Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.763210 Psychology Original Research Development and Validation of the Teen Dating Aggression Measure Among Canadian Youth Persram Ryan J. 1 * Wong Tracy K. Y. 1 Vargas-Madriz Luis Francisco 1 Konishi Chiaki 1 Dryburgh Nicole S. J. 2 Dirks Melanie A. 2 Martin-Storey Alexa 3 Craig Wendy 4 1Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada 2Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada 3Department of Psychoeducation, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada 4Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada

Edited by: Ilaria Grazzani, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by: Emanuela Confalonieri, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy; Jennifer Yahner, Urban Institute, United States

*Correspondence: Ryan J. Persram, ryan.persram@mail.mcgill.ca

This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

14 12 2021 2021 12 763210 23 08 2021 22 11 2021 Copyright © 2021 Persram, Wong, Vargas-Madriz, Konishi, Dryburgh, Dirks, Martin-Storey and Craig. 2021 Persram, Wong, Vargas-Madriz, Konishi, Dryburgh, Dirks, Martin-Storey and Craig

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Teen dating violence (TDV) victimization is a traumatic experience that can have adverse consequences for adolescents. Current measures that assess TDV do not fully distinguish between psychological and relational forms of aggression, nor do they capture aggressive acts that are common within adolescent relationships. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Teen Dating Aggression Measure (TeDAM) using a sample of 730 Canadian adolescents (M = 15.88 years, SD = 1.23). The measure is an expansion of the Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships Inventory and includes items that describe other forms of violence such as coercion and control, along with more traditional indicators of dating violence (e.g., sexual aggression). Factor analyses yielded three factors, namely psychological aggression, sexual and physical aggression, and relational aggression, which were correlated with more frequent cannabis and alcohol use as well as rape myth acceptance. These results provide initial support for the utility of the TeDAM for assessing TDV with adolescents.

dating violence adolescents aggression teen dating violence adolescent dating aggression Public Health Agency of Canada10.13039/100011094

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Adolescence represents a developmental period marked by increased interest and involvement in extrafamilial romantic relationships with same or other-gender partners (Collins et al., 2009; Furman and Collins, 2009). Although teen dating is typically characterized by affectionate and anticipated or actual sexual behaviors, violence, and aggression can also occur (Wolfe et al., 2001; Collins et al., 2009). This type of aggression and violence is commonly referred to as teen dating violence (TDV) and is a prevalent social problem. In a recent study using the Health-Behavior in School-Aged Children (HBSC) survey, Exner-Cortens et al. (2021) found that one in three Canadian teenagers had some experience with dating violence in the past year. In particular, adolescents reported experiencing psychological aggression the most (27.8%), followed by cyber aggression (17.5%) and physical aggression (11.8%) (Exner-Cortens et al., 2021). Moreover, national statistics indicate that 20% of police-reported incidences included some form of dating violence among 15–24-year-olds (Statistics Canada, 2018). Finally, youth victims of TDV are more likely to experience adverse outcomes such as increased depression and anxiety (Garthe et al., 2021), engage in more substance abuse and risky sexual behavior (Alleyne et al., 2011), and are at greater risk for future intimate partner violence (IPV) (Exner-Cortens et al., 2013). Despite being a public health concern, current measures of TDV are limited for various conceptual and methodological reasons. To address these limitations, the purpose of this study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of a more expansive measure that evaluates TDV victimization more broadly.

      Teen dating violence is conceptually similar to IPV observed among adult populations (Espelage et al., 2020). For example, TDV incorporates actions and behaviors that are intended to hurt or manipulate a partner’s social relationships and can be manifested in different ways (Linder et al., 2002; Breiding et al., 2015; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). Instruments that assess TDV typically measure three types: (a) physical, (b) sexual, and (c) psychological violence (Smith et al., 2015; Exner-Cortens et al., 2016a,b). Physical dating violence refers to a range of behaviors in which an individual experiences physical injury such as hitting and kicking (Breiding et al., 2015). Sexual dating violence refers to behaviors in which a partner forces or attempts to force the partner to engage in physical or non-physical sexual acts (Breiding et al., 2015). Psychological dating violence denotes behaviors meant to humiliate or control the partner (e.g., name calling, restricting access to friends) (Breiding et al., 2015). However, what is unique to TDV is that it can differ based on the types of aggressive behaviors and actions that often take place amongst adolescent peer groups that may not be particularly salient among adults.

      In general, victimization resulting from TDV is linked to various negative outcomes. For example, experiencing physical or verbal TDV is associated with recent alcohol and marijuana use (e.g., Parker et al., 2016). Moreover, adolescents who have been exposed to sexual assault have differential perceptions of rape myth acceptance, which refers to a stigmatic set of beliefs that victims are at fault for their assault or exposure to dating violence (e.g., Edwards et al., 2011; Dworkin et al., 2017). Subscales of rape myth acceptance include rape denial, which refers to attitudes reflective of victim-blaming or not believing rape victims, and traditional gender expectations, which represent typical roles of men and women in relationships (Dworkin et al., 2017). In Dworkin et al. (2017), positive associations between the two rape myth acceptance subscales and each of depressed mood and alcohol use among a sample of adolescents were evident. More broadly, positive associations were observed between rape myth acceptance as an overall measure and sexual dating aggression (Reyes and Foshee, 2013). Thus, the consequences of being victimized by a romantic partner has an effect on the ways in which adolescents might cope with and perceive such experiences.

      Currently, there are several measures that assess TDV, each of which evaluate different aspects of teen dating perpetration and victimization (see Exner-Cortens et al., 2016a,b). A comprehensive review by Exner-Cortens et al. (2016a,b) highlights dating violence behaviors and attitudes as distinct themes for measuring TDV. Despite the breadth of these measures, there are two main limitations to note among them. First, many of the measures are adapted from adult scales that do not fully capture the adolescent experience (e.g., Conflict Tactics Scale; see Straus, 1979; Cascardi et al., 1999). Although adult-based dating violence measures could be adapted for use with adolescents, the nature of adapting remains problematic. From a developmental perspective, adolescent and adult intimate relationships differ in important ways (Knox et al., 2009). For example, adolescents are less likely to live with and be economically bound to their partner (Knox et al., 2009). Moreover, adolescents are also more likely to disclose victimization experiences with peers than adults, who are more likely to seek help from professionals (Knox et al., 2009). Furthermore, adolescents are likely to remain in contact with the partner because they often go to the same school or even attend the same class (Knox et al., 2009). Due to these differences, adult-based dating violence measures might not adequately capture the uniqueness of TDV. For example, including items that capture economic threats (e.g., removal of economic support) or failing to assess relational aggression (e.g., spreading rumors to friends) would not adequately capture adolescent developmental. As such, we argue that they do not fully capture the everyday aggressive acts that might be more common to the adolescent dating experience. This is especially the case for relationally and psychologically aggressive acts, as they are common during adolescence and occur in both dating and peer relationships (e.g., Linder et al., 2002; Ellis et al., 2009).

      In adolescence, relationally and psychologically aggressive acts are common among teen dating partners relative to physical aggression (e.g., Morelli et al., 2018; Dosil et al., 2020; Asghari et al., 2021). In addition, psychological aggression appears to occur more frequently than relational aggression in the context of adolescent romantic relationships relative to those between adults (Morelli et al., 2018; Dosil et al., 2020). Similar to the aforementioned TDV experiences, adolescent victimization based on psychological or relational acts are associated with greater psychological distress in romantic relationships (Jouriles et al., 2009; Goncy et al., 2017), increased emotional and behavioral problems (e.g., Leadbeater et al., 2008), and alcohol use (Schad et al., 2008). As such, the inclusion of aggressive acts that adolescents commonly see in their romantic partners beyond what has been developed for adults is necessary.

      Second, adolescent-based measures of TDV tend to have poor psychometric properties. For example, the Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships Inventory (CADRI) is a widely used instrument to measure teen dating perpetration and victimization and has been validated in many countries, including Canada, the United States, Spain, Mexico, among others (Wolfe et al., 2001; Smith et al., 2015; Exner-Cortens et al., 2016a,b). In terms of its factor structure, the CADRI includes five first-order factors, including threatening behavior, physical abuse, relational aggression, verbal emotional abuse, and sexual abuse, each of load onto a second-order latent factor called abuse. Moreover, internal consistency and test-retest reliability were adequate across adolescent sex and grade (>0.70) for verbal/emotional and physical abuse, but weaker for threatening behaviors (0.54–0.73), relational aggression (0.16–0.69), and sexual abuse (0.36–0.59) (Wolfe et al., 2001). Subsequent validation studies offered only partial psychometric evidence for the CADRI. For example, Fernández-Fuertes et al. (2006) assessed the five-factor model for both TDV perpetration and victimization with Spanish adolescents using exploratory factor analysis (EFA) instead of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Hokoda et al. (2006) only considered the reliability of CADRI for both TDV perpetration and victimization with Mexican youth. Furthermore, although the short form of CADRI (CADRI-S) has demonstrated validity and reliability among general and high-risk adolescents, it has lower sensitivity compared with the full CADRI (Fernández-González et al., 2012). Importantly, the CADRI-S was assessed only from the perpetuator’s perspective (Fernández-González et al., 2012). Thus, the extent to which CADRI or CADRI-S adequately measures TDV victimization experiences remains unclear.

      The Measure of Adolescent Relationship Harassment and Abuse (MARSHA) is a recent and comprehensive instrument that aims to evaluate different aspects of TDV perpetration (i.e., social control, physical abuse, sexual abuse, isolation, cyber control, intimidation) and victimization (i.e., privacy control, social control, physical abuse, sexual abuse, and intimidation (Rothman et al., 2020, 2021). This measure was initially developed from focus groups and interviews with a group of adolescents and young adults (11–20 years old, Mage = 16.4) (Rothman et al., 2020), and its reliability and validity were evaluated with a culturally diverse sample (11–20 years old) that has a mean age of 18 (Rothman et al., 2021). Given that the validation group consisted mainly of older adolescents and young adults, the extent to which MARSHA adequately captures TDV victimization experiences during the period of adolescence is inconclusive.

      Previous reviews have addressed several gaps in extant TDV measurements (Smith et al., 2015; Exner-Cortens, 2018). For example, items that measure sexual violence are often neglected or limited, possibly because of pressure from schools to avoid sexual related topics (Smith et al., 2015; Exner-Cortens, 2018). Moreover, psychological aggression (e.g., insulting) is often measured in a way that parallels physical aggression, which limits understanding on the nuances that distinguish the two constructs (Follingstad, 2007). To address these gaps, this study validated a comprehensive TDV victimization measure that was extended from the CADRI measure. This measure considers not only fundamental TDV constructs (i.e., physical abuse, psychological abuse, and sexual abuse), but also those that have not been adequately evaluated, such as relational forms of violence and aggression. The validation of the new measure was conducted in Canada, where TDV remains an alarming significant public health problem among adolescents (Shaffer et al., 2021).

      The present study had three main objectives. First, we sought to extend the CADRI measure to include typical everyday actions and behaviors that could be experienced by adolescents who have been in a romantic relationship. Our second objective was to replicate the multidimensionality of TDV as observed in the other measures using exploratory and confirmatory factor analytical approaches. Finally, we aimed to establish concurrent validity with this measure using participants’ ratings of alcohol overconsumption, cannabis use, and rape myth acceptance views as outcome variables.

      Materials and Methods Participants

      A total of 730 adolescents between grades 7 and 12 were recruited from high schools in three provinces (i.e., Quebec, Ontario, and Manitoba) in Canada (Mage = 15.88 years, SD = 1.23). Twelve schools participated in the study, four of which were from Quebec, six were from Ontario, and two from Manitoba. Demographic information for the full sample is presented in Table 1. The majority of participants identified their gender as female, followed by male, and non-binary. Half of the sample also identified their ethnicity as White/European (50.1%), and most of the participants did not have previous experiences with relationship violence (90.9%).

      Demographic information of samples.

      Sample 1 Sample 2 Total sample
      n 353 377 730
      Age M (SD) 15.89 (1.23) 15.89 (1.29) 15.89 (1.26)
      Gender
      Woman 63.7% 62.3% 63.0%
      Man 34.8% 34.7% 34.8%
      Non-Binary 0.6% 1.6% 1.1%
      No answer 0.8% 1.3% 1.1%
      Grade
      7 0% 0.3% 0.1%
      8 0% 0.8% 0.4%
      9 26.9% 26.5% 26.7%
      10 27.5% 26.3% 26.8%
      11 26.9% 26.8% 26.8%
      12 17.6% 17.5% 17.5%
      No answer 1.1% 1.9% 1.5%
      Ethnicity
      African/Caribbean 8.2% 11.9% 10.1%
      East Asian 6.5% 4.0% 5.2%
      First Nations 4.8% 6.4% 5.6%
      Inuit 0% 0.5% 0.3%
      Latin American 1.4% 2.4% 1.9%
      Métis 2.0% 3.2% 2.6%
      Middle Eastern/West Asian 3.4% 3.7% 3.6%
      South Asian 6.2% 5.8% 6.0%
      Southeast Asian 12.5% 15.9% 14.2%
      White/European 50.1% 48.0% 49.0%
      Different 6.2% 6.9% 6.6%
      No answer 14.4% 12.2% 13.3%
      Experiences with relationship violence
      Yes 7.6% 9.3% 8.5%
      No 90.9% 87.8% 89.3%
      No answer 1.4% 2.9% 2.2%

      The percentage of ethnicity is greater than 100% because participants could identify with more than one ethnic group.

      Procedure

      Ethical approval was obtained from each of the relevant research ethics bodies at the universities and local school boards. The research team first visited each participating school to explain the purpose of the study and administer consent forms. Once a signed guardian consent form was returned, students were asked to provide assent to participate in the study. During the study, participants responded to various questionnaires that lasted approximately 1 h.

      Measures Teen Dating Aggression Measure

      We modified the original CADRI to capture adolescent dating violence more adequately and to expand upon the types of violence assessed. Our modification included combining the two items assessing sexual violence to address potential concerns by schools and their respective boards, i.e., “Forced me to have sex with them when I didn’t want to” and “Touched me sexually when I didn’t want them to” were combined into one broad item: “Forced me to do something sexual that I didn’t want to do.” After the original CADRI items, 20 new statements were added that describe additional forms of violence such as coercion (e.g., “Kept pressuring me to do something even after I made it clear that I did not want to”) and control (e.g., “Made me let them read my emails or texts when I didn’t want them to”). For each of the 44 items on the adapted scale, participants rated how often the behavior occurred with a dating partner over the past three months using a Likert scale from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“most days of the week”).

      Rape Myth Acceptance

      Participants were asked to rate 10 items that assessed how much they agreed with statements regarding rape myths (RMA) on 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“Strongly Disagree”) to 4 (“strongly agree”). Items were adapted from established scales of this construct (e.g., Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale; Payne et al., 1999). Consistent with Dworkin et al. (2017), two scores were computed. The first reflected a measure of traditional gender expectations (e.g., “Girls should have sex with the guy they are dating when he wants”) while the second measured rape denial (e.g., “If a girl is sexually assaulted while drunk, she is to blame”). Items were summed for each score and higher scores indicated greater endorsement of RMA. Cronbach’s alpha for traditional gender expectations (0.81) and rape denial (0.73) were high across both samples.

      Cannabis and Alcohol Consumption

      Participants were asked to assess how often they used cannabis or marijuana using a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (“Never”) to 6 (“About 6 or 7 times a week”) (M = 1.48, SD = 1.06). Similarly, alcohol overconsumption in the past month (i.e., drinking to the point of drunkenness) was rated on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging between 1 (“Never”) and 6 (“5 or more times”) (M = 1.22, SD = 0.64).

      Plan of Analysis

      The analytical plan followed five steps. First, we split the sample using complex sampling procedures (described below). Second, descriptive correlations and inter-item correlations were computed in SPSS to evaluate normality of the data. Third, an EFA of the first sample was conducted. Fourth, CFA to verify the factor structure that was derived from the EFA with the second sample. Finally, bivariate correlations were conducted to examine the associations among the scales and with adolescent rape myth acceptance, and cannabis and alcohol consumption. The EFA and CFA were analyzed with Mplus 8 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998-2017). To evaluate the fit of the models, fit indices suggested by Hu and Bentler (1999) and Kline (2016), which includes the Comparative Fit Index (CFI; values of at least 0.90), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI; values of at least 0.95), the Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (RMSEA; values no greater than 0.08), and the Weighted Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (WRMR; values no greater than 1.00) were used.

      Complex Sampling

      In order to examine the factor structure and provide validation, complex random sampling in SPSS was used to split the main sample into two groups. Three strata were used to randomly assign participants into one of the two samples: (a) gender identity, (b) grade level, and (c) previous experience with relationship violence. Sample 1 (S1) was comprised of 353 participants (63.7% female) and Sample 2 (S2) included 377 participants (62.3% female).

      Results Descriptive Statistics

      Descriptive statistics including skewness and kurtosis were calculated for each item in both samples (see Table 2). In general, each of the items did not have a mean score above 2 but had skewness and kurtosis values that exceeded |1.50| (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013). Given that these items were rarely endorsed by the participants, the data were treated as categorical rather than continuous, but maintained the original Likert scale options that were originally posed. Moreover, we conducted a missing values analysis and found that none of the items had a proportion of missing values that exceeded 5% (between 2.10 and 3.00%).

      Descriptive statistics of items.

      Sample 1
      Sample 2
      Item M (SD) Skewness Kurtosis M (SD) Skewness Kurtosis
      1. Tried to turn my friends against me. 1.14 (0.52) 4.827 26.266 1.08 (0.41) 6.704 52.934
      2. Said or did something just to make me feel jealous. 1.47 (0.93) 2.280 4.754 1.30 (0.75) 3.097 10.439
      3. Destroyed or threatened to destroy something I valued. 1.06 (0.33) 7.601 71.641 1.08 (0.46) 6.952 51.593
      4. Brought up something bad I had done in the past. 1.37 (0.81) 2.605 7.068 1.32 (0.82) 2.982 8.684
      5. Threw something at me. 1.06 (0.29) 4.925 25.558 1.06 (0.39) 8.034 69.774
      6. Said or did something just to make me angry. 1.43 (0.88) 2.249 4.582 1.30 (0.79) 3.210 10.647
      7. Spoke to me in a hostile or mean tone of voice. 1.31 (0.79) 2.963 8.701 1.23 (0.73) 3.869 15.505
      8. Forced me to do something sexual that I didn’t want to. 1.10 (0.48) 5.798 37.403 1.11 (0.48) 5.903 39.534
      9. Threatened me to get me to do something sexual with him/her. 1.06 (0.38) 8.290 75.200 1.03 (0.27) 9.238 89.141
      10. Insulted me. 1.31 (0.74) 2.879 8.722 1.24 (0.71) 3.390 11.834
      11. Kissed me when I didn’t want him/her to. 1.12 (0.52) 5.028 27.856 1.11 (0.50) 5.357 31.206
      12. Said things to my friends about me to turn them against me. 1.10 (0.48) 5.628 35.374 1.07 (0.36) 6.800 55.719
      13. Ridiculed or made fun of me in front of other people. 1.15 (0.50) 4.269 20.880 1.15 (0.58) 5.007 27.423
      14. Kept track of who I was with and where I was. 1.46 (1.03) 2.419 4.908 1.32 (0.91) 3.244 9.749
      15. Blamed me for a problem or fight we were having. 1.39 (0.90) 2.784 7.463 1.33 (0.84) 3.139 9.986
      16. Kicked, hit, or punched me. 1.06 (0.33) 7.636 72.293 1.04 (0.27) 8.701 83.428
      17. Accused me of flirting with someone else. 1.38 (0.89) 2.768 7.410 1.27 (0.76) 3.393 11.855
      18. Tried to frighten me on purpose. 1.11 (0.46) 5.005 28.453 1.10 (0.48) 5.724 35.086
      19. Slapped me or pulled my hair. 1.10 (0.43) 6.212 46.008 1.06 (0.35) 6.710 47.969
      20. Threatened to hurt me. 1.04 (0.26) 7.739 68.247 1.04 (0.32) 9.080 92.644
      21. Threatened to break up with me or end our friendship. 1.15 (0.52) 4.866 28.858 1.16 (0.64) 4.757 23.674
      22. Threatened to hit or throw something at me. 1.03 (0.25) 10.331 115.005 1.03 (0.27) 10.823 135.353
      23. Pushed, shoved, grabbed, or shook me. 1.07 (0.32) 6.146 44.956 1.08 (0.38) 5.768 41.353
      24. Spread rumors about me. 1.11 (0.51) 5.754 35.926 1.11 (0.43) 4.357 19.650
      25. Screamed or yelled at me. 1.18 (0.60) 4.298 21.027 1.16 (0.60) 4.648 23.063
      26. Said mean things to me. 1.31 (0.75) 2.893 8.835 1.27 (0.77) 3.617 13.527
      27. Left me out of an activity or a social group on purpose. 1.11 (0.46) 5.836 39.436 1.12 (0.51) 5.596 34.564
      28. Told me that he/she would break up with me or end our friendship if I did not do something he/she wanted. 1.11 (0.53) 5.827 36.527 1.16 (0.60) 5.407 29.007
      29. Said means things about me to other people. 1.12 (0.48) 5.401 34.067 1.27 (0.77) 4.386 20.275
      30. Talked about how other people were better or more fun than me. 1.15 (0.58) 4.911 26.171 1.14 (0.57) 4.737 24.246
      31. Told me that other people didn’t like me. 1.16 (0.59) 4.458 21.801 1.13 (0.51) 4.749 25.825
      32. Told me that I was not a good boyfriend/girlfriend or friend. 1.13 (0.51) 5.025 29.018 1.16 (0.62) 4.759 23.904
      33. Gave me the silent treatment. 1.35 (0.76) 2.489 6.015 1.31 (0.78) 3.051 9.504
      34. Got upset when I spent time with other people. 1.48 (1.01) 2.280 4.387 1.31 (0.82) 3.208 10.342
      35. Said mean things to me about someone else who is important to me. 1.25 (0.73) 3.393 11.941 1.22 (0.72) 3.958 16.238
      36. Got upset when I did really well on something. 1.09 (0.43) 6.006 39.999 1.05 (0.35) 9.136 95.263
      37. Told me that I needed to spend more time with him/her. 1.47 (1.04) 2.321 4.374 1.33 (0.81) 3.023 9.474
      38. Made me let them read my e-mails or texts when I didn’t want them to. 1.18 (0.65) 4.132 17.189 1.11 (0.56) 5.937 36.925
      39. Made me do something I really didn’t want to do. 1.13 (0.55) 5.237 30.363 1.11 (0.52) 5.966 38.681
      40. Was mean to me or insulted me to get me to do something for him/her. 1.10 (0.49) 5.958 38.831 1.09 (0.51) 6.746 47.175
      41. Got mad at me when I said “no” to him/her about something. 1.25 (0.75) 3.525 12.582 1.20 (0.64) 4.214 19.880
      42. Threatened me to try to get me to do something he/she wanted me to do. 1.10 (0.52) 6.180 40.223 1.07 (0.42) 7.353 58.502
      43. Insulted me or said mean things to me when I said “no” to him/her about doing something. 1.14 (0.61) 5.129 27.284 1.09 (0.51) 6.593 46.002
      44. Kept pressuring me to do something even after I made it clear that I did not want to. 1.22 (0.75) 3.762 13.993 1.17 (0.65) 4.734 23.186
      Exploratory Factor Analysis

      An EFA was conducted on the first sample using the weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator and the geomin oblique rotation (epsilon = 0.50). Using the eigenvalue-greater-than-1 rule (Kaiser, 1960), results from this analysis suggest a 7-factor model (see Table 3). However, the increase in number of factors could also result in multiple cross-loadings, therefore, we also examined the changes in model fit to determine which factor structure best fit the data. Specifically, we calculated the change (Δ) in RMSEA, CFI, TLI, and SRMR by comparing each model with the model that preceded it. For example, the change in fit for a 2-factor solution was calculated by taking the difference between its fit and the fit from the 1-factor solution. In line with empirical suggestions for factor retention (e.g., Clark and Bowles, 2018), ΔCFI and ΔTLI improvements of at least 0.01 were considered in determining the final factor structure.

      Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) model fit indices for the factor structure of the Teen Dating Aggression Measure (TeDAM).

      Chi-Square test of model fit
      Solution Eigenvalue Δ χ2 df p RMSEA Δ CFI Δ TLI Δ SRMR Δ
      1-Factor 25.46 0.71 1109.419 740 <0.001 0.038 0.972 0.971 0.112
      2-Factor 3.051 0.08 897.463 701 <0.001 0.028 0.010 0.985 0.013 0.984 0.013 0.082 0.03
      3-Factor 2.052 0.06 774.471 663 0.002 0.022 0.006 0.992 0.007 0.99 0.006 0.068 0.014
      4-Factor 1.625 0.05 697.42 626 0.025 0.018 0.004 0.995 0.003 0.993 0.003 0.057 0.011
      5-Factor 1.124 0.03 638.449 590 0.082 0.015 0.003 0.996 0.001 0.995 0.002 0.052 0.005
      6-Factor 1.064 0.03 579.466 555 0.229 0.011 0.004 0.998 0.002 0.997 0.002 0.047 0.005
      7-Factor 0.899 0.02 530.392 521 0.378 0.007 0.004 0.999 0.001 0.999 0.002 0.043 0.004

      Analysis was performed on Sample 1 and items 13, 18, 28, and 43 were removed. All chi-square tests were statistically significant, p < 0.05. Δ represents the change between for each respective fit index from the current model and a previous model.

      Upon examination of change in model fit as well as considering the overall conceptualization of the factors, a 3-factor solution appeared to be the best fitting model (see Table 3). In this model, the change in model fit from a 2-factor solution to a 3-factor solution produced ΔRMSEA = 0.02, ΔCFI = 0.01, ΔTLI = 0.01, and ΔSRMR = 0.01. The change in the subsequent models did not necessarily improve the overall model fit. From there, the factor loadings of the items were evaluated; items that cross-loaded onto more than one factor were dropped if the standardized loading was < 0.32. The correlations between each of the three factors (F1, F2, F3) were statistically significant (p < 0.05): F1-F2 = 0.46, F1-F3 = 0.52, F2 and F3 = 0.50.

      Further investigation of the items showed that three items closely cross-loaded (difference between 0.02 and 0.06) onto two factors and did not quantitatively or conceptually fit with the factor in which it strongly loaded (discussed next). The three items were: (a) “Ridiculed or made fun of me in front of other people”; (b) “Told me that he/she would break up with me if I did not do something he/she wanted”; and (c) “Insulted me or said mean things to me when I said ‘no’ to him/her about doing something.” Thus, we removed the items and reanalyzed the EFA, which still supported a 3-factor solution. Table 3 provides fit indices and change (Δ) in model fit between each of the solutions. In this final model, there were fewer cross-loadings; when they did occur, there was discussion of the item in question and consensus was achieved if the authors agreed that it conceptually fit with the factor to which it was primarily loaded. The three factors consisted of constructs related to: (a) psychological aggression, (b) physical, sexual aggression, and (c) relational aggression. Table 4 provides the standardized loadings for each of the three factors.

      Standardized factor loadings and descriptive statistics for the 3-factor solution of the Teen Dating Aggression Measure (TeDAM).

      3-Factor solution
      Item F1 F2 F3
      F1: Psychological aggression
      1. Tried to turn my friends against me. 0.37*
      2. Said or did something just to make me feel jealous. 0.58*
      3. Destroyed or threatened to destroy something I valued. 0.77*
      4. Brought up something bad I had done in the past. 0.74*
      6. Said or did something just to make me angry. 0.71*
      7. Spoke to me in a hostile or mean tone of voice. 0.63* 0.38*
      10. Insulted me. 0.64*
      14. Kept track of who I was with and where I was. 0.76*
      15. Blamed me for a problem or fight we were having. 0.64* 0.40*
      17. Accused me of flirting with someone else. 0.82*
      25. Screamed or yelled at me. 0.62*
      26. Said mean things to me. 0.52* 0.46*
      33. Gave me the silent treatment. 0.54*
      34. Got upset when I spent time with other people. 0.72*
      35. Said mean things to me about someone else who is important to me. 0.42* 0.34*
      37. Told me that I needed to spend more time with him/her. 0.59* 0.45*
      38. Made me let them read my e-mails or texts when I didn’t want them to. 0.65* 0.43*
      F2: Physical, sexual aggression
      5. Threw something at me. 0.42*
      8. Forced me to do something sexual that I didn’t want to. 0.80*
      9. Threatened me to get me to do something sexual with him/her. 0.82*
      11. Kissed me when I didn’t want him/her to. 0.45* 0.61*
      16. Kicked, hit, or punched me. 0.74*
      19. Slapped me or pulled my hair. 0.61*
      20. Threatened to hurt me. 0.64* 0.34*
      22. Threatened to hit or throw something at me. 0.86*
      23. Pushed, shoved, grabbed, or shook me. 0.87*
      39. Made me do something I really didn’t want to do. 0.58*
      40. Was mean to me or insulted me to get me to do something for him/her. 0.67* 0.40*
      41. Got mad at me when I said “no” to him/her about something. 0.33* 0.56*
      42. Threatened me to try to get me to do something he/she wanted me to do. 0.65* 0.41*
      44. Kept pressuring me to do something even after I made it clear that I did not want to. 0.67*
      F3: Relational aggression
      12. Said things to my friends about me to turn them against me. 0.65*
      21. Threatened to break up with me or end our friendship. 0.39* 0.65*
      24. Spread rumors about me. 0.86*
      27. Left me out of an activity or a social group on purpose. 0.83*
      29. Said means things about me to other people. 0.86*
      30. Talked about how other people were better or more fun than me. 0.53*
      31. Told me that other people didn’t like me. 0.65*
      32. Told me that I was not a good boyfriend/girlfriend 0.63*
      36. Got upset when I did really well on something. 0.38* 0.62*

      Analysis was performed on Sample 1 and items 13, 18, 28, and 43 were removed. All factor loadings were statistically significant, *p < 0.05.

      Factor 1: Psychological Aggression

      The first factor was comprised of 17 items, with standardized loadings ranging from 0.37 to 0.82. These items appear to reflect a psychological or manipulative form of aggression perpetrated to victims. Example items include, “gave me the silent treatment,” “insulted me,” “brought up something bad I had done in the past,” and “accused me of flirting with someone else.” The Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was 0.94.

      Factor 2: Physical, Sexual Aggression

      In the second factor, the standardized loadings of 14 items (factor loadings ranging between 0.42 and 0.87) suggest overt physical and sexual aggression acts committed toward a victim. Example items within this factor include, “threw something at me,” “threatened to hurt me,” “kicked, hit, or punched me,” and “kissed me when I didn’t want him/her to.” Cronbach’s alpha was 0.89.

      Factor 3: Relational Aggression

      In this last factor, 9 items with standardized loadings ranging between 0.53 and 0.86 reflect an indirectly applied form of relational aggression. In particular, the items reflected behaviors that a partner may do or say to others about a victim. Unlike the first factor that focused on psychological aggression targeted toward a victim, the items on this scale generally involved behaviors that implicated other individuals, such as a victims’ friends. Some examples from this scale include, “talked about how other people were better or more fun than me,” “left me out of an activity or social group on purpose,” “spread rumors about me,” and “said mean things about me to other people.” Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was 0.91.

      Confirmatory Factor Analysis

      Three separate CFAs were conducted with the second sample to compare a 3-factor model with a 2-factor model and 1-factor model, respectively. Given the use of the WLSMV estimator, the DIFFTEST function was employed using the 3-factor model as the initial comparison against the 2-factor and 1-factor models. In comparison to the 3-factor model, the 2-factor and 1-factor models each had statistically significant worse fit (all ps < 0.001). Thus, we retained the 3-factor model identified from the EFA used with the first sample (see Table 5). In general, model fit was acceptable, χ2(737) = 1611.27, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.06 (0.05–0.06); standardized factor loadings were strong (>0.60) and statistically significant (p < 0.001) (see Table 6 for loadings by factor). Further, reliability using McDonald’s omega (ω), which does not assume equal factor loadings (see Hayes and Coutts, 2020), showed that the scales were highly reliable.

      Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model fit for the factor structure of the Teen Dating Aggression Measure (TeDAM).

      Chi-Square test of model fit
      Chi-Square for difference test
      Solution χ2 df p RMSEA Δ CFI Δ TLI Δ WRMR Δ χ2 df p
      3-Factor 1611.267 737 <0.001 0.057 0.000 0.941 0.001 0.938 0.002 1.588 0.019
      2-Factor 1633.886 739 <0.001 0.057 0.000 0.940 0.001 0.936 0.000 1.607 0.009 36.069 2 <0.001
      1-Factor 1644.878 740 <0.001 0.057 0.939 0.936 1.616 42.621 3 <0.001

      Analysis was performed on Sample 2 and items 13, 18, 28, and 43 were removed All chi-square tests were statistically significant. Difference test uses the DIFFTEST option in Mplus using the 3-factor solution as the initial comparison (it has more free parameters). Statistically significant chi-square for difference tests mean that adding more restrictions (i.e., 2-factor and 1-factor) worsens model fit.

      Confirmatory factor analysis standardized loadings and omega reliability estimates (ω).

      3-Factor solution
      Item ω F1 F2 F3
      F1: Psychological aggression 0.98
      1. Tried to turn my friends against me. 0.85*
      2. Said or did something just to make me feel jealous. 0.84*
      3. Destroyed or threatened to destroy something I valued. 0.89*
      4. Brought up something bad I had done in the past. 0.86*
      6. Said or did something just to make me angry. 0.89*
      7. Spoke to me in a hostile or mean tone of voice. 0.89*
      10. Insulted me. 0.82*
      14. Kept track of who I was with and where I was. 0.80*
      15. Blamed me for a problem or fight we were having. 0.89*
      17. Accused me of flirting with someone else. 0.82*
      25. Screamed or yelled at me. 0.86*
      26. Said mean things to me. 0.92*
      33. Gave me the silent treatment. 0.84*
      34. Got upset when I spent time with other people. 0.90*
      35. Said mean things to me about someone else who is important to me. 0.84*
      37. Told me that I needed to spend more time with him/her. 0.78*
      38. Made me let them read my e-mails or texts when I didn’t want them to. 0.89*
      F2: Physical, sexual aggression 0.98
      5. Threw something at me. 0.73*
      8. Forced me to do something sexual that I didn’t want to. 0.87*
      9. Threatened me to get me to do something sexual with him/her. 0.78*
      11. Kissed me when I didn’t want him/her to. 0.77*
      16. Kicked, hit, or punched me. 0.77*
      19. Slapped me or pulled my hair. 0.90*
      20. Threatened to hurt me. 0.87*
      22. Threatened to hit or throw something at me. 0.85*
      23. Pushed, shoved, grabbed, or shook me. 0.87*
      39. Made me do something I really didn’t want to do. 0.95*
      40. Was mean to me or insulted me to get me to do something for him/her. 0.97*
      41. Got mad at me when I said “no” to him/her about something. 0.91*
      42. Threatened me to try to get me to do something he/she wanted me to do. 0.95*
      44. Kept pressuring me to do something even after I made it clear that I did not want to. 0.91*
      F3: Relational aggression 0.96
      12. Said things to my friends about me to turn them against me. 0.82*
      21. Threatened to break up with me or end our friendship. 0.84*
      24. Spread rumors about me. 0.88*
      27. Left me out of an activity or a social group on purpose. 0.62*
      29. Said means things about me to other people. 0.86*
      30. Talked about how other people were better or more fun than me. 0.84*
      31. Told me that other people didn’t like me. 0.94*
      32. Told me that I was not a good boyfriend/girlfriend 0.91*
      36. Got upset when I did really well on something. 0.80*

      Analysis was performed on Sample 2 and items 13, 18, 28, and 43 were removed. All factor loadings were statistically significant, *p < 0.05.

      Mean-Level Differences by Gender Identity and Grade Level

      Scores for each of the three dimensions were aggregated for each of the three scales and then compared as a function of participant gender identity and grade level. A one-way MANOVA with the three scores as the dependent variables did not yield a statistically significant multivariate effect of gender identity for Sample 1, Wilk’s λ = 0.97, F(9, 825.19) = 1.31, p = 0.23, ηp2 = 0.01, or Sample 2, Wilk’s λ = 0.97, F(9, 886.03) = 1.28, p = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.01. Additionally, there were no observed multivariate effects of grade level for Sample 1, Wilk’s λ = 0.98, F(12, 894.44) = 0.52, p = 0.90, ηp2 = 0.01, or Sample 2, Wilk’s λ = 0.95, F(18, 1021.55) = 1.00, p = 0.46, ηp2 = 0.02. The lack of significant group differences suggest that the means from each of the three dimensions did not significantly differ as a function of the participant’s gender identity or their grade.

      Concurrent Validity

      In the last analysis, we tested concurrent associations between the TeDAM, perceptions of rape myths acceptance, cannabis use, and alcohol consumption (see Table 7 for descriptive statistics). Accordingly, bivariate correlations including bootstrapping (n = 10,000) to estimate 95% confidence intervals were computed between the mean scores of the three factors along with cannabis use, alcohol overconsumption, and rape myth acceptance for the whole sample. Results showed similar findings across each of the three factors (see Table 8). Psychological aggression was positively associated with alcohol overconsumption, cannabis use, and rape denial. Physical and sexual aggression was also positively correlated with each of alcohol overconsumption, cannabis use, and rape denial. Similarly, relational aggression was positively related to alcohol overconsumption, cannabis use, and rape denial. There were no statistically significant associations between the TeDAM scales and traditional gender expectations (ps > 0.05).

      Descriptive statistics of study variables for the overall sample.

      Variable M (SD) Minimum Maximum
      TeDAM
      Psychological aggression 1.29 (0.56) 1.00 4.28
      Physical, sexual aggression 1.10 (0.32) 1.00 4.41
      Relational aggression 1.12 (0.38) 1.00 4.89
      Cannabis use 1.48 (1.06) 1.00 6.00
      Alcohol overconsumption 1.22 (0.64) 1.00 6.00
      Rape myth acceptance
      Traditional gender expectations 3.82 (1.40) 3.00 12.00
      Rape denial 16.00 (6.80) 4.00 16.00

      Scores on the TeDAM are an average of the number of items for each scale, while the rape myth acceptance scores are summed items.

      Bivariate correlations between Teen Dating Aggression Measure (TeDAM) scales and outcome variables.

      Cannabis use Alcohol overconsumption RMA—Traditional gender expectations RMA rape denial
      Psychological aggression 0.28** (0.16–0.39) 0.17* (0.06–0.30) 0.02 (–0.06 to 0.16) 0.10* (0.01–0.21)
      Physical, sexual aggression 0.21** (0.11–0.32) 0.12* (0.01–0.25) 0.03 (–0.10 to –0.21) 0.11* (–0.02 to 0.23)
      Relational aggression 0.23** (0.11–0.35) 0.17** (0.11–0.35) 0.02 (–0.09 to –0.17) 0.08* (–0.03 to 0.20)

      95% confidence intervals are reported with bootstrapping = 10,000. RMA, rape myth acceptance scale. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.

      Discussion

      Findings from this study provide initial psychometric support for the TeDAM and its use among adolescents. Specifically, factor analyses for the TeDAM suggested that a solution that included three factors, namely behaviors regarding (a) psychological aggression, (b) physical and sexual aggression, and (c) relational aggression, were most appropriate. This factor structure was further supported in a CFA and high reliability estimates.

      Each of the factors represented the various ways in which adolescents might experience dating aggression. The first factor, physical and sexual aggression, is common among all measures within the TDV literature. In line with other measures of dating violence victimization, including the CADRI (Wolfe et al., 2001) and the MARSHA (Rothman et al., 2021), the TeDAM yielded a factor that incorporated elements of physical (e.g., slapped, hair pulled, objects thrown) as well as sexual aggression (e.g., threats to coerce sexual acts, sexual force). The second factor reflected psychological aggression, which are harmful verbal and/or emotional acts directed toward the victim (Follingstad et al., 2005). Previous work by Follingstad (2007) suggests that psychological aggression in the form of humiliation and insults differs from physically aggressive acts. Indeed, the factor structure of the TeDAM supports the claim that dating violence and aggression should not be limited to physical acts. Specifically, psychological aggression included items related to verbal and emotional aggression (e.g., making accusations, insulting, giving the silent treatment) as well as manipulative forms of aggression (e.g., made to allow to read social media, keeping track of a partner). Lastly, the third factor related to acts that were characterized as relational aggression. Although this factor could be interpreted similarly to the psychological aggression, the relational component referred to behaviors pertaining to the implicates the peer group (or others) to harm the relationship. For example, items within this scale included aggressive acts such as spreading rumors or using others (e.g., a victim’s friends) to make threats or exclude a partner. Within the context of adolescent peer relations, psychological forms of aggression are distinct from relational aggression (see Linder et al., 2002). In particular, the target of psychological aggression is the victim while the target for relational aggression is the relationship. The methods employed within each form of aggression can vary, but the target remains consistent. The addition of a scale focused on relational aggression scale is supported by previous research on IPV, in which romantic relational victimization was found to be negatively correlated with romantic relationship quality (Linder et al., 2002).

      There were three findings related to the validation of the measure. First, we found that each of the three factors was associated with adolescent cannabis use and the overconsumption of alcohol. Specifically, more frequent experiences with each of the three aggressive factors were associated with increased use of cannabis and drinking. These findings are in line with previous work that suggests that cannabis and alcohol are associated with victimization from TDV and assault (e.g., Dworkin et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2017). The last two findings refer to the association between the TDV factors and rape myth acceptance. In particular, there were no significant associations with the traditional gender expectations scale, whereas each of the factors were positively correlated with rape denial. Previous studies have shown positive associations between rape myth acceptance and sexual dating aggression (Reyes and Foshee, 2013). Our findings are partially in line with these results, with the exception of traditional gender expectations. There are two potential methodological explanations for this. First, the IRMAS, the measure with which the items were derived and adapted, is known to produce floor effects, which could explain the lack of a significant association (Trottier et al., 2021). Moreover, with the exception of Dworkin et al. (2017), research on the association between TDV victimization and rape myth acceptance and its specific subscales have not been investigated with an adolescent population and therefore requires replication.

      Broadly, the strengths of the TeDAM include the support for a scale on relational aggression. This addition is important because it incorporates the typical actions and behaviors that adolescents engage in with their peers as well as their romantic partners, thus making the measure more developmentally relevant. From a measurement perspective, the TeDAM is also straightforward to use, easy to score, and has strong reliability and validity. Nevertheless, this study acknowledges some limitations. First, although the large sample was obtained from three different provinces across Canada, these were not nationally representative, therefore we could not test for sociodemographic differences (e.g., ethnicity). Second, although our sample size was large enough to assess the factor structure of this measure, we were unable to fully evaluate the psychometric properties using measurement invariance, which would address the extent to which the items were interpreted in a similar manner across different groups. As such, replication of the factor structures and an analysis of the equivalence across sex, gender identity, and previous experiences with relationship violence would benefit the utility of the TeDAM. Finally, the present study was focused on dating victimization experiences of youth. However, it is also necessary to consider the extent to which dating partners perpetrate such acts. Given that the additional items were written similarly to the CADRI, we argue that the target of scale can change between the victim to the perpetrator, which would be in line with other measures, including the CADRI (Wolfe et al., 2001) and the MARSHA (Rothman et al., 2021, 2020). We speculate that a similar factor structure would emerge with dating violence perpetrators.

      In summary, the goal of this study was to create an assessment of TDV that was designed to reflect the adolescent experience and include relevant forms of TDV. This measure begins to address the gaps in extant TDV measures by including items related to overt sexual violence, psychological aggression, and relationally aggressive behaviors. Results provided initial psychometric support for a developmentally relevant assessment of adolescent aggressive experiences in the context of romantic relationships. Given both the short- and long-term consequences of victimization from dating violence, there are meaningful implications for researchers, as this could provide more authentic findings when investigating the phenomenon of TDV, as well as have important implications for practitioners (e.g., clinicians) looking to obtain a more comprehensive view of the experiences of TDV victimization among the youth they service. Together, with further development and implementation of the TeDAM, this study has crucial theoretical implications as it could help increase our understanding in the field of TDV.

      Data Availability Statement

      The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because there are ethical and consent restrictions that do not allow for the release of data outside of the research team. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to WC, wendy.craig@queensu.ca.

      Ethics Statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by 1. Research Ethics Board Office (McGill University) #20-07-044 - Research Ethics, 2. General Research Ethics Board (Queen’s University) #GPSYC-997-20. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

      Author Contributions

      WC, MD, AM-S, and CK conceptualized and designed the larger study which includes this study. ND coordinated data collection as well as cleaned the data. For this manuscript, RP, TW, LV-M, and CK contributed to the conceptualization and plan. RP organized the data. RP and LV-M conducted the statistical analyses. RP wrote the first draft of the manuscript. TW, LV-M, and ND wrote sections of the manuscript. CK oversaw the manuscript writing throughout the process. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s Note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Funding

      This research was supported by a grant from the Public Health Agency of Canada (1718-HQ-000788) awarded to WC.

      References Asghari M. Connolly J. Cochrane-Brink K. (2021). Peer and dating aggression among early adolescent boys and girls admitted to a secure inpatient psychiatric unit: links with maltreatment. J. Aggress. Maltrea. Trauma 30, 154174. 10.1080/10926771.2020.1783735 Alleyne B. Coleman-Cowger V. H. Crown L. Gibbons M. A. Vines L. N. (2011). The effects of dating violence, substance use and risky sexual behavior among a diverse sample of Illinois youth. J. Adolesc. 34 1118. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.03.006 20452662 Breiding M. Basile K. C. Smith S. G. Black M. C. Mahendra R. R. (2015). Intimate Partner Violence Surveillance: Uniform Definitions and Recommended Data Elements. Version 2.0. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. Cascardi M. Avery-Leaf S. O’Leary K. D. Slep A. M. S. (1999). Factor structure and convergent validity of the Conflict Tactics Scale in high school students. Psychol. Assess. 11 546555. 10.1037/1040-3590.11.4.546 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2020). Preventing Teen Dating Violence. Available online at: https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/intimatepartnerviolence/teendatingviolence/fastfact.html (accessed December 30, 2020). Clark D. A. Bowles R. P. (2018). Model fit and item factor analysis: overfactoring, underfactoring, and a program to guide interpretation. Multivariate Behav. Res. 53 544558. 10.10180/00273171.2018.1461058 Collins W. A. Welsh D. P. Furman W. (2009). Adolescent romantic relationships. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60 631652. Dosil M. Jaureguizar J. Bernaras E. Sbicigo J. B. (2020). Teen dating violence, sexism, and resilience: a multivariate analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 118. 10.3390/ijerph17082652 32294915 Dworkin E. R. Sessarego S. N. Pittenger S. L. Edwards K. M. Banyard V. (2017). Rape myth acceptance in sexually-assaulted adolescents’ school contexts: associations with depressed mood and alcohol use. Am. J. Community Psychol. 60 516526. 10.1002/ajcp.12173 28921576 Edwards K. M. Turchik J. A. Dardis C. M. Reynolds N. Gidycz C. A. (2011). Rape myths: history, individual and institutional-level presence, and implications for change. Sex Roles 65 761773. 10.1007/s11199-011-9943-2 Ellis W. E. Crooks C. V. Wolfe D. A. (2009). Relational aggression in peer and dating relationships: links to psychological and behavioural adjustment. Soc. Dev. 18, 253269. 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00468.x Espelage D. L. Leemis R. W. Niolon P. H. Kearns M. Basile K. C. Davis J. P. (2020). Teen dating violence perpetration: protective factor trajectories from middle to high school among adolescents. J. Res. Adolesc. 30 170188. 10.1111/jora.12510 31169951 Exner-Cortens D. (2018). “Measuring adolescent dating violence,” in Adolescent Dating Violence: Theory, Research, and Prevention, eds Wolfe D. A. Temple J. R. (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 315340. 10.1016/B978-0-12-811797-2.00013-X Exner-Cortens D. Baker E. Craig W. (2021). The national prevalence of adolescent dating violence in Canada. J. Adolescent Health 69, 495502. 10.1016/j.adolhealth.2021.01.032 Exner-Cortens D. Eckenrode J. Rothman E. (2013). Longitudinal associations between teen dating violence victimization and adverse health outcomes. Pediatrics 131 7178. 10.1542/peds.2012-1029 23230075 Exner-Cortens D. Gill L. Eckenrode J. (2016a). Measurement of adolescent dating violence: a comprehensive review (Part 1, behaviours). Aggress. Violent Behav. 27 6478. 10.1016/j.avb.2016.02.007 Exner-Cortens D. Gill L. Eckenrode J. (2016b). Measurement of adolescent dating violence: a comprehensive review (Part 2, attitudes). Aggress. Violent Behav. 27 93106. Fernández-Fuertes A. A. Fuertes A. Pulido R. F. (2006). Evaluación de la violencia en las relaciones de pareja de los adolescentes. Validación del Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships Inventory (CADRI)-versión española. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 6 339358. Fernández-González L. Wekerle C. Goldstein A. L. (2012). Measuring adolescent dating violence: development of ‘conflict in adolescent dating relationships inventory’ short form. Adv. Ment. Health 11 3554. 10.5172/jamh.2012.11.1.35 Follingstad D. R. (2007). Rethinking current approaches to psychological abuse: conceptual and methodological issues. Aggress. Violent Behav. 12 439458. 10.1016/j.avb.2006.07.004 Follingstad D. R. Coyne S. Gambone L. (2005). A representative measure of psychological aggression and its severity. Violence Vict. 20 2538. 10.1891/vivi.2005.20.1.25 11261958 Furman W. Collins W. (2009). “Adolescent romantic relationships and experiences,” in Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups, eds Rubin K. H. Bukowski W. M. Laursen B. (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 341360. Garthe R. C. Sullivan T. N. Behrhorst K. L. (2021). A latent class analysis of early adolescent peer and dating violence: associations with symptoms of depression and anxiety. J. Interpers. Violence 36(5-6), 20312049. 10.1177/0886260518759654 29475422 Goncy E. A. Sullivan T. N. Farrell A. D. Mehari K. R. Garthe R. C. (2017). Identification of patterns of dating aggression and victimization among urban early adolescents and their relations to mental health symptoms. Psychol. Violence 7, 5868. 10.1037/vio0000039 Hayes A. F. Coutts J. J. (2020). Use omega rather than Cronbach’s alpha for estimating reliability. But…. Commun. Methods Meas. 14 124. 10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629 Hokoda A. Ramos-Lira L. Celaya P. Vilhauer K. Angeles M. Ruíz S. (2006). Reliability of translated measures assessing dating violence among Mexican adolescents. Violence Vict. 21 117127. 10.1891/0886-6708.21.1.117 11261958 Hu L. Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6 155. 10.1080/10705519909540118 Johnson R. M. LaValley M. Schneider K. E. Musci R. J. Pettoruto K. Rothman E. F. (2017). Marijuana use and physical dating violence among adolescents and emerging adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 174 4757. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.01.012 28314193 Jouriles E. N. Garrido E. Rosenfield D. McDonald R. (2009). Experiences of psychological and physical aggression in adolescent romantic relationships: links to psychological distress. Child Abuse Negl. 33, 451460. 10.1016/j.chiabu.208.11.005 Kaiser H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20 141151. 10.1177/001316446002000116 Kline R. B. (2016). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford. Knox L. Lomonaco C. Alpert E. (2009). “Adolescent relationship violence,” in Intimate Partner Violence: A Health-Based Perspective, eds Mitchell C. Anglin D. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 511530. Leadbeater B. J. Banister E. M. Ellis W. E. Yeung R. (2008). Victimization and relational aggression in adolescent romantic relationships: The influence of parental and peer behaviours, and individual adjustment. J. Youth Adolesc. 37, 359372. 10.1007/s10964-007-9269-0 27307651 Linder J. R. Crick N. R. Collins W. A. (2002). Relational aggression and victimization in young adults’ romantic relationships: associations with perceptions of parent, peer, and romantic relationship quality. Soc. Dev. 11 6986. 10.1111/1467-9507.00187 Morelli M. Bianchi D. Chirumbolo A. Baiocco R. (2018). The cyber dating violence inventory. Validation of a new scale for online perpetration and victimization among dating partners. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 15, 464471. 10.1080/17405629.2017.1305885 Muthén L. K. Muthén B. O. (1998-2017). Mplus user’s Guide, 8th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. Parker E. M. Debnam K. Pas E. T. Bradshaw C. P. (2016). Exploring the link between alcohol and marijuana use and teen dating violence victimization among high school students. The influence of school context. Health Educ. Behav. 43 528536. 10.1177/1090198115605308 26377526 Payne D. L. Lonsway K. A. Fitzgerald L. F. (1999). Rape myth acceptance: exploration of its structure and its measurement using the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale. J. Res. Pers. 33, 2768. 10.1006/jrpe.1998.2238 Reyes H. L. M. N. Foshee V. A. (2013). Sexual dating aggression across grades 8 through 12: timing and predictors of onset. J. Youth Adolesc. 42 581595. 10.1007/s10964-012-9864-6 23180071 Rothman E. F. Cuevas C. A. Mumford E. A. Bahrami E. Taylor B. G. (2021). The psychometric properties of the Measure of Adolescent Relationship Harassment and Abuse (MARSHA) with a nationally representative sample of US youth. J. Interpers. Violence 10.1177/0886260520985480 [Epub ahead of print]. 33399026 Rothman E. F. Paruk J. Cuevas C. A. Temple J. R. Gonzales K. (2020). The development of the Measure of Adolescent Relationship Harassment and Abuse (MARSHA): input from Black and multiracial, Latinx, Native American, and LGBTQ+ youth. J. Interpers. Violence 124. 10.1177/0886260520936367 32627640 Schad M. M. Szwedo D. E. Antonishak J. Hare A. Allen J. P. (2008). The broader context of relational aggression in adolescent romantic relationships: predictions from peer pressure and links to psychosocial functioning. J. Youth Adolesc. 37, 346358. 10.1007/s10964-007-9226-y 18523685 Shaffer C. S. Adjei J. Viljoen J. L. Douglas K. S. Saewyc E. M. (2021). Ten-year trends in physical dating violence victimization among adolescent boys and girls in British Columbia, Canada. J. Interpers. Violence 36(9-10), 39473964. 10.1177/0886260518788367 30019602 Smith J. Mulford C. Latzman N. E. Tharp A. T. Niolon P. H. Blachman-Demner D. (2015). Taking stock of behavioral measures of adolescent dating violence. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 24 674692. 10.1080/10926771.2015.1049767 29606849 Statistics Canada (2018). Victims of Police-Reported Intimate Partner and Non-Intimate Partner Violence, by Victim Sex, Age Group and Relationship of Accused to Victim, Canada. Available online at: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/pub/85-002-x/2019001/article/00018/tbl/tbl02-1-eng.htm (accessed December 30, 2020). Straus M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: the Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. J. Marriage Fam. 41 7586. Tabachnick B. G. Fidell L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson. Trottier D. Benbouriche M. Bonneville V. (2021). A meta-analysis on the association between rape myth acceptance and sexual coercion perpetration. J. Sex Res. 58 375382. 10.1080/00224499.2019.1704677 31865775 Wolfe D. A. Scott K. Reitzel-Jaffe D. Wekerle C. Grasley C. Straatman A. (2001). Development and validation of the conflict in adolescent dating relationships inventory. Psychol. Assess. 13 277293. 10.1037/1040-3590.13.2.277
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.focusvideo.com.cn
      www.iibggm.com.cn
      kesilai.com.cn
      gyetgc.com.cn
      jisail.com.cn
      www.ipingo.com.cn
      suyin.net.cn
      www.nkyzjw.com.cn
      qkut.com.cn
      www.whzddl.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p