Front. Psychol. Frontiers in Psychology Front. Psychol. 1664-1078 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724871 Psychology Original Research Comparison of Japanese and Scottish Mother–Infant Intersubjectivity: Resonance of Timing, Anticipation, and Empathy During Feeding Negayama Koichi 1 * Delafield-Butt Jonathan T. 2 Momose Keiko 1 Ishijima Konomi 3 Kawahara Noriko 4 1Faculty of Human Sciences, Waseda University, Saitama, Japan 2Laboratory for Innovation in Autism and Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, United Kingdom 3Department of Child Studies, Shiraume Gakuen University, Tokyo, Japan 4Faculty of Home Economics, Kyoritsu Women’s University, Tokyo, Japan

Edited by: Leonardo De Pascalis, University of Liverpool, United Kingdom

Reviewed by: Madhavilatha Maganti, Ashoka University, India; Dolores Rollo, University of Parma, Italy

*Correspondence: Koichi Negayama, negayama@waseda.jp

This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology

14 10 2021 2021 12 724871 14 06 2021 16 09 2021 Copyright © 2021 Negayama, Delafield-Butt, Momose, Ishijima and Kawahara. 2021 Negayama, Delafield-Butt, Momose, Ishijima and Kawahara

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Feeding involves communication between mothers and infants and requires precise synchrony in a special triadic relationship with the food. It is deeply related to their intersubjectivity. This study compared the development of mother–infant intersubjectivity through interactional synchrony in feeding between 11 Japanese and 10 Scottish mother–infant dyads, observed at 6 and 9 months by video. Japanese mothers were more deliberate in feeding at an earlier age, whereas Scottish mothers were significantly more coercive than Japanese mothers at an earlier age. Japanese mothers brought the spoon to infants with a pause to adjust the timing of insertion to match their infants’ readiness, whereas this pause was not observed in Scottish mothers. Isomorphic mouth opening between mothers and infants was observed. This empathic maternal display is an important element of intersubjectivity in infant feeding that differed between Scottish and Japanese mothers. Scottish mothers’ mouth opening always followed their infants’ mouth opening, but about half of Japanese mothers preceded their infants. Further, the mouths of Scottish infants and mothers opened almost at the same time as spoon insertion. In contrast, Japanese mothers’ mouth opening did not co-occur with the insertion but was close to spoon arrival, a subtle but important difference that allows for greater infant autonomy. The time structure of Scottish mother-infant interactions was simpler and more predictable at 9 months than in Japan, where the structure was more variable, likely due to a stronger regulation by Scottish mothers. In conclusion, Scottish mother-infant intersubjectivity is characterized as more maternally reactive and mother-centered, whereas Japanese mother-infant intersubjectivity is characterized as more maternally empathetic and infant-centered. Cultural differences in intersubjectivity during feeding between Japan and Scotland are further discussed in relation to triadic relationships and parenting styles.

intersubjectivity interactional synchrony Japan and Scotland mother–infant relations empathy in feeding mouth opening

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Feeding is essential for infant survival and health, and weaning is a biologically significant framework for understanding the development of infant independence from the mother (Trivers, 1974; Negayama, 1996). Human infants gradually become autonomous feeders over the first years of life in terms of hand and tool use (Connolly and Dalgleish, 1989; Norimatsu, 1993; Kawahara, 2005), and food choices (Pliner, 1994; Tovar et al., 2016; Cole et al., 2018).

      Mothers assist their children’s feeding by providing food, cooking, and assisting with feeding when they are young and unable to manage it on their own (Wright, 1989). Such maternally supported infant feeding requires synchrony of intentions and actions on both sides, with attunement of these between mother and infant to ensure effective food intake, without choking. Interactional synchrony, or shared time, is fundamental to many domains of mother–infant intersubjectivity, and exists in the relationship even during the prenatal period – it is the basis of harmony between them (Feldman, 2003; Trevarthen et al., 2006). Infant feeding supported by the mother consists of a sequence of actions, or behavioral units, made with shared timing, and is one striking example of mother-infant intersubjectivity (Feldman, 2007).

      Empathy and Mother-Infant Intersubjectivity

      As they feed their young infants, some mothers show an interesting behavior of mouth movement (Figure 1) which is isomorphic with the infants’ mouth movement (Negayama, 1993; Kawata, 2014; Toyama, 2014). The behavioral symmetry could be a demonstration of mother-infant intersubjectivity. The neurophysiological background of behavioral mirroring has recently been discussed (Meyza and Knapska, 2018), and mouth mirror neurons are found for ingestive behavior in non-human primates (Ferrari et al., 2003).

      A Japanese (top) and a Scottish (bottom) mothers showing an empathetic mouth movement (opening and closing) while her infant is taking food.

      Co-occurrence of symmetrical mouth movements is a sign of empathy in the feeding mothers (Negayama, 1993), and complements infant-mother “sympathy” as directly feeling the other person’s affective and intentional state (Trevarthen, 2016). It is supposed that orofacial communicative behavior has its evolutionary origin in the mother–infant dyad (Ferrari and Coudé, 2018). Non-verbal expressive actions of the body serve to mediate connectedness, and together with the mirror neuron system enables ‘direct neural resonance,’ a specific neural mechanism of shared affective and intentional neuromotor states that afford informed, intersubjective interactions with others (Gallese, 2009; Gallagher, 2012).

      Human movements are never just functional and performative, but simultaneously express feeling in their kinematic form (Stern, 2010). These ‘forms of vitality’ convey an affective quality within the functional, performative aspects of movement that enrich intersubjectivity, sharing feelings in the intentional acts (Di Cesare et al., 2017; Di Cesare et al., 2020). And withholding expectation by pausing or coming in too quickly can raise arousal to generate excitement, or anxiety (Gratier, 2003).

      This feeling is shared between individuals in their form and timing of body movement, made in the intersubjective ‘dance’ of their interaction (Malloch and Trevarthen, 2009a). Sharing feelings in sympathy, individuals may then reflect on these empathically to give “emotional and mental sensitivity to another’s state” (de Waal and Preston, 2017). Where sympathy names feelings in direct resonance, empathy allows for additional reflection or mentalizing and “assessing the reasons for it and adopting the other’s point of view.” Sympathy allows the organism to quickly relate to the states of others, and effectively empathize with their experience. These are essential for the regulation of social interactions, coordinated activity, and cooperation toward shared goals (Clay et al., 2018). In this paper, we employ a mother-infant feeding paradigm as a lens with which to observe the nuance of the intersubjective relation between mother and infant. Mother-infant feeding requires shared timing and symmetry of behavior. It is a clear and accessible case of embodied mother-infant intersubjectivity, with the maternal empathetic mouth opening giving one sensitive behavioral index of it.

      Differences in Mother–Infant Intersubjectivity by Infant’s Age and Culture

      Importantly, interactional synchrony is required between mothers and infants to attune and harmonize their behaviors for cooperative effect. The process of mother-infant feeding is a continuous adjustment of the timing of food giving (maternal) and intake (infant) actions. Empathetic mouth opening by the mothers to the infants’ mouth opening at the moment of infant eating is a situation typical in everyday feeding of shared timing between the two.

      The intentional nature of all human action, evident from before birth, underpins shared action understanding (Delafield-Butt and Gangopadhyay, 2013; Trevarthen and Delafield-Butt, 2017) generated within embodied, enactive interactions (Trevarthen and Delafield-Butt, 2013; Fantasia et al., 2014). Thus, motor timing in interaction shared between mother and infant presents an empirical measure of the shared intersubjective exchange. Set within shared projects, such as during infant pick-up or during feeding, these single acts become serially organized to give narrative structure, a narrative arc, as each act unfolds over time, altogether achieving the shared goal of the project (Trevarthen and Delafield-Butt, 2013; Rossmanith et al., 2014; Delafield-Butt and Trevarthen, 2015).

      Empathy of feelings shared in sympathy cumulatively works over the whole process of feeding. Communication between feeders and infants with control, request, rejection, and cooperation between them (Stevenson et al., 1990; Negayama, 1993; Toyama, 2014; Fries et al., 2017; Nonaka and Stoffregen, 2020) reflects biological and cultural fundamentals of mother-infant relationships and their development.

      Feeding induces a stronger empathetic mouth opening in mothers when providing food on their own than when just watching the infant being fed by the father (Negayama, 2000). This finding suggests that synchrony is not just reflection of behavior but could be boosted by the mothers’ anticipation of infants’ food intake. Eating is a continuous narrative between mother and infant containing intention-reading and anticipation.

      From as early as 2 months old, infants adapt their action to match imminent action intentions of their mother. For example, when being picked up from the floor, an infant will arch its back and raise its arms and stiffen in preparation for the new forces and requirements of being picked up (Reddy et al., 2013). The infant is prospectively aware of the imminent consequences of their mothers’ actions, revealing an anticipatory awareness and self-generated adaptive, preparatory response to meet those expected demands. This basic action understanding is the embodied basis of understanding other minds (Trevarthen, 2001; Gallagher, 2012).

      As demonstrated in Japanese mother-infant tickling play (Ishijima and Negayama, 2013), infants start to show joint attention with the mothers to a part of the infant’s body at 6 months. This is “proto-triadic relationship” (Negayama, 2012, forthcoming) that precedes “genuine triadic relationship” at 9 months (Tomasello, 1995; Negayama, forthcoming). Feeding is an example of the triadic relationship between parent, infant and food. The development of this triadic relationship should be reflected in differences of behaviors and their timing.

      Feeding is also an accessible, important paradigm to understand cultural differences in intersubjectivity in the mother–infant relationship. We are particularly interested in whether or not and how the mothers in Japan and Scotland differ in their empathetic mouth openings.

      Previous studies have shown Japanese and Scottish mothers differ in distance regulation with their infants, and its manner (Negayama and Trevarthen, under review). Scottish mothers took greater initiative than Japanese mothers in this aspect. Japan–Scotland differences were also found in bedtime and sleep routines in both the home and day nurseries (Negayama, 1997; Negayama and Kawahara, 2010). Scottish mothers prefer putting children to bed without physical contact, whereas the Japanese mothers keep physical contact until their children fall asleep. The mothers and infants in Japan more often co-sleep at night, whereas those in the Western countries prefer to sleep separately (Caudill and Weinstein, 1969). These findings suggest a stronger empathy toward their children in Japanese mothers, and a preference for peaceful togetherness rather than their independence.

      Interestingly, Japanese mothers often consume the food left by their infants, and give their own food to their infants when their infants’ plates are empty (“cross-feeding,” Negayama, 2006), which suggests they actively generate a sense of “oneness,” a significant intimacy of shared, common experience in the dyad. This behavior is seldom observed in Scotland. Scottish mothers quickly withdraw from the role of feeder when their infants become autonomous in eating (Negayama, 2000). In contrast, Japanese infants refuse to be fed passively and do not establish independence in eating until later (Negayama, 1993). These examples suggest that Japanese mothers are more strongly guided by their empathy and motivation for shared experience with their infants.

      On the other hand, if mothers prefer to direct and control their infants, then their infant’s food intake would be regulated more strongly by their mothers. Conversely, if the mothers prefer to facilitate and share their infants’ experience, then the infants would become more autonomous and the mothers will follow the infants. Thus, the present study sought to shed light on mother–infant intersubjectivity in feeding and its cultural differences in Japan and Scotland, by measuring the timing of their feeding interactions, especially over mouth openings of mothers and infants. It also could be postulated that cultural differences in mother–infant feeding relationships are expected to be evident over development, in this case between 6 and 9 months of age.

      Purpose of the Present Study

      Coordinated action timing and synchrony of feeding behaviors between mother and infant are for efficient feeding, matching the intentions of the mother and the infant. This matching is examined by recording the timing of the mouth openings of mother and infants during the feeds. Apparently ‘synchronous’ behaviors could be found to be asynchronous when examined in detail. For example, a mother may open her mouth earlier than her infant, or vice versa. Isomorphism of the two behaviors makes the minute analysis easy. The initiative for action in the intersubjectivity of mother-infant feeding can be examined by comparing the onset times of their mouth openings with micro-analysis of their occurrence. Alternatively, the mother’s mouth opening might be regulated by some adjacent behaviors, for example, by the spoon’s arrival (i.e., stagnation) at the infant’s mouth, and insertion into it.

      Fine micro-analysis of interaction timing during feeding serves as a promising window for exploring the age and cultural differences in mother–infant relationships. The basic question here is: Is the mother’s empathetic mouth opening directly caused by the infant’s mouth opening, or elicited by the mother’s own intention? To answer this question, three behaviors of mother’s empathetic mouth opening, infant mouth opening, and spoon carriage (particularly spoon arrival and insertion) are analyzed and compared. The analysis may demonstrate a variation by age and culture in the manner and style of more general mother–infant intersubjectivity, specifically analyzed here during feeding.

      Materials and Methods Participants

      Eleven Japanese (two boys and nine girls, five first-borns) and 10 healthy Scottish infants (five boys and five girls, seven first-borns) at the ages of 6 months participated with their mothers. They were recruited at local nursery schools by delivering an invitation letter in Japan and personally through word of mouth, parent groups, and nurseries in Scotland. Each mother and infant pair participated in the observation twice: first at the infants’ age of ca. 6 months (Age 1, mean ages/SDs were 192.0/9.1 and 191.6/24.1 days in Japan and Scotland, respectively); and second at ca. 9 months (Age 2, mean ages/SDs were 282.2/16.2 and 289.8/16.7 days in Japan and Scotland, respectively). The other details of the participants are provided in Negayama et al. (2015). Participant numbers were limited and considered acceptable within the design due to the study requiring a precise time of laboratory-based recording within limited developmental periods.

      This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of Waseda University (No. 2012-273) and the University of Strathclyde Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from each mother or father at the start of the study.

      Procedure and Data Recording

      The present study is a part of a larger research project carried out at Ages 1 and 2, in which (1) mothers put down then picked up their infants from the floor, (2) mothers fed their infant with solid food with a spoon, (3) mothers tickled their infant in free play for about 15 min, and (4) mothers and infants played an action-word game task. The first part (study of pick-up) has already been reported (Negayama et al., 2015).

      Data recordings of Japanese and Scottish participants were carried out at a laboratory at Waseda University in Japan and another at the University of Strathclyde in Scotland. The entire process was recorded using two or more standard home video cameras with the assistance of a motion-capture system. The mothers were briefly interviewed after the experiment for background information on the infants (the birth date, family composition, parents’ education and occupation, etc.). Written informed consent was obtained from each mother or father.

      One Japanese case and one Scottish case were not observed at Age 1 because solid feeding was not started yet. Feeding of 10 Japanese and 9 Scottish mother-infant dyads was filmed at Age 1, and of 11 Japanese and 10 Scottish dyads was filmed again at Age 2. Mothers brought solid foods from home to the laboratory and gave them to their infants with a spoon in their own way. The entire session was videotaped in each observation.

      Behavioral Analysis

      Feeding is a sequence of behaviors: food is scooped by a spoon and carried to an infant’s mouth. The spoon stagnates before (or after in rare cases) reaching the infant’s mouth (arrival), and is inserted (spoon tip passing the edge of the lips) into the infant’s mouth. If the spoon does not stagnate, the time of arrival equals to the time of insertion. The infant opens the mouth at the moment of spoon insertion, but the mouth opening may be later than the insertion time. Some mothers coerce the infant to take the food provided by an exaggerated shooting movement toward the infant’s mouth to elicit the infant’s mouth opening. The spoon can be inserted and pulled out repeatedly until there is no food left on the spoon (final withdrawal), and then the spoon is returned to the initial position. One spoon refers to feeding from the start of the spoon carriage to the return. Simply the initial five consecutive spoon feeds were analyzed for these behaviors.

      During feeding, mothers exhibit empathetic mouth opening (sometimes with chewing and closing), a fluid and reflexive behavior without reflective mentalization or strong volition. Another type of mouth opening also occurred which was to intentionally encourage the infant to take the food. This type quite often occurred with vocalization. In the present study, we focused on the former mouth movement, which was usually silent, as a behavior indicative of the mother’s intersubjectivity to the infant and ignored the latter one.

      For a fine analysis of mother-infant interactions concerning the mother’s empathetic mouth opening, a good video image of the mouth movements by the mothers and infants was needed. However, the initial five spoons were not appropriate for this analysis because the mother talked or the hand of the mother or infant hid their mouth. So, we selected as many as possible other episodes out of the entire feeding session for the analysis. the number of episodes collected by this way were 43 and 43 (range/median = 0–7/6 and 0–7/6) for Japanese Age 1 and Age 2, respectively, and 50 and 52 (range/median = 3–5/5 and 3–9/4.5) for Scottish Age 1 and Age 2, respectively.

      The time sequence of the spoon-feeding episodes was measured using the video analysis software ELAN (Version 5.9.1) [Computer software] (2020), and then the time gaps of the behavior occurrences were calculated from the onset times of adjacent behaviors to determine the sequential relationship of the behaviors.

      Because of the small numbers of samples and episodes, age and cultural comparisons were made on the basis of median values of the time for each dyad, and non-parametric tests were conducted for statistical analyses. Differences between the two age periods were examined by pairwise comparison by the Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test, and differences between Japan and Scotland at each age were examined by Mann–Whitney U test. For both, the significance level was set at 0.05.

      Results

      In order to substantiate intersubjectivity by a behavior, the timing and shape should be shared. Adjustment of mutual behaviors is needed for feeding, which is measured by timing of onset. The time structure of the behaviors, the synchrony of the mouth openings of the mothers and infants measured by the onset time are the focus of the analysis. Firstly, to understand the characteristics of the feeding interactions, a sequence of feeding was analyzed. The onset time and duration of each behavior were compared between the two stages (Age 1 and Age 2) and between the two countries (Japan and Scotland) in general based on both the initial five feeding episodes and the additionally selected episodes. Then secondly, the mother’s empathetic mouth opening was focused on, and the relationship of the timings of the behaviors of the mothers and infants were analyzed based on the additionally selected episodes.

      Sequence of Feeding

      One spoon carriage consisted of a series of different behavioral components (Figure 2): spoon departure from the starting point, spoon arrival near the infant’s mouth, spoon insertion into the mouth, first spoon pulling-out, (repetition of insertion and pulling-out), final spoon withdrawal, and spoon return to the initial position. The infant’s mouth opening to capture food occurs typically somewhere between spoon arrival and insertion. The relationships of the mother’s empathetic mouth opening with the infant’s mouth opening (Arrow A), spoon arrival (Arrow B), and spoon insertion (Arrow C) were focused on.

      The sequence of spoon-feeding segmented by the onset time of behaviors. The ten intervals denoted by circled numbers were selected for analysis. Arrows A, B, and C are assumed causal directions of the mother’s empathetic mouth opening.

      A sequence of spoon movements can be divided into seven sections by the onset times of those behavioral components. Ten intervals were identified for analysis as indicated by Intervals ① to ➉ in Figure 2. The feeding sequence was normally composed of these sections, and a general time sequence of feeding was separately compared between the two age points and between the two countries on the basis of the lengths of the intervals in the initial five spoons (Table 1).

      Comparison of median behavior intervals (sec) by country and age.

      From To Age Country
      Mann–Whitney
      Japan Scotland
      Departure Arrival 1 1.77 2.18 0.97
      2 1.44 1.99 0.01*
      Wilcoxon 0.037* 0.86
      Departure Insertion 1 3.15 3.87 0.40
      2 1.69 2.37 0.13
      Wilcoxon 0.009** 0.008**
      Insertion Withdrawal 1 4.09 3.07 0.28
      2 1.27 1.08 0.35
      Wilcoxon 0.009** 0.008**
      Insertion Pull-out 1 3.82 1.09 0.028*
      2 1.26 0.95 0.15
      Wilcoxon 0.007** 0.17
      Arrival Insertion 1 0.18 0.56 0.60
      2 0.45 0.02 0.56
      Wilcoxon 0.68 0.87
      Arrival Infant’s mouth open 1 0.09 0.27 0.50
      2 0.20 –0.06 0.47
      Wilcoxon 0.96 0.68
      Infant’s mouth open Insertion 1 0.32 0.24 0.66
      2 0.39 0.36 0.51
      Wilcoxon 0.39 0.31
      Infant’s mouth open Mother’s mouth open 1 0.11 0.14 0.30
      2 0.35 0.37 0.74
      Wilcoxon 0.26 0.86
      Arrival Mother’s mouth open 1 0.01 0.58 0.05
      2 0.40 0.42 0.80
      Wilcoxon 0.14 0.44
      Mother’s mouth open Insertion 1 0.04 0.01 0.22
      2 0.11 0.15 1.00
      Wilcoxon 0.44 0.26

      **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

      The bold means statistical significance.

      The mother’s empathetic mouth movement was the focus of the present study, but in Table 1, the behavior did not show significant differences between ages in Japan and Scotland (Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test) or between countries at Age 1 as well as Age 2 (Mann–Whitney U test) in any of the Intervals ① – ⑥. Statistical significance was found only in the Intervals ➆ – ➉, i.e., preceding and following behaviors sandwiching the interactions containing the mouth opening. Thus there were no age or cultural differences in the length of core interactions containing the mouth openings.

      In Japan, spoon arrival time (calculated from the start of the spoon’s movement toward the infant, Interval ➈) and its insertion time (also calculated from the start of the spoon’s movement toward the infant, Interval ➉) were longer at Age 1 than at Age 2 (Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test, p’s < 0.05 and < 0.01, effect sizes = 0.66 and 0.82, n = 10, for ➈ and ➉, respectively). This means that Japanese mothers spent longer in the preparatory phase of feeding at Age 1. However, such an age difference was observed in Scotland only for spoon insertion (Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test, p’s = 0.86 and < 0.01, effect sizes = 0.06 and 0.89, n = 9, for ➈ and ➉, respectively).

      The duration of spoon-in-the-mouth (calculated as the duration between insertion and first withdrawal, Interval ➆) was also longer at Age 1 than at Age 2 in Japan (Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test, p < 0.01, effect size = 0.85, n = 10), but not significantly different between ages in Scotland (Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test, p = 0.17, effect size = 0.45, n = 9). This means that Japanese mothers gave their infants more time for food intake at Age 1. The total time from spoon insertion to withdrawal (i.e., final pulling out, Interval ➇) was shorter at Age 2 than at Age 1 in both Japan and Scotland (Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test, p < 0.01, effect size = 0.82, n = 10), suggesting a more efficient and cooperative food-taking behavior on the side of the infants and smoother, more in-step food-giving on the side of the mothers at Age 2.

      The Japan–Scotland comparison shows a significantly longer duration of the time of spoon-in-the-mouth until its withdrawal at Age 1 (calculated as the duration of the spoon in the mouth from insertion until the first spoon pulling-out, Interval ➆, Mann–Whitney U = 18, p < 0.05, effect size = 0.51, n = 19), but not at Age 2 in Japan. This indicates that Japanese mothers were more patient and appeared to wait for their young infants to take the food from the spoon when it was in the mouth than their Scottish counterparts with their 6-month-old infants. Conversely, a quicker carrying of a spoon (Interval ➈) was found in Japan than in Scotland at 9 months of age (Mann–Whitney U = 91, p = 0.01, effect size = 0.55, n = 21), but not at the earlier age. Despite their patience at the younger age, feeding times between Japanese mothers and infants became well-coordinated quickly.

      Coercive behavior (Figure 3) was seen in some mothers during the movement of the spoon toward the infant’s mouth (Interval ➈). Figure 4 shows the incidence of coercive feeding behavior in Japanese and Scottish mother-infant dyads at Ages 1 and 2. This behavior induced the infants to take the food, provided by an exaggerated shooting movement of the spoon approaching the infant’s mouth. It was significantly more frequent in Scottish mother-infant dyads at Age 1 (Fisher’s exact test, p < 0.01), but not at Age 2. This appears to indicate greater initiative in feeding by Scottish mothers.

      Coercive behavior in a Scottish mother. The mother performs a shooting movement of the spoon toward her infant.

      Coercive feeding in Japanese and the Scottish mothers at Age 1 (left) and Age 2 (right). Fisher’s exact test demonstrated significance between countries at Age 1, but not at Age 2. The differences in Ages 1 and 2 are significant (p < 0.01) and not significant, respectively.

      In contrast, Japanese mothers appeared inclined to adapt to the infants’ initiative, waiting for an adequate time for their infants to take the food. The spoon was then kept in the infants’ mouth for a significantly longer time in the Japanese dyads than in the Scottish ones at Age 1 (Table 1). This suggests that the Japanese mothers have greater patience and a stronger infant-centeredness, allowing for and following their infants’ initiative for food intake.

      Relationship Between the Infants’ Mouth Opening and Their Mothers’ Empathetic Mouth Opening

      The general analysis of the initial five spoons did not show any significant differences in the interactions relating with mothers’ empathetic mouth movement. So the behavior was further analyzed in detail by deliberately selected video episodes in which the mothers’ empathetic mouth opening was clearly observable.

      This behavior might be induced by the infant’s mouth opening (Arrow A in Figure 2), the mother’s intention to insert the spoon into her infant’s mouth at the spoon arrival might trigger it (Arrow B), or the mother’s spoon insertion might evoke her mouth opening (Arrow C). Coupling the timing of the mother’s mouth opening with that of the infant’s and the spoon arrival/insertion gives a hint to understand the mechanism underlying the mother’s empathy.

      Figure 5 is a scatter plot of the differences of the onset times between the infants’ mouth opening and that of the mothers’ empathetic mouth openings (Interval ➀; y-axis), along with the time interval from the spoon’s arrival to the mother’s mouth opening (Interval ④; x-axis) in Japan and Scotland. Data from Age 1 (blue circles) and Age 2 (red squares) are presented.

      Time differences (Sec) in the onset of the infant’s mouth opening to that of mother’s mouth opening (Interval ①; y-axis) along the time from spoon arrival to the mother’s mouth opening (Interval ④; x-axis) at Age 1 (blue) and Age 2 (red) in Japan (left) and Scotland (right). *p < 0.05, +p < 0.10.

      Time gaps between the Scottish infants’ and mothers’ mouth openings were more or less constant mostly within a 0–0.5 s range. The symbols were vertically plotted over zero for them, which means that the mothers quickly opened their mouths almost always later than the infants’ mouth opening. In comparison, the symbols for Japanese mothers scattered more widely around zero, showing that the Japanese dyads were more flexible and variable in their interaction.

      The regression line at Age 1 is horizontal in Scotland, meaning that the Scottish mothers’ mouth openings were a direct reaction to the infants’ preceding mouth opening irrespective of the length of time after spoon arrival. This is related to the mothers’ coerciveness in feeding. At Age 2, however, the time gap in Scotland decreased with the time after arrival. This reflects a change in the Scottish infants at Age 2 to start opening their mouths even before the spoon arrival by reading their mothers’ intention.

      In contrast, some Japanese mothers opened their mouths earlier than their infants’ mouth opening (the symbols plotted under zero). This means that Japanese mothers’ mouth openings were not a direct reaction to their infants’ behaviors, but were induced by the mother’s own anticipation or intention. It is notable that the time elapsed from the infant’s mouth opening to the mother’s mouth opening increased along with the length of time from spoon arrival at both Ages, which suggests that the Japanese mothers’ mouth opening was always linked with her own spoon carriage rather than evoked by the preceding infants’ mouth opening.

      Figure 6 compares the times of the infants’ mouth opening (blue circles) and the mothers’ mouth opening (red squares) after the moment of the spoon’s arrival distributed along the time elapsed from the spoon arrival to its insertion. The dashed line indicates the time of spoon insertion after arrival. If the mouth openings of the mothers and infants were induced by spoon insertion, then the lines would parallel the dashed line. When the regression lines are below the dashed line, it means that the mouth opened before spoon insertion. But if the occurrence is coupled with spoon arrival, the regression lines should be horizontal, near the zero level.

      Time (Sec) of the mouth opening by the infants (blue) and the mothers (red) after the spoon arrival (Intervals ③ and ④; y-axis) along the time elapsed after the spoon arrival to insertion (Interval ⑥; x-axis). The dashed line indicates the time of spoon insertion after arrival. **p < 0.01.

      Naturally, the infants’ mouth opened near the time of spoon insertion, and the blue lines are almost parallel to the dashed line. The red lines are also parallel to and overlap the dashed line in Scotland, which means that the Scottish mothers showed perfect co-occurrence of spoon insertion and the infant’s and mother’s mouth openings.

      On the other hand, Japanese mothers (red lines of the top two figures) showed a quite different tendency, i.e., being constantly horizontal at slightly above the zero level, which means that the Japanese mothers tended to open their mouths immediately following the spoon arrival irrespective of the time of the spoon insertion (proving Arrow B rather than A or C of Figure 2). In other words, the Japanese mothers apparently carried the spoon to the arrival point at just the right moment for the infants’ food intake judged from their infants’ state, and adjusted the spoon carriage to that state. The mothers stagnated the spoon and waited for the infant’s mouth opening if the infants unexpectedly did not show it immediately. This appears to be a more infant-centered feeding style that observes and imagines their infants’ state, anticipating their infant’s food taking. The mother’s mouth opening was thought to be caused by her empathetic motivation. In comparison, the Scottish mothers adapted the carriage of the spoon to the infants’ condition after arrival. Together with their high incidence of coercive feeding, this suggests a more assertive feeding style that attempts to lead the infants. This suggests their mouth opening was a response to the infant’s food-taking evoked by the mothers’ coercive spoon carriage (Arrow A rather than B or C of Figure 2).

      The Scottish dyads were concentrated around the left-end zone of the horizontal axis. That is, little arrival-insertion gap, indicating that the spoon was pushed directly to the infant’s mouth without preceding stagnation or adjustment. This tendency can be pointed out in Scotland at both Ages 1 and 2, but is particularly remarkable at 9 months of age. The symbols of Japanese dyads were more widely scattered because the mothers tended to be more deliberate in attuning to their infant’s initiative before they initiated carriage of the spoon for feeding.

      More constant reaction of the Scottish mothers to the infants’ mouth opening with a short interval mentioned above suggests that their intersubjectivity was directly triggered by the infants’ mouth opening. Intersubjectivity among the Japanese mothers, in contrast, appears to be based on empathy with the infants’ intentions. In other words, the Japanese mothers’ intersubjectivity was of a more anticipatory nature that followed their infant’s initiative, and the Scottish mothers’ intersubjectivity was of more directive.

      The analyses above show the connection between the mouth openings of infants and mothers. To examine the regularity of the interactions containing the mouth openings of the mother and infants, the correlation matrix of Intervals ➀ to ➅ at Age 1 and Age 2 by Spearman’s rho are shown separately for Japanese and Scottish dyads in Table 2. Regularity demonstrated by high correlations between the intervals was thought as an evidence of structuredness of the feeding sequence, which made the mothers and infants to synchronize their mutual behaviors easier.

      Correlations of length of intervals at Age 1 (Top) and Age 2 (Bottom).

      Age 1 From arrival to insertion ➅ From arrival to infant’s mouth open ➂ From infant’s mouth open to insertion ➄ From infant’s mouth open to mother’s mouth open ➀ From arrival to mother’s mouth open ➃ From mother’s mouth open to insertion ➁
      Japan From arrival to insertion ➅ 1.000 0.976** –0.399 –0.276 0.494 0.059
      From arrival to infant’s mouth open ➂ 1.000 –0.345 –0.383 0.417 0.133
      From infant’s mouth open to insertion ➄ 1.000 0.267 –0.250 0.117
      From infant’s mouth open to mother’s mouth open ➀ 1.000 0.567 −0.750*
      From arrival to mother’s mouth open ➃ 1.000 −0.783*
      From mother’s mouth open to insertion ➁ 1.000
      Scotland From arrival to insertion ➅ 1.000 0.831** –0.051 –0.034 0.695* –0.186
      From arrival to infant’s mouth open ➂ 1.000 –0.150 –0.367 0.800** 0.017
      From infant’s mouth open to insertion ➄ 1.000 0.017 –0.267 0.017
      From infant’s mouth open to mother’s mouth open ➀ 1.000 0.183 −0.833**
      From arrival to mother’s mouth open ➃ 1.000 –0.450
      From mother’s mouth open to insertion ➁ 1.000

      Age 2 From arrival to insertion ➅ From arrival to infant’s mouth open ➂ From infant’s mouth open to insertion ➄ From infant’s mouth open to mother’s mouth open ➀ From arrival to mother’s mouth open ➃ From mother’s mouth open to insertion ➁

      Japan From arrival to insertion ➅ 1.000 0.655* 0.309 –0.455 0.224 0.564
      From arrival to infant’s mouth open ➂ 1.000 –0.309 –0.624 0.115 0.527
      From infant’s mouth open to insertion ➄ 1.000 0.188 0.345 –0.006
      From infant’s mouth open to mother’s mouth open ➀ 1.000 0.539 −0.915**
      From arrival to mother’s mouth open ➃ 1.000 –0.479
      From mother’s mouth open to insertion ➁ 1.000
      Scotland From arrival to insertion ➅ 1.000 0.899** −0.795** −0.860** 0.847** –0.317
      From arrival to infant’s mouth open ➂ 1.000 −0.879** −0.758* 0.964** –0.600
      From infant’s mouth open to insertion ➄ 1.000 0.806** −0.794** 0.345
      From infant’s mouth open to mother’s mouth open ➀ 1.000 −0.648* 0.212
      From arrival to mother’s mouth open ➃ 1.000 –0.564
      From mother’s mouth open to insertion ➁ 1.000

      **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

      The bold means statistical significance.

      The most conspicuous result is the high ratio of significant correlations among the intervals at 9 months in Scotland. Particularly, there was an increase of significant correlations in the combinations with the infants’ mouth opening in Scottish dyads compared to Japanese ones. This means that the time series of behaviors was more structured and therefore predictable in Scotland at Age 2.

      We interpret this higher correlation as a result of reduced lability on the infant behavior due to the coercive maternal feeding style. This gave a compelling, structured interaction. Thus, the Scottish infants were unable to adjust their behaviors to this feeding style at Age 1, and later came to adjust their behavior to the mothers’ feeding at Age 2.

      In contrast, the Japanese mothers allowed greater freedom for their infants, which was possible by the more sensitively attuned feeding by the mothers that followed their infants and made the interaction more flexible. In Japan, the interactions became more complex, because the infants’ spontaneity was prioritized and thereby interactions were more diverse. Thus the correlation of the Japanese dyads remained low at Age 2.

      Discussion

      The results indicated that Japanese mothers were more attuned to their infants’ states during feeding when the infants were at Age 1. In contrast, the Scottish mothers were more assertive in the spoon carriage with displays of coercive behavior with their 6-month-old infants. The mothers of both countries frequently opened their mouths when their infants’ mouths opened to take food. The Scottish mothers’ mouth openings occurred as a response to the infants’ mouth opening by the spoon insertion, which itself was initiated by their more directive, mother-centered style of feeding. In contrast, Japanese mothers opened their mouths by empathic anticipation, encouraging their infants’ food-taking behavior based on a more affective infant-centeredness.

      Synchrony and Intersubjectivity

      Temporal coordination in the mother-infant interaction during feeding is guaranteed by the infants’ timely mouth opening at the moment of spoon insertion and the mothers’ adjustment of their behavior to the infants’ states.

      The infants opened their mouths almost simultaneously but slightly earlier than the spoon insertion. This behavior was possible because of the infants’ anticipation of the insertion, to which Tau Theory might be applicable for an explanation of the perception of gap-closure (Lee, 1976; Lee, 2009) operative in infant perception and action (Agyei et al., 2016; Delafield-Butt et al., 2018). This could be an underlying mechanism of the intersubjectivity between infants and mothers.

      The infants responsively adjusted their mouth behavior to the mothers’ spoon carriage. Mothers also carried the spoon at the right moment for coordinated action and common purpose with their infants, which requires the mother to perceive her infant’s state of readiness, interest, and intention correctly. As the present study demonstrates, mothers open their mouths to their infants’ food intake. This mother–infant symmetry of behaviors is strong evidence of their intersubjectivity. The mothers gave food to the infants, but simultaneously behave as if they were being fed as well, which shows shared feeling. Mothers’ mouth opening occurred slightly later than the mouth opening of their infants, a feature more evident in Scotland.

      Maternal experience of mother–infant intersubjectivity is different from, but deeply related to their infant’s experience of that same intersubjectivity (Trevarthen, 2001). Food-providing by mothers is a uniquely human behavior, allowing the mothers’ intersubjectivity and the infants’ intersubjectivity to intersect in a clear and explicit manner in body movement and overt behavior. According to Trevarthen and Hubley (1978) and Trevarthen (1998), infants acquire secondary intersubjectivity at 9 months, characterized by a genuine triadic relationship (Tomasello, 1995; Negayama, forthcoming). The well-coordinated feeding of the Scottish mothers and infants at 9 months despite the mothers’ coercive feeding at 6 months as well as the Japanese infants’ quicker food taking at 9 months than at 6 months can be explained by the developing attunement of the 9-month-old infants to the shared object, the spoon.

      Co-occurrence of the same mouth openings (mirroring) in mothers and infants is another unique characteristic of human feeding. The synchronous mouth opening by a feeder was also observed in an infant’s father and a 2-year-old sibling at feeding infant (Negayama, 2006). Joint attention of the mother and the infant to an object in a triadic relationship requires taking the other’s perspective (Tomasello et al., 1993). This allows for infant learning the particular mindedness of his or her mother. Learning such perspective-taking is embodied within the intersubjective engagement between the mother and infant.

      Tickling play is another interaction between mother and infant with joint attention to a part of the infant’s body, which similarly demonstrates this proto-triadic relationship evident at 6 months (Negayama, 2012). Feeding is another special, preliminary type of triadic relationship between mothers, infants, and food as a target. Food stimulates the infants’ naso-oral sense organs, but it also arouses the mothers’ taste and tactile naso-oral sensations. This is another dimension of mother-infant empathy, enriching the intersubjective triadic relationship with imagined expectations and real sensory experiences of taste, naso-oral sensations and sensorimotor tasks. And mother and infant ultimately share in the pleasure or displeasure in the accomplishment of the shared project (Delafield-Butt, 2018). This resonant relationship of intercorporeality can be understood as “embodied simulation” (Gallese, 2009), and the embodied triadic relationship is called a quasi-triadic relationship (Negayama, forthcoming; Negayama and Nakano, in press). Feeding shares the same basis of learning in co-created, co-operative projects that form the foundation of shared meaning-making invariant in human life (Trevarthen and Delafield-Butt, 2015; Delafield-Butt and Trevarthen, 2020).

      Age and Cultural Differences

      The Scottish mothers appeared more coercive and took the initiative to provide food at 6 months of age, and their infants followed them. Furthermore, their tendency to push a spoon into the infant’s mouth without a preceding stagnation or adjustment sensitive to their infant’s intention was observed. The Scottish mothers were more mother-centered, and the infants adjusted their behavior to their mothers. This means that the process was framed by the mothers’ leadership, and the structure became simpler because of the infants’ compliance.

      In contrast, there was a greater flexibility in Japanese mothers, which was related to their long and deliberate spoon carriage by their infant-centeredness and cooperativeness. The Japanese mothers in the infant pick-up experiment of this study were also gentler and more deliberate with kneeling and had a slower approach before picking up the infants than the Scottish mothers, and the Scottish mothers were quicker in crouching to pick up the infants than the Japanese mothers (Negayama et al., 2015). Therefore, the Japanese mothers’ intersubjectivity was framed by expectation of the infants’ food-taking rather than mirroring of the infants’ behavior.

      The Japanese mothers’ mouth opened with a constant length of time after arrival, irrespective of the time to insertion. In other words, the mothers started the carriage at an appropriate time for food intake and did not need to adjust the movement to the infant state after departure, because the two were already aligned: the Japanese infants were more autonomous than the Scottish infants, and the Japanese mothers followed their infants and waited for the right time to bring the spoon of food for feeding by monitoring them.

      It is also notable that some Japanese mothers opened their mouths earlier than the infants’ mouth opening. These could be signs of anticipatory intersubjectivity among the Japanese dyads, and the infants would gradually learn the same psychological trait of intention-reading through the above-mentioned resonant experience between mother and infant at 6 months of age.

      Such empathy-driven intersubjectivity is more likely to characterize Japanese dyads with infant-centeredness in their mothers. Japanese mothers’ strong empathy was also pointed out when putting the infants to sleep with physical contact. This was to avoid a sadness or hardship on both sides by being left alone (Negayama, 1997), which could be related to ‘Amae’ (Doi, 1992). In Scotland, on the other hand, the mothers’ strong coerciveness during feeding played an important role in its shared timing or synchrony. This is similar with Scottish mothers’ preference for isolated sleeping in a bed separate from their infants (Negayama, 2006, forthcoming). The Scottish mothers then mirrored their infants’ mouth opening, generating a reactive intersubjectivity. At 9 months, i.e., the age of secondary intersubjectivity and genuine triadic relationship, however, the Scottish infants became to open their mouth before the spoon insertion by reading the mother’s intention.

      Intersubjectivity is the basis of coordination of feelings, intentions, and desires between bodies (Trevarthen, 2001). This coordination of movement and voice develops a shared project, or narrative, that allows for the development of inter-personal and cultural meaning (Gratier and Trevarthen, 2008; Malloch and Trevarthen, 2009b; Trevarthen and Delafield-Butt, 2013; Delafield-Butt and Trevarthen, 2015; Delafield-Butt, 2018). Mother-infant feeding timing and synchronization of mouth movements serve as indicators, or physical expressions of an embodied intersubjective resonance between them, serving to structure and organize the shared project. The different elements of timing, anticipation, and empathetic mirroring underpin the process. In this paper, we have identified differences in these psycho-motor structures that illustrate two different cultural types of intersubjectivity in Japan and Scotland, and suggested how these become culturally transmitted, and learned by the next generation.

      Intentions are carried in movement, and the source of those intentions (empathetic or directive, as we have found here) becomes apparent in their shared experience. This allows learning of the particular maternal characteristics of mind-mindedness (Meins et al., 2002), and the transmission of a culture. Murray et al. (2018) postulate neurofunctional architecture which increases an occurrence of infant behavior when a caregiver mirrors the infant behavior. This functional architecture is a part of intersubjectivity of infants to respond in certain ways to specific forms of parental display (Murray et al., 2016). Bozicevic et al. (2021) demonstrate that cultural difference in maternal responsiveness mediate difference in the infant’s later communicative behavior.

      Two different types of parenting have been repeatedly pointed out in contexts other than feeding: control or regulator type and warmth or facilitator type (Raphael-Leff, 1993; Azuma, 1994; Bornstein, 2015). These studies show Japanese parenting can be classified as the latter type, relying more on affective ties and empathy than parental control. On the other hand, Western parenting can be relatively classified as the former type. Our present result follows this dichotomy, and the different characteristics of intersubjectivity in feeding between Japan and Scotland described here are embodied in it. Japanese mothers’ lack of happiness and satisfaction in their motherhood compared to French and American mothers (Negayama et al., 2012) could be related to this limited autonomy in mothers.

      Study Limitations and Future Work

      First of all, this study is based on a small sample size, and the findings should hence be taken with caution despite an availability of other studies providing support for the current conclusion. Non-parametric tests applied to the small sample size have limited what we could determine. It should also be noted that this study took place in semi-naturalistic conditions within a public, laboratory setting in front of video and motion capture cameras. Although the mothers were instructed to behave normally, they were on stage in public, and this may have affected their intersubjective posture with their infants. The performance we observed may have been a publicly acceptable form of interaction that might have differed from that in private. Future studies will confirm these results with unobtrusive video. Recent advances in markerless motion capture technology for more naturalistic data collection could test for potential differences between public and private forms of intersubjective cooperation in feeding, and in other shared behaviors.

      Further Perspectives

      Eating is not just an activity where children take food to their mouths and swallow it. Instead, it is a cluster of behaviors related to parent-child relationships in a broader context. Feeding is a social activity involving both cooperation and antagonism between mothers and infants. Infants’ desires about what, when, and how they eat do not always accord with the expectations of their mothers. Parents and infants sometimes conflict with each other and try to read each other’s intentions and negotiate. The onset of refusal behavior by children in the transition from dependent to independent eating is a meaningful change (Negayama, 2011). This happens at about 9 months of age, the beginning of the genuine triadic relationship (Negayama, 1993).

      Children trying to eat in the way they want and rejecting their parents’ control frustrates their parents who want to feed them properly. The children read the parents’ intentions and try to manipulate them. This is related to parents’ pushy feeding or coercive control, showing the conflicting nature of eating as a situation of mutual manipulation by the parents and the children (Jansen et al., 2017).

      Through such negotiation and co-regulation, children establish a parent-child centrifugalism, promoting independence from their parents. It is not a coincidence that these changes co-occur with the development of secondary intersubjectivity at 9 months of age. Kawata (2014) explained this kind of children’s protest for autonomy in eating as “psychological reactance” (Brehm and Brehm, 2013). Companionship between mother and infant develops on the basis of this co-regulation (Trevarthen, 2006).

      Data Availability Statement

      The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

      Ethics Statement

      The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of Waseda University (No. 2012-273) and the University of Strathclyde. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants, in the case of adults, and the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin, in the case of the infants. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) and minor(s)’ legal guardian/next of kin for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

      Author Contributions

      KN contributed to the conception and design of the study, led data collection in Japan, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JD-B contributed to the design of the study, led data collection in Scotland, and co-authored and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to methodology development and data collection. KN and KM performed the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to observation and manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s Note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Funding

      This project was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Number 23653193 to KN, MEXT KIBANKEISEI Grant Number S1001028 to H. Kumano, and a JSPS Visiting Fellowship and BTG grant from the University of Strathclyde to JD-B.

      We thank the mothers and their babies for their time and participation, without whom this work would not have been possible.

      References Agyei S. B. van der Weel F. R. R. van der Meer A. L. H. (2016). Development of visual motion perception for prospective control: brain and behavioral studies in infants. Front. Psychol. 7:100. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00100 26903908 Azuma H. (1994). Education and Socialization in Japan: A Comparison Between Japan and the United States. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. Bornstein M. H. (2015). “Children’s parents,” in Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, 7th Edn, eds Bornstein M. H. Leventhal T. (New Jersey NJ: Wiley), 55132. Bozicevic L. De Pascalis L. Montirosso R. Ferrari P. F. Giusti L. Cooper P. J. (2021). Sculpting culture: early maternal responsiveness and child emotion regulation – a UK-Italy comparison. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 52 2242. 10.1177/0022022120971353 Brehm S. S. Brehm J. W. (2013). Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. New York, NY: Academic Press. Caudill W. Weinstein H. (1969). Maternal care and infant behavior in Japan and America. Psychiatry 32 1243. 10.1080/00332747.1969.11023572 5779087 Clay Z. Palagi E. de Waal F. B. M. (2018). “Ethological approaches to empathy in primates,” in Neuronal Correlates of Empathy: From Rodent to Human, eds Meyza K. Z. Knapska E. (London: Academic Press), 5366. 10.1016/B978-0-12-805397-3.00005-X Cole N. C. Musaad S. M. Lee S.-Y. Donovana S. M. The S. T. R. O. N. G., and Kids Team. (2018). Home feeding environment and picky eating behavior in preschool-aged children: a prospective analysis. Eat. Behav. 30 7682. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.06.003 29894927 Connolly K. Dalgleish M. (1989). The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 25 894912. 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894 de Waal F. B. M. Preston S. D. (2017). Mammalian empathy: behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18 498509. 10.1038/nrn.2017.72 28655877 Delafield-Butt J. (2018). “The emotional and embodied nature of human understanding: sharing narratives of meaning,” in The Child’s Curriculum: Working with the Natural Voices of Young Children, eds Trevarthen C. Delafield-Butt J. Dunlop A.-W. (Oxford: Oxford University Press). 10.1093/oso/9780198747109.003.0004 33782627 Delafield-Butt J. Trevarthen C. (2020). “Infant intentions: learning with others,” in Encyclopedia of Teacher Education, ed. Peter M. (Singapore: Springer Nature). 10.1007/978-981-13-1179-6_74-1 Delafield-Butt J. T. Freer Y. Perkins J. Skulina D. Schögler B. Lee D. N. (2018). Prospective organization of neonatal arm movements: a motor foundation of embodied agency, disrupted in premature birth. Dev. Sci. 21:e12693. 10.1111/desc.12693 29920860 Delafield-Butt J. T. Gangopadhyay N. (2013). Sensorimotor intentionality: the origins of intentionality in prospective agent action. Dev. Rev. 33 399425. 10.1016/j.dr.2013.09.001 Delafield-Butt J. T. Trevarthen C. (2015). The ontogenesis of narrative: from moving to meaning. Front. Psychol. 6:01157. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01157 26388789 Di Cesare G. De Stefani E. Gentilucci M. De Marco D. (2017). Vitality forms expressed by others modulate our own motor response: a kinematic study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:565. 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00565 29204114 Di Cesare G. Gerbella M. Rizzolatti G. (2020). The neural bases of vitality forms. Natl. Sci. Rev. 7 202213. 10.1093/nsr/nwz187 Doi T. (1992). On the concept of Amae. Infant Mental Health J. 13 711. 10.1002/1097-0355(199221)13:1<7::AID-IMHJ2280130103>3.0.CO;2-E ELAN (Version 5.9.1) [Computer software] (2020). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, The Language Archive. Available online at https://archive.mpi.nl/tla/elan (accessed March 30, 2020). Fantasia V. De Jaegher H. Fasulo A. (2014). We can work it out: an enactive look at cooperation. Front. Psychol. 5:874. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00874 25152745 Feldman R. (2003). Infant-mother and infant-father synchrony: the co-regulation of positive arousal. Infant Mental Health J. 24 123. 10.1002/imhj.10041 Feldman R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing: physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48 329354. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x 17355401 Ferrari P. F. Coudé G. (2018). “Mirror neurons, embodied emotions, and empathy,” in Neuronal Correlates of Empathy: From Rodent to Human, eds Meyza K. Z. Knapska E. (London: Academic Press), 6777. 10.1016/B978-0-12-805397-3.00006-1 Ferrari P. F. Gallese V. Rizzolatti G. Fogassi L. (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17 17031714. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02601.x 12752388 Fries L. R. Martin N. van der Horst K. (2017). Parent-child mealtime interactions associated with toddlers’ refusals of novel and familiar foods. Physiol. Behav. 176 93100. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.001 28315360 Gallagher S. (2012). “Neurons, neonates, and narrative: from embodied resonance to empathic understanding,” in Moving Ourselves: Bodily Motion and Emotion in the Making of Intersubjectivity and Consciousness, eds Foolen A. Lüdtke U. Zlatev J. Racine T. (Amsterdam: John Benjamins). 10.1075/ceb.6.07gal 33486653 Gallese V. (2009). Mirror neurons, embodied simulation, and the neural basis of social identification. Psychoanal. Dial. 19 519536. 10.1080/10481880903231910 Gratier M. (2003). Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cogn. Dev. 18 533554. 10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.009 Gratier M. Trevarthen C. (2008). Musical narratives and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction. J. Consciousness Stud. 15 122158. Ishijima K. Negayama K. (2013). Mother-infant interaction in tickling play: intention-reading based on narrative sharing. Japanese J. Dev. Psychol. 24 326336. Jansen P. W. de Barse L. M. Jaddoe V. W. V. Verhulst F. C. Franco O. H. Tiemeier H. (2017). Bi-directional associations between child fussy eating and parents’ pressure to eat: who influences whom? Physiol. Behav. 176 101106. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.02.015 28215424 Kawahara N. (2005). Development of eating with tool in toddlers. Japanese J. Appl. Psychol. 31 98112. Kawata M. (2014). Primordium Mechanism of the Development of Self in Infancy: Origin of Objective Self and a Hinge Effect of Triadic Relationship. Kyoto: Nakanishiya Shuppan. Lee D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception 5 437459. 10.1068/p050437 1005020 Lee D. N. (2009). General Tau theory: evolution to date. Perception 38 837850. 10.1068/pmklee 19806967 Malloch S. Trevarthen C. (eds) (2009a). Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Malloch S. Trevarthen C. (2009b). “Musicality: communicating the vitality and interests of life,” in Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship, eds Malloch S. Trevarthen C. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 112. Meins E. Fernyhough C. Wainwright R. Das Gupta M. Fradley E. Tuckey M. (2002). Maternal mind–mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Dev. 73 17151726. 10.1111/1467-8624.00501 12487489 Meyza K. Z. Knapska E. (2018). Neuronal Correlates of Empathy: From Rodent to Human. London: Academic Press. Murray L. Bozicevic L. Ferrari P. F. Vaillancourt K. Dalton L. Goodacre T. (2018). The effects of maternal mirroring on the development of infant social expressiveness: the case of infant cleft lip. Hindawi Neural Plasticity 2018 110. 10.1155/2018/5314657 30647731 Murray L. De Pascalis L. Bozicevic L. Hawkins L. Sclafani V. Ferrari P. F. (2016). The functional architecture of mother-infant communication, and the development of infant social expressiveness in the first two months. Sci. Rep. 6:39019. 10.1038/srep39019 27966659 Negayama K. (1993). Weaning in Japan: a longitudinal study of mother and child behaviours during milk- and solid-feeding. Early Dev. Parent. 2 2937. 10.1002/edp.2430020106 Negayama K. (1996). “Eating behaviour up to weaning,” in Eating, eds Nakajima Y. Imada S. (Tokyo: Asakura shoten). Negayama K. (1997). “Parent-child relationship and independence: focusing on Japan-U.K. comparison,” in Cultural Psychology: Theory and proof, eds Kitayama K. K. Azuma S. H. (Tokyo: Tokyo University Press), 160179. Negayama K. (2000). Feeding as a communication between mother and infant in Japan and Scotland. Ann. Rep. Res. Clin. Center Child Dev. 22 5968. Negayama K. (2006). Child-care as Kowakare. Tokyo: NHK Books. Negayama K. (2011). Kowakare: a new perspective on the development of early mother-offspring relationship. Int. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 45 8599. 10.1007/s12124-010-9148-1 21161454 Negayama K. (2012). “Bodily contact as the basis of interindividual relationship,” in The Basis of Development: Body, Cognition and Emotion, eds Negayama K. Naka M. (Tokyo: Shin-Yo-Sha), 119130. Negayama K. (forthcoming). Parent-infant Centrifugalism and Centripetalism: Overcoming the Prison of “Parenting”. Tokyo: Waseda University Press. Negayama K. Delafield-Butt J. T. Momose K. Ishijima K. Kawahara N. Lux E. J. (2015). Embodied intersubjective engagement in mother–infant tactile communication: a cross-cultural study of Japanese and Scottish mother–infant behaviors during infant pick-up. Front. Psychol. 6:66. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00066 25774139 Negayama K. Kawahara N. (2010). Cross-cultural comparison of nursery staff’s tactics to put children into sleep between Japan and Scotland. Japan J. Med. Psychol. Study Infants 19 117123. Negayama K. Nakano S. (in press). “Co-players in companionship,” in Rhythm, Sympathy, and Human Being: Celebrating the Rhythms, Sympathies, and Many Beings of Colwyn Trevarthen, eds Delafield-Butt J. Reddy V. (Oxford: Oxford University Press). Negayama K. Norimatsu H. Barratt M. Jean-François B. (2012). Japan–France–US comparison of infant weaning from mother’s viewpoint. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 30 7791. 10.1080/02646838.2011.649473 22745518 Negayama K. Trevarthen C. (under review). Mother-infant co-regulation of distance across age and culture. Nonaka T. Stoffregen T. A. (2020). Social interaction in the emergence of toddler’s mealtime spoon use. Dev. Psychobiol. 62 11241133. 10.1002/dev.21978 32383216 Norimatsu H. (1993). Development of child autonomy in eating and toilet training: one-to three-year-old Japanese and French children. Early Dev. Parent. 2 3950. 10.1002/edp.2430020107 Pliner P. (1994). Development of measures of food neophobia in children. Appetite 23 147163. 10.1006/appe.1994.1043 7864609 Raphael-Leff J. (1993). Facilitators and regulators: two approaches to mothering. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 56 379390. 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1983.tb01571.x 6661409 Reddy V. Markova G. Wallot S. (2013). Anticipatory adjustments to being picked up in infancy. PLoS One 8:e65289. 10.1371/journal.pone.0065289 23840324 Rossmanith N. Costall A. Reichelt A. F. López B. Reddy V. (2014). Jointly structuring triadic spaces of meaning and action: book sharing from 3 months on. Front. Psychol. 5:1390. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01390 25540629 Stern D. N. (2010). Forms of Vitality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/med:psych/9780199586066.001.0001 Stevenson M. B. Roach M. A. Ver Hoeve J. N. Leavitt L. A. (1990). Rhythms in the dialogue of infant feeding: preterm and term infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 13 5170. 10.1016/0163-6383(90)90005-S Tomasello M. (1995). “Joint attention as social cognition,” in Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development, eds Moore C. Dunham P. J. (Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates), 103130. Tomasello M. Kruger A. C. Ratner H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 16 495552. 10.1017/S0140525X0003123X Tovar A. Vaughn A. E. Fallon M. Hennessy E. Burney R. Østbye T. (2016). Providers’ response to child eating behaviors: a direct observation study. Appetite 105 534541. 10.1016/j.appet.2016.06.020 27328098 Toyama N. (2014). The development of Japanese mother–infant feeding interactions during the weaning period. Infant Behav. Dev. 37 203215. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.01.002 24607573 Trevarthen C. (1998). “The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity,” in Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, ed. Bråten S. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 1546. Trevarthen C. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42 348. 10.1111/1469-7610.00701 Trevarthen C. (2006). “Stepping away from the mirror: pride and shame in adventures of companionship,” in Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis (Dahlem Workshop Reports), eds Sue Carter C. Ahnert L. Grossmann K. E. Hrdy S. B. Lamb M. E. Porges S. W. (Cambridge: MIT Press), 5584. Trevarthen C. (2016). The spiritual nature of the infant self: an imaginative actor in relations of affection. J. Consciousness Stud. 23 258282. Trevarthen C. Aitken K. J. Vandekerckhove M. Delafield-Butt J. Nagy E. (2006). “Collaborative regulations of vitality in early childhood: stress in intimate relationships and postnatal psychopathology,” in Developmental Psychopathology, eds Cicchetti D. Cohen D. J. (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 65126. 10.1002/9780470939390.ch2 Trevarthen C. Delafield-Butt J. T. (2013). “Biology of shared meaning and language development: regulating the life of narratives,” in The Infant Mind: Origins of the Social Brain, eds Legerstee M. Haley D. Bornstein M. (New York, NY: Guildford Press), 167199. Trevarthen C. Delafield-Butt J. T. (2015). “The infant’s creative vitality, in projects of self-discovery and shared meaning: how they anticipate school, and make it fruitful,” in International Handbook of Young Children’s Thinking and Understanding, eds Robson S. Quinn S. F. (Abingdon, Oxfordshire & New York, NY: Routledge). Trevarthen C. Delafield-Butt J. T. (2017). “Development of consciousness,” in Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development, eds Hopkins B. Geangu E. Linkenauger S. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 821835. 10.1017/9781316216491.131 Trevarthen C. Hubley P. (1978). “Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year,” in Action, Gesture and Symbol: the Emergence of Language, ed. Lock A. (London: Academic Press), 183229. Trivers R. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. Am. Zool. 14 249264. 10.1093/icb/14.1.249 Wright P. (1989). “Feeding experiences in early infancy,” in Handbook of the Psychophysiology of Human Eating, ed. Shepherd R. (Chichester: John Wiley), 157178.
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016fismall.com.cn
      www.mggzow.com.cn
      lgtozt.com.cn
      www.longtuty.org.cn
      gzchst.com.cn
      www.rpsrdi.com.cn
      ucersh.com.cn
      www.sbgdss.org.cn
      www.tjftz.com.cn
      www.wzchain.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p