Edited by: Roberto Therón, University of Salamanca, Spain
Reviewed by: Behzad Hassannezhad Kashani, Imam Reza International University, Iran; Anastasia G. Kononova, Michigan State University, United States
This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Watching vlogs of social media influencers has become a favorite pastime for children and adolescents. For advertisers, vlogs are an excellent way to reach young viewers. As such, vlogs have become a powerful marketing tool. However, for children and adolescents it is often unclear whether a vlog contains advertising, which raises questions regarding the fairness and transparency of this type of advertising. If children do not recognize the commercial intent of in-vlog advertising, then they are unlikely to activate their advertising literacy, which may serve as a critical coping mechanism. The aim of this study was to investigate if a sponsorship disclosure stimulates children and adolescents’ (7–16 years old) to activate their advertising literacy when exposed to embedded advertising in vlogs and, subsequently, if advertising literacy activation is related to children’s brand attitude. Furthermore, we investigated whether the relation between exposure to a sponsorship disclosure and advertising literacy activation was moderated by children’s dispositional advertising literacy and their age. An innovative aspect of the current study is that advertising literacy activation was measured in two ways: with a self-reported questionnaire and via an indirect measurement task (Advertising Literacy Activation Task). The results showed that the children who were exposed to a sponsorship disclosure did not activate their advertising literacy to a higher extent than the children who were not exposed to such a disclosure. This might be because of the high prominence of the brand in the vlog; thus children may not have needed the disclosure to realize that the vlog was sponsored and accordingly activate their advertising literacy. The results also showed that stronger attitudinal advertising literacy activation led to a more negative brand attitude. Interestingly, this effect was only found when attitudinal advertising literacy was assessed with a questionnaire and not when it was assessed with the indirect measurement task. Thus, children who were more critical toward the in-vlog advertisement through self-reporting also had a more negative brand attitude. This suggests that direct and indirect measurements of advertising literacy activation reveal different processes through which children make sense of, and are affected by, advertising.
香京julia种子在线播放
Watching online video’s is one of the favorite leisure time activities of today’s children and teens. They watch online video’s primarily on YouTube, but other platforms, such as Instagram, Snapchat, and the fast growing video-sharing platform TikTok, are used as well (
However, for children, the commercial intent of in-vlog advertising is often unclear because the commercial message is fully embedded in the entertaining content (
In order to help children better recognize in-vlog advertising and activate their advertising literacy while viewing it, vloggers are required to add a sponsorship disclosure to their video if it contains commercial content (e.g.,
The first aim of this study was to investigate if a disclosure can stimulate children’s advertising literacy activation when they are exposed to in-vlog advertising. Furthermore, we explored whether the relation between exposure to the disclosure and advertising literacy activation is moderated by children’s dispositional advertising literacy and their age. Our second aim was to investigate whether and how advertising literacy activation is related to children’s responses to the advertised brand (i.e., brand attitude). We focus on 7- to 16-year-olds because children in this age group are most interested in watching vlogs (
In this study, we adopt an innovative approach to measuring activation of children’s advertising literacy. Research on children’s advertising literacy activation traditionally measures this concept using self-reported questionnaires (e.g.,
Advertising literacy, also referred to as persuasion knowledge (
To investigate if a disclosure can stimulate children to activate their dispositional advertising literacy when they are exposed to in-vlog advertising, the present study draws on insights from information-processing theory (
The information that is stored in long-term memory is believed to be organized in mental structures called schemas (
When combining the perspectives of information-processing and schema theories, they predict that exposure to advertising, which serves as a cue for information retrieval, should result in the activation of the advertising schema, including the subschema containing conceptual and attitudinal advertising literacy. As such, exposure to advertising could lead to the activation of children’s dispositional advertising literacy. However, information-processing theory also predicts that activating relevant information can be difficult for children. Even though information retrieval as a results of exposure to media content is largely automatic (
The activation of advertising literacy can be stimulated by exposing children to a ‘retrieval’ cue (
In order for children to activate their conceptual and attitudinal advertising literacy, these types of advertising literacy have to be present in the first place. When children do not have certain knowledge about the persuasive and selling intent of advertising in general (i.e., dispositional conceptual advertising literacy) or do not have a general skeptical attitude toward advertising (i.e., dispositional attitudinal advertising literacy), they will also not be able to activate this knowledge or attitude when they are exposed to advertising. Therefore, we argue that there will be an interaction effect between exposure to the disclosure and children’s dispositional advertising literacy. Specifically, we expect that the effect of the disclosure on advertising literacy activation will be stronger for children with higher levels of dispositional advertising literacy. This leads to the second hypothesis:
Insights in children’s cognitive development suggest that activating relevant information from long-term memory is more difficult for younger children (i.e., younger than 12 years) than for older children (i.e., 12 years and older). As such, the processing of in-vlog advertising can differ markedly between younger and older children (
The prefrontal brain plays a significant role in children’s ability to activate and retrieve relevant information from memory (
To investigate whether and how advertising literacy activation is related to children’s responses to the advertised brand (i.e., brand attitude), the present study draws on insights from the Persuasion Knowledge Model (PKM,
Earlier research has found that, among adults, a better understanding of sponsored content resulted in a more critical attitude toward not only the sponsored content but also toward the brands (
Earlier research on the relationship between
The conceptual models for the hypotheses can be found in
Conceptual model for hypotheses related to conceptual advertising literacy.
Conceptual model for hypotheses related to attitudinal advertising literacy.
Over the last 10 years, several scholars have worked on the development of a valid and reliable measurement tool to assess children’s dispositional advertising literacy (e.g.,
An issue with measuring children’s advertising literacy activation in this manner is that it is based on self-report questionnaires. Although questionnaires are suitable for assessing the dispositional dimensions of advertising literacy, they are considered less appropriate for assessing advertising literacy activation (
An alternative way in which children’s activation of advertising literacy can be measured, and a way with which the disadvantages of self-report can be avoided, is the use of indirect measures (e.g., reaction time measures). The use of indirect measures is very common in schema activation research (e.g.,
In the current study, in addition to a self-reported measure, a new indirect measurement task was used to assess children’s advertising literacy activation: The Advertising Literacy Activation Task (ALAT;
In the ALAT, the categorization task is designed in such a way that children are asked to categorize targets, in this case advertising-related words (e.g., buy), non-advertising related words (e.g., weather) and attitudinal words (e.g., boring) into one of two categories: advertising or news. The ALAT is an indirect measurement task, because it does not directly ask participants questions regarding their advertising literacy activation, but indirectly assesses it from their reactions on the task (
To explore if the way in which advertising literacy activation is measured (i.e., indirect measurement vs. self-report) affects the hypothesized relationships as depicted in
RQ: To what extent do the hypothesized relationships between the sponsorship disclosure, children’s level of advertising literacy activation and their responses to the advertised brand differ when advertising literacy activation is measured via an indirect measurement task compared to a self-report questionnaire?
We conducted an experiment with a one-factor (disclosure vs. no disclosure) design. The experiment was conducted over 2 weeks in July 2018 in a science museum for children in Amsterdam, the Netherlands. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Social Sciences Department at Radboud University, the Netherlands (ECSW2017-1303-492).
In total, 289 participants took part in the experiment. Thirty-one participants were excluded from the data analysis for one of the following reasons: (1) They did not sufficiently master the Dutch language, (2) The manipulation failed (e.g., the participant saw both versions of the stimulus material), (3) They did not finish the entire experiment, (4) They did not fall within the age range for this study (i.e., younger than 7 years old or older than 16 years old), or (5) Their performance on the indirect measurement task was not good enough (i.e., more than 25% of their data was removed after outlier analysis). Of the participants that remained (
Data collection took place in the science museum, which has a designated area where scholars are allowed to do research. This research area functioned as a research lab where the experiment was conducted under controlled conditions. The research area consists of two rooms: one room where participants and their parents were given details about the study and parents signed the consent form and one room where the children actually participated in the study. In the second room, four desks were each equipped with a laptop, a headphone, a mouse, and a button box. Recruitment of the participants took place inside the museum. Participants were told that the study was about vlogs and the way that people process information presented in vlogs. When they (and their parents) agreed to participate, they were taken to the area where the study took place. The consent form was signed by a parent or guardian for all children. After signing the consent form, participants were taken to the room where the actual study was conducted. They were positioned behind one of the laptops and asked to put on the headphone.
In the first part of the study, participants watched a vlog that contained either a disclosure at the beginning of the vlog (experimental condition) or not (control condition). Next, they performed the indirect measurement task to assess their advertising literacy activation. Finally they filled out a questionnaire. The experiment took approximately 20 min to complete. After completion, participants were thanked for their contribution and given a debriefing letter which explained the purpose of the study. None of the participants received an incentive for participating in the study.
The vlog used in this study was selected on the basis of three selection criteria. First, the vlog needed to be attractive for children of all ages (e.g., we did not want to use a vlog that would be perceived as being childish). Second, the product/brand in the vlog needed to be attractive for children of all ages. Third, the product was preferably easy to buy for the participants (e.g., not too expensive and easily available). Based on these criteria, we selected a video of a Dutch male vlogger, who has approximately 146,000 followers on YouTube. In the vlog, he visits a popular pizza chain to make pizzas for a (sponsored) pizza lunch at a secondary school. He then visits the school where the children have to complete a set of challenges in order to win the lunch. The original vlog lasted 7 min and 3 s. For the purpose of this study some parts of the vlog were removed (e.g., a fragment in which the vlogger drives to the school, which was not necessary for the narrative). The final version of the vlog used in this study lasted 4 min and 36 s.
The selected vlog includes several features which are typical for (sponsored) vlogs. First, the vlogger films himself using two main camera angles: one where the camera is focused on the environment (e.g., the vloggers’ house or a shopping street) and another that’s selfie-style. Second, the vlogger films himself in ordinary settings (e.g., on the street, in a pizza store, in a class-room) while constantly providing engaging, improvised commentary as he films. Third, the vlog shows a give-away, which is an persuasive tactic that is often used in sponsored vlogs. The give-away consists of winning a pizza lunch from the sponsoring pizza chain for the entire class.
From one of our earlier studies (
For the experimental condition, we added a disclosure at the start of the video because a disclosure at the beginning of a video is more effective than a disclosure concurrent with the product placement (
Children’s advertising literacy activation was assessed with both an indirect measurement task (the ALAT) and a (direct) self-reported questionnaire measure.
Directly after watching the video, children performed the indirect measurement task: The Advertising Literacy Activation Task (ALAT) as described in
Words used in the Advertising Literacy Activation Task.
Practice | Advertising | News | Attitudinal |
Order | Product | Jeugdjournaal1 | Annoying |
Purchase | Advertisement | NOS | Boring |
Brand | Discount | Journaal2 | Fun |
Pay | Price | Reporter | Interesting |
Journalist | Buy | Event | Funny |
Studio | Persuade | Weather | Stupid |
Domestic | Offer | Informative | Fake |
Foreign | Stuff | Countries | Mislead |
Tempt | Information | Lies | |
Store | Anchor | Lying |
Both accuracy and speed of categorizing the advertising-related words were considered as indicators for advertising literacy activation. More accurate and faster categorization of the advertising-related words shows a higher level of advertising literacy activation (
In the final dataset, seconds were converted to milliseconds. Outliers in response time were removed via the method described by
We created four difference scores: two for conceptual advertising literacy activation and two for attitudinal advertising literacy activation. The difference score for categorization of the conceptual words (i.e., conceptual advertising literacy activation) was calculated by subtracting the value on the categorization of the news words (incongruent words) from the value on the categorization of the advertising words (congruent words). A higher score would then indicate stronger advertising literacy activation, because a higher score means that the child accurately categorized more advertising words than news words. The difference score for speed of categorization of the conceptual words (i.e., conceptual advertising literacy activation) was calculated by subtracting the mean reaction time value on the advertising words (congruent words) from the mean reaction time value on the news words (incongruent words). This way, a higher score again indicated stronger advertising literacy activation, because a higher score means that the child was faster in categorizing the advertising words than in categorizing the news words. The same was done for the attitudinal words, where the difference for categorization and speed of categorization were calculated in the same way, but with the negative words as congruent words and positive words as incongruent words.
The second measure of children’s advertising literacy activation was a self-reported questionnaire (part of the general questionnaire, see below). Children’s conceptual advertising literacy activation was measured with six questions. Two were related to advertising recognition (‘Did this video contain advertising?’ and ‘Was this video sponsored by a brand?’). Four were related to the commercial intent (‘Is the aim of this video to ….’ followed by “make people like brand X,” “make people want to have brand X,” “make people think positively about brand X,” and “make people buy brand X”;
Children’s dispositional conceptual advertising literacy was assessed with five questions (‘Is the aim of advertising to ….’ followed by “make you want to buy the advertised product,” “make you want to have the advertised product,” “make you think positively about the advertised product,” “to make you feel positively about the advertised product,” “seduce you to buy the advertised product”;
Brand attitude was measured with six questions [‘Do you think brand X is ….’ followed by “nice,” “nasty” (R), “good,” “stupid” (R), “tasty,” “bad” (R);
As control variables, we assessed sex, type of school (primary/high school), and grade. In addition, we measured brand recall and brand recognition. Brand recall was measured with one question (‘Which brand or brands did you see in the video?,’ 49.6% correct) followed by one question assessing brand recognition (‘Which of the following brands did you see? This can be one brand, several brands, or no brand.’ Participants were presented a list of six brands including three pizza brands, 89.9% correctly identified the brand). We also measured prior exposure to the specific video (10.5% yes), familiarity with the vlogger (61.6% yes), and brand familiarity (93.4% yes). We also measured how often children watched videos from this vlogger (
We included two questions to check if participants perceived the manipulation as intended. First, we asked them if they saw a text at the beginning of the video (answer options: yes; no; I don’t know). A Chi2 analysis showed that the manipulation was successful. In the condition with the disclosure, 78.0% of the children reported seeing a disclosure versus 14.3% of the control condition, χ2(2,
All analyses were done in SPSS (Version 25). We first performed several randomization checks to see which variables needed to be included as covariates in the main analyses. Randomization checks were all χ2 and
The second analysis tested the model presented in
Means, standard deviations, and Pearson correlations for all variables related to advertising literacy and brand attitude.
2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | |||
(1) Child age | 10.45 | 1.94 | 0.36*** | 0.06 | 0.52*** | –0.02 | 0.23*** | –0.05 | −0.13* | 0.06 | 0.20** |
(2) Dispositional conceptual advertising literacy | 5.13 | 0.93 | 0.08 | 0.49*** | 0.05 | 0.18** | 0.15* | –0.06 | 0.13 | 0.17** | |
(3) Dispositional attitudinal advertising literacy | 4.19 | 0.80 | 0.05 | 0.31*** | –0.07 | 0.27*** | –0.09 | 0.13 | −0.18** | ||
(4) Conceptual advertising literacy activation (self-report) | 4.40 | 1.09 | 0.10 | 0.12 | 0.03 | –0.08 | 0.07 | 0.09 | |||
(5) Attitudinal advertising literacy activation (self-report) | 2.54 | 0.91 | 0.06 | 0.11 | –0.03 | 0.02 | −0.46*** | ||||
(6) Categorization of conceptual words (DS) | –0.08 | 0.22 | 0.18** | 0.20*** | 0.02 | 0.13* | |||||
(7) Categorization of attitudinal words (DS) | 0.67 | 0.73 | –0.08 | 0.27*** | 0.00 | ||||||
(8) Speed of categorizing conceptual words (DS) | –54.44 | 189.20 | −0.28*** | 0.02 | |||||||
(9) Speed of categorizing attitudinal words (DS) | 194.15 | 601.21 | –0.03 | ||||||||
(10) Brand attitude | 4.78 | 0.93 |
We ran a number of randomization checks to see which variables needed to be included as covariates in the main analyses. The following variables were equally distributed across both conditions: sex, χ2(1,
The results for
Hypotheses testing for conceptual advertising literacy activation.
Dependent variable | Conceptual advertising literacy activation |
|||||||||||
Accuracy of categorization |
Speed of categorization |
Self-report |
Brand attitude |
|||||||||
Disclosure | −0.03 (0.52) | −0.05 | 0.957 | −130.79 (461.81) | −0.28 | 0.777 | −3.58 (2.20) | −1.63 | 0.104 | −0.07 (0.12) | −0.58 | 0.561 |
Disclosure*dispositional | 0.01 (0.10) | 0.11 | 0.910 | 42.63 (86.88) | 0.49 | 0.624 | 0.72 (0.41) | 1.73 | 0.084 | |||
Disclosure*dispositional*age | −0.03 (0.08) | −0.34 | 0.735 | −25.77 (71.94) | −0.35 | 0.721 | −0.49 (0.34) | −1.43 | 0.154 | |||
Accuracy of categorization | 0.51 (0.27) | 1.85 | 0.065 | |||||||||
Speed of categorization | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.06 | 0.955 | |||||||||
Self-report | 0.07 (0.06) | 1.21 | 0.227 |
With respect to H2a, the relationship between exposure to the disclosure and conceptual advertising literacy activation was not moderated by children’s dispositional conceptual advertising literacy. This was the case when conceptual advertising literacy activation was assessed via the indirect measurement task as well as when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire (
With respect to H3a, there was no three-way interaction between exposure to the disclosure, children’s dispositional conceptual advertising literacy, and age-group on conceptual advertising literacy activation. This was the case when conceptual advertising literacy activation was assessed via the indirect measurement task as well as when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire (
Finally, with respect to H4a, children’s conceptual advertising literacy activation did not have a positive effect on brand attitude. This was the case when conceptual advertising literacy activation was assessed via the indirect measurement task as well as when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire, as can be seen in
The results for
Hypotheses testing for attitudinal advertising literacy activation.
Dependent variable | Attitudinal advertising literacy activation |
|||||||||||
Accuracy of categorization |
Speed of categorization |
Self-report |
Brand attitude |
|||||||||
Disclosure | 0.63 (1.47) | 0.43 | 0.666 | 525.72 (1370.16) | −0.38 | 0.702 | 1.43 (1.97) | 0.72 | 0.470 | 0.01 (0.12) | 0.09 | 0.932 |
Disclosure*dispositional | −0.11 (0.34) | −0.33 | 0.742 | 174.05 (321.99) | 0.54 | 0.589 | −0.34 (0.46) | −0.74 | 0.459 | |||
Disclosure*dispositional*age | 0.05 (0.27) | 0.17 | 0.866 | −117.16 (249.10) | −0.47 | 0.639 | 0.33 (0.36) | 0.94 | 0.348 | |||
Accuracy of categorization | 0.05 (0.09) | 0.55 | 0.580 | |||||||||
Speed of categorization | 0.00 (0.00) | −0.40 | 0.690 | |||||||||
Self-report | −0.49 (0.07) | −7.41 | <0.001 |
With respect to H2b, the relation between exposure to the disclosure and attitudinal advertising literacy activation was not moderated by children’s dispositional attitudinal advertising literacy. This was the case when attitudinal advertising literacy activation was assessed via an indirect measurement task as well as when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire (
With respect to H3b, there was no three-way interaction between exposure to the disclosure, children’s dispositional attitudinal advertising literacy, and age-group on attitudinal advertising literacy activation. This was the case when attitudinal advertising literacy activation was assessed via an indirect measurement task as well as when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire (
Finally, with respect to H4b, children’s attitudinal advertising literacy activation had a negative effect on brand attitude, but only when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire (
The first aim of this study was to investigate if a disclosure can stimulate children’s advertising literacy activation when they are exposed to in-vlog advertising. The second aim was to investigate if advertising literacy activation was related to children’s brand attitude. One of the main contributions of this study is that it assessed children’s advertising literacy activation with both an indirect measure [the Advertising Literacy Activation Task (ALAT)] (
A possible explanation for the fact that the disclosure did not activate children’s advertising literacy could be that the ‘hidden’ advertising in the vlog used here was actually not really hidden at all. For instance, brand recall and brand recognition were relatively high even in the condition without the disclosure (52.4% for brand recall and 88.1% for brand recognition). The high prominence of the brand could have triggered children to activate their advertising literacy. This is supported by the fact that advertising recognition and sponsorship recognition were equally high in both conditions (4.65 and 4.67, respectively, in the control condition, versus 4.88 and 4.76, respectively, in the experimental condition as measured on a scale from 1 to 6). Thus, it seems that children did not need the disclosure in order to recognize advertising and subsequently activate their advertising literacy.
It was expected that the relation between the disclosure and children’s advertising literacy activation was moderated by two variables: their dispositional advertising literacy and their age. There was no support for either moderator. Younger children (aged 7–11 years old) are considered cued processors (
Interestingly, there were no differences in the relationship between disclosure exposure and activation of advertising literacy for the two measurement methods used to assess activation (i.e., indirect measurement task and self-reported questionnaires). The use of the two different measurement methods produces the same results. Thus, we can conclude with more certainty that a disclosure, as used here, does not help children activate their advertising literacy when they are exposed to in-vlog advertising with highly prominent brand placement.
With regard to the second aim, our study showed that there was no positive relation between conceptual advertising literacy activation and brand attitude when activation was assessed with the indirect measurement task nor when it was assessed with a self-reported questionnaire. The reason for this could be that children’s brand attitude was already quite positive and thus a better realization that the vlog was made to make people like and buy the brand (i.e., higher conceptual advertising literacy activation) could not further increase this attitude.
For attitudinal advertising literacy we did find that stronger literacy activation led to a more negative attitude toward the brand, which is in line with previous studies (e.g.,
Another explanation for the difference between the two measurement methods could be that there was a discrepancy between the level of measurement of advertising literacy activation (indirect) and the level of measurement of brand attitude (direct). An indirect way of measuring children’s advertising literacy activation may also need an indirect way of assessing their brand attitude, for instance with an Implicit Association Task (IAT;
Finally, the difference between the two measurements methods could be explained by the wording and order of questioning that was used in the questionnaire. The questionnaire contained questions regarding the fairness of the presence of this specific brand, while the indirect measurement task assessed a more general skeptical attitude and disliking. Furthermore, the self-reported questions regarding activation of attitudinal advertising literacy were directly administered after the questions regarding brand attitude. It could be that children therefore linked these two concepts.
Despite careful preparation, this study is subject to some limitations, leading to suggestions for future research. A first limitation is that the study implemented a single-message design. We decided to use a single-message design to keep the overall feasibility of the study high (i.e., chance of achieving required sample size) and the burden on the young participants acceptable. Moreover, by using a single-message design, we were better able to interpret any possible effects. However, a disadvantage of a single message design is that the effects, or lack of effects, could be driven by other message features (e.g., type of vlogger, gender of vlogger, narrative in the story). Future research could examine the hypothesized relationships for a variety of vlog messages. Doing so would allow to draw conclusions beyond one instance of a sponsored vlog.
Another limitation concerns the stimulus material, specifically the selected vlog and sponsoring brand, and the disclosure message used. In the current study, we used a vlog in which the brand was already quite prominent—this may have made the commercial intent of the vlog too obvious (
Another suggestion for future research related to the stimulus material is to work with a brand that is less familiar and less popular. In the current study, over 90% of the participants were familiar with the brand and the overall attitude toward the brand was very positive. This could be because children already had a favorable attitude toward the brand. As a consequence, it may have been difficult for the vlog and the disclosure to affect this existing attitude. The attitude toward a less familiar or unfamiliar brand may be easier to influence with exposure to a disclosure. Using a new and unfamiliar brand could therefore provide further insight into the relation between children’s dispositional advertising literacy, advertising literacy activation, advertising susceptibility, and the usefulness of a disclosure.
In future research, it is also important to focus on children’s understanding of the meaning of the disclosure. The present study used an explicit disclosure based on current regulations in the United States and Europe (e.g.,
Another suggestion for future research with regard to the stimulus material used is to add a condition in which participants are shown a neutral ‘dummy’ message (e.g., “You are going to see an online video now”) at the start of the video. In doing so, a condition is added in which the structure of the message (i.e., a sentence presented on a black screen before the vlog content + vlog content) is exactly the same. As a result, several alternative explanations for a possible disclosure effect (e.g., the potential effect could be driven by the mere presence of a text before the video, not so much by the content of the text, and reading a text requires mental resources for processing which might consecutively affect children’s responses to the video) can be excluded.
With regard to the dependent measures for advertising literacy, future research could focus on other dimensions, representing more sophisticated levels of understanding (e.g., understanding of persuasive tactics used, understanding of the economic model of advertising) as well. In the current study, we chose to focus on two dimensions of conceptual ad literacy (i.e., advertising recognition and understanding of advertising’s selling and persuasive intent) and two dimensions of attitudinal advertising literacy (i.e., skepticism and critical attitude/disliking). The reason for this was that in the literature these variables are assumed to be important basic dimensions of advertising literacy, together forming the fundament of children’s advertising schemas (
Finally, there are some suggestions for future research with regard to the measurement methods used. In the current study two different measurement methods were used. Although it seems that the questionnaire measurement and indirect measurement yielded similar results, more research is needed on this topic. First, it is important to get a broader understanding whether the indirect and direct measure actually assess the exact same construct because this is not always clear (
Second, this work used a questionnaire measurement that provided support for the role of attitudinal advertising literacy activation in making children less susceptible to advertising effects whereas the indirect measurement did not. This finding could be further explored in future research that assesses brand attitude with both a questionnaire (as was done in this study) as well as with an indirect measurement task (e.g., an IAT or Approach-Avoidance Task). This will provide a better understanding of the relationship between different measurement methods in assessing children’s advertising literacy activation and their brand responses. For future research it is important to consider that the indirect measurement has several advantages over the questionnaire measurement—it is more difficult to give social desirable answers when an indirect measure is used (see
The results of this study have implications for the scientific community as well as government guidelines and advertisers. First, this study showed that a textual disclosure reading “[name vlogger] is being paid by [name company] to advertise in this video” does not necessarily increase the extent to which children activate their advertising literacy when exposed to in-vlog advertising. In this study, this may be due to the fact that the brand is very prominent in the video. Prominently placed brands seem to be an inherent trigger for children to activate their advertising literacy regardless of the presence of a disclosure. However, whether the prominence of the brand indeed made it easier for the children to activate their advertising literacy remains to be tested in future research. An important scientific implication of this study therefore is that the effectiveness of a disclosure for in-vlog advertising should always be considered in relation to the specific features of the brand placement and the vlog characteristics (e.g., prominence of the brand, familiarity of the brand and the vlogger, prior attitude to the brand and the vlogger). These features can make it either easier or harder for children to use their retrieval strategies in order to activate their advertising literacy schemas. From a theoretical perspective it is therefore important to take the characteristics of the message into account when studying children’s advertising information-processing and schema activation abilities.
Another theoretical implication is that this study showed that both direct and indirect measures are needed to get a comprehensive view of the relation between children’s advertising literacy and advertising susceptibility. The present study showed that direct and indirect measurements of advertising literacy activation reveal different processes through which children make sense of, and are affected by, advertising. That is, direct measures reveal more conscious and deliberate ways of advertising processing, while indirect measures reveal more subconscious and automatic ways of advertising processing. In daily life, children only use few mental resources to process advertising messages, meaning that children’s advertising processing is rather automatic (
Furthermore, our findings have important practical implications because such disclosures are typically required in current advertising guidelines in Europe and the United States. The current study showed that the disclosure, as used here, has failed to further raise the level at which children activate their advertising literacy while watching a vlog. This is not to say that all sponsorship disclosures are ineffective. It is important to first get an understanding under what circumstances sponsorship disclosures
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee Social Science (ECSS) Radboud University Nijmegen. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.
RH, ER, HS, EvR, and MB designed the experiment and wrote the manuscript together. RH collected and analyzed the data.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors would like to thank the NEMO museum for providing the opportunity to collect data in the museum. The authors would also like to thank Alma van den Berg, Nadine Bol, Marieke Boschma, Willem Hoek, Nina Hemrika, Josje van der Meer, Claire Segijn, Pauline Sluijpers, and Sanne Tamboer for their help with the data collection.