      1http://www.noldus.com

      2https://audacity.sourceforge.net/

      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.gxkgwx.com.cn
      mbsale.com.cn
      heyin.net.cn
      www.lygckjd.com.cn
      www.lykxgm.org.cn
      www.wbbdtu.com.cn
      okpktg.com.cn
      shanglaowu.com.cn
      nmxdqm.com.cn
      www.v0kwfy.net.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p