Front. Psychiatry Frontiers in Psychiatry Front. Psychiatry 1664-0640 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1120109 Psychiatry Original Research The role of early social play behaviors and language skills for shy children’s later internalizing difficulties in school Baardstu Silje 1 * Sette Stefania 2 Brandlistuen Ragnhild Eek 3 Wang Mari Vaage 3 1Department of Childhood and Families, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway 2Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy 3Department of Child Health and Development, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway

Edited by: Louis A. Schmidt, McMaster University, Canada

Reviewed by: Keren Hanetz-Gamliel, The Academic College of Tel Aviv-Yaffo, Israel; Daphna G. Dollberg, The Academic College of Tel Aviv-Yaffo, Israel

*Correspondence: Silje Baardstu, siljekvam.bardstu@fhi.no

This article was submitted to Public Mental Health, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychiatry

01 03 2023 2023 14 1120109 09 12 2022 13 02 2023 Copyright © 2023 Baardstu, Sette, Brandlistuen and Wang. 2023 Baardstu, Sette, Brandlistuen and Wang

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Research has demonstrated links from early childhood shyness to socioemotional problems later in life. This longitudinal study explored the role of early social play behaviors and language skills in the associations between childhood shyness and later internalizing and language difficulties in school. Participants were N = 7,447 children (50.1% girls) from the Norwegian Mother, Father, and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). Latent direct, indirect, and interaction path analyses were performed within a structural equation framework. Results showed that mother-rated childhood shyness from age 18 months to age five years was associated with mother-rated internalizing difficulties and language problems at age eight years. Lower levels of teacher-reported social play behaviors and poorer language skills in preschool increased the risk of later anxiety problems among shy children, whereas higher levels of language competencies and social play behaviors buffered against later anxiety problems. The study identifies some of the early risk and protective factors that may influence shy children’s socio-emotional functioning and adjustment.

MoBa shyness adjustment longitudinal risk and protective factors anxiety depression language

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      1. Introduction

      Shyness is a temperament trait that is characterized by wariness, self-consciousness, and reticence in the face of social novelty and/or situations of perceived social evaluation (1). An extensive body of studies have demonstrated how early shyness confer increased risk for both concurrent and prospective difficulties in several developmental domains, and particularly in the areas of socio-communicative skills and socioemotional adjustment (2, 3). However, there is growing recognition for the notion that there is heterogeneity in these observed outcomes [i.e., (47)]. In other words, not all shy children are destined to experience or develop such difficulties. This notion elucidates the importance of identifying early risk and protective factors that may influence shy children’s developmental course, as this would allow for greater precision in understanding individual differences in the adjustment and functioning of shy children.

      Several factors that may influence shy children’s adjustment have been suggested in the past decades, including the role of biological factors, such as cortisol levels, individual factors, such as regulatory functions [i.e., attention bias and inhibitory control; (812)], and environmental factors, such as parental socialization practices and the quality of relationships with peers and others [for an overview, see (13)]. Some studies have also demonstrated the protective (i.e., moderating) role of language skills and social competencies for shy children’s social and emotional functioning (2, 1417).

      However, most of this research has mainly been cross-sectional and using small samples of relatively young children (i.e., toddlers, preschoolers). For this reason, there is a need for more longitudinal studies that may expand our knowledge about the long-term nature of associations between shyness, social behaviors, language skills, and internalizing difficulties from early childhood and into school age, including indirect pathways as well as moderating processes. Thus, using longitudinal data from more than 7,000 children followed from 18 months to age 11 years, the purpose of the present study was twofold: the first aim was to examine the long-term prospective links between early childhood shyness and later internalizing and language problems in school, and the second aim was to explore the mediating and/or moderating role of children’s early social play behaviors and language competencies, as reported by early childhood and education care (ECEC) teachers, in these links.

      Temperamental shyness overlaps conceptually with other related constructs, such as anxious solitude, behavioral inhibition, and social reticence. For instance, all these constructs share an underlying core related to social fear, anxiety, and wariness, and they also display relatively similar patterns of associations with adjustment difficulties [see (13), for an overview]. In this study, we conceptualize shyness as a temperament trait that is characterized by wariness, anxiety, and discomfort in response to social novelty and/or self-consciousness in situations of perceived social evaluation (1, 18). This discomfort mainly derives from the interpersonal nature of situations and often elicits inhibited and awkward behaviors, including a desire to withdraw from social interactions (19).

      Over the past decades, much research has uncovered the behavioral and psychosocial correlates of temperamental shyness in childhood [for a review, see Rubin et al. (20)]. On a behavioral level, shyness may manifest as avoidant, withdrawn, awkward, and inhibited behaviors (e.g., freezing behaviors, watching other children playing but not joining in) during interactions with peers or in unfamiliar social situations (21, 22). Psychosocially, shy children tend to experience lower self-esteem, are perceived to have lower social competence, and have higher anxiety levels compared to non-shy children (1, 23).

      Many shy children may also experience an approach-avoidance conflict in which their desire for social interaction (i.e., social approach) is simultaneously inhibited by social fear and anxiety (i.e., social avoidance) (19, 24). This motivational conflict could perhaps explain why shy children often talk less and are less likely to initiate and participate in social interactions with peers compared to more outgoing children (2). Consequently, there is a concern that shy children may have fewer opportunities to practice and develop social and communicative skills and competencies, and that they may also develop feelings of low self-worth and poor self-esteem if they often experience failure in their social engagement with peers (25, 26). In this sense, there is a general conception that shyness is associated with increased risk for poor socio-emotional functioning, including peer problems and loneliness, which, in turn, may increase their risk for emotional difficulties, such as social anxiety and depression [e.g., (27, 28)].

      A growing literature has underlined that shyness is concurrently related to emotional difficulties, such as social anxiety and depression, throughout development [i.e., (29, 30)]. Longitudinal studies have also confirmed such links. For instance, Poole et al. (31), across three laboratory visits in early-to-middle childhood, found two trajectories of shyness, including a high-stable class and a low-stable class. Results revealed that teachers and parents perceived children in high-stable classes as more socially anxious than children in the low-stable class. Gender differences were also found in the high-stable class, with boys displaying more depressive symptoms than girls. Furthermore, Karevold et al. (3), following a sample of 921 Norwegian children from 18 months to 12.5 years, reported that mother-reported shyness at age four was a predictor of parent and self-reported anxiety and parent-reported depression (the latter with a lower effect size) at 12.5 years. Similarly, Bohlin and Hagekull (32), in a sample of 100 participants, found that parent-reported shyness in infancy was a significant predictor of social anxiety and depression at 21 years as reported by participants themselves; however, the association between early shyness and later depression was no longer significant when the analysis controlled for social anxiety at age 21.

      Overall, these findings from both concurrent and longitudinal studies are in line with a recent metanalysis which concluded that behavioral inhibition (a construct conceptually similar to shyness) in early childhood represents one of the main risk factors of subsequent anxiety, and especially social anxiety disorders (28). Yet, although the concurrent and longitudinal associations between shyness and internalizing problems have been empirically demonstrated, the roles of possible risk and protective factors that may exacerbate or mitigate such links over time are less clear (28).

      In addition to emotional problems, research has also consistently shown shyness to be associated with language and socio-communicative difficulties, including less complex language and poorer expressive, pragmatic, and receptive language skills [for an overview, see Coplan et al. (2)]. Shy children are also found to speak less than less shy children in familiar and unfamiliar settings (33) and to show a skill delay in the social use of language (34).

      Several processes behind the shyness and language problems link have been suggested, as summed up by Coplan and Evans (35). For instance, as language is learned through active participation, shy children may be less linguistically competent because their withdrawn demeanor and restricted verbal participation in social settings hampers their opportunities to acquire, practice, and develop language skills (i.e., “competence deficit”). Others suggest that shy children’s poorer verbal abilities may rather reflect a “performance deficit” in which shy children’s underlying social anxiety and wariness may act to inhibit these children’s propensity to speak and respond in social settings (36). Support for this latter view comes from research showing that more shy children often score lower on tests of expressive language than on tests of receptive language (37).

      Despite the vast body of studies linking shyness with adjustment difficulties and poor social and communicative functioning, it is clearly not the case that all shy children experience such difficulties. In this respect, a growing body of research has suggested several risk and protective factors that may influence shy children’s developmental course, including biological factors (i.e., cortisol levels) and environmental factors (i.e., parental socialization practices, the quality of relationships with others) [for an overview, see Coplan et al. (13)].

      A small body of studies has also explored the role of individual factors for shy children’s socio-emotional functioning. For instance, studies have reported that the association between shyness and internalizing problems is lower among shy children with higher levels of temperamental activity or greater sports participation (38, 39). Furthermore, shy preschool-children with higher emotion-related competencies (i.e., ability to recognize the emotions of others) are found to show better social and emotional adjustment (i.e., less anxious-withdrawal and peer rejection) compared to shy children with lower levels of emotion recognition (30). Moreover, greater inhibitory control, a component of temperamental effortful control, is found to increase shy children’s risk of negative adjustment, including heightened anxiety problems in kindergarten (8), higher levels of social anxiety, as well as less prosocial behaviors and more problematic peer interactions in both preschool and school settings (9, 40, 41).

      Furthermore, as suggested by Coplan and Weeks (2), language skills could be a positive resource for shy children’s socioemotional functioning. That is, shy children who are more able to communicate their thoughts and who are confident in their ability to use appropriate language may be less prone to feel anxious and wary around peers. Better language skills could also foster positive social interactions, and may, in this sense, be an essential tool that could aid shy children to make and keep friends. There is some support for this proposition from a handful of studies demonstrating that shy children who also are verbally skilled (i.e., expressive, receptive, and pragmatic language skills) tend to show less inhibition, loneliness, social anxiety, peer difficulties, and asocial behaviors compared to shy children who are less verbally skilled (2, 15, 42). Despite these research efforts, there is still a lack of longitudinal studies examining such pathways over time.

      Moreover, relatively less is known about the role of shy children’s social interaction skills and behaviors for their future socio-emotional and communicative functioning. As suggested by Asendorpf (24) as well as the “shy but getting by” model by Coplan et al. (13), such skills and behaviors are likely to promote resiliency and foster positive development among shy children by aiding in the formation of positive interactions and relationships with peers and others. In this sense, positive social skills, such as play behaviors with peers, may increase shy children’s social engagement and their opportunities to learn and practice social and communicative skills, which ultimately could ameliorate their increased risk of language and internalizing problems.

      There is some support for such assertions from studies showing that shy (i.e., anxious solitary) children with higher levels of social competencies (i.e., more agreeable) had higher quality peer relations and were better liked by peers compared to shy children with lower levels of social competencies (14, 16). Further, studies also show that shy children with higher levels of positive affect (i.e., positive facial expression, smiling) have fewer symptoms of social anxiety and display more sociability and advanced theory of mind compared to children with more negative affect (4, 7). Thus, considering these results, it is plausible to expect social competencies in the form of social play behaviors with peers to also influence the prospective link between early shyness and later adjustment and language functioning. To date, however, such interactions are yet to be explored empirically.

      1.1. The present study

      On this background, the present study aspires to (1) examine the long-term association between shyness in childhood (from age 18 months to five years) and later adjustment outcomes (language and internalizing difficulties) in school at ages eight and 11 years, and (2) explore the possible influence of early social play behaviors and language competencies as measured in preschool in these prospective links.

      First, based on previous research (3, 35), we hypothesize that higher levels of early shyness will be positively associated with both language problems as well as with internalizing problems during the school years (at age eight and 11 years). However, in accordance with a previous meta-analysis (28), we expect that associations with anxiety symptoms will be stronger than associations with depressive symptoms.

      Second, building on previous theorizing and empirical evidence (13, 15, 16, 24), we explore if differences in early social play behaviors and language competencies during the preschool years could influence the strength of these longitudinal associations. More specifically, we hypothesize that the associations between early shyness and later language and internalizing problems will decrease at higher levels of social play behaviors and language skills but increase at higher levels of language problems and lower levels of social play behavior with peers.

      Finally, as previous research has indicated that social play behaviors and language competencies may act as mediators in the associations between shyness and adjustment outcomes (33, 43, 44), we also explored whether social play behaviors and language competencies would account for (i.e., mediate) the prospective associations between early shyness and later adjustment outcomes in the current study. In this sense, we hypothesize that higher shyness in childhood predicts less social play behaviors in preschool, which in turn, predict poorer language abilities and adjustment in school. Similarly, based on perspectives arguing that shy children’s poor language abilities may reflect a “performance deficit” (because shy children’s wariness and social reticence may inhibit their propensities to speak in social settings), we hypothesize that higher levels of childhood shyness predict poorer language competencies in preschool, which, in turn, will be prospectively associated with poorer adjustment and language competencies in school.

      Previous research has also demonstrated gender differences in shyness, as well as in its associations with developmental outcomes (2, 45, 46), although such differences have not been consistently reported (47, 48). Studies also have shown parental socioeconomic factors to be associated with a variety of child outcomes (49, 50). For this reason, we included both gender and mother’s education level in all our analyses.

      2. Materials and methods 2.1. Participants and procedure

      The participants of this study represent a sub-cohort from the Norwegian Mother, Father, and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). The MoBa study is a prospective population-based pregnancy cohort study conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health (51, 52). Participants of the MoBa were recruited from all over Norway from 1999 to 2008. The women consented to participation in 40.6% of the pregnancies. The MoBa now includes 114,500 children, 95,200 mothers, and 75,200 fathers. Pregnancy and birth records from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (MBRN) are linked to the MoBa database (53).

      The sub-cohort of the current study includes a total of 7,447 children (50.1% girls). This sample consists of children born between 2006 and 2009 with ECEC teacher rated questionnaire data at five years of age. For this sub-cohort, we also included mother-rated data from child age 18 months, three, five, and eight years, and primary school teacher-rated data at child age 11 years (response rate = 51%).

      The ECEC teachers (response rate = 40%) were recruited from all over Norway over a three-years period through invitation from the participating mothers, meaning that the sample was spread across different geographical locations. We have ECEC center ID on most of these children (n = 5,773) from across 2,738 ECEC centers. Among these children, the majority (n = 3,035) were the sole target-child in the center, whereas the remaining children were in the same ECEC center as one or more children in the sample. Although we have no information about whether these children were in the same department or not, there is a possibility that some children might have been rated by the same ECEC teacher and, thus, there is a possibility of interdependence between observations. However, we have previously explored the potential of clustering effects at the ECEC level (i.e., multiple children in the same ECEC center) in the subsample and found that results remained identical, which suggests that the effect of clustering is limited (50). Due to missing ID information for the schools included, we were not able to test for clustering effects at the primary school level. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility of interdependence between observations at the school level.

      The establishment of MoBa and initial data collection was based on a license from the Norwegian Data Protection Agency and approval from The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics. The MoBa cohort is regulated by the Norwegian Health Registry Act. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The present research project is approved by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REK) (2015/1324). We use the twelth version of the quality-assured dataset released for research in 2019 (54).

      2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Shyness

      Mothers assessed child shyness at child age 18 months, three, and five years via the shyness subscale of the Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability Temperament Survey–Short Form [EAS; (55)]. Previous studies have demonstrated satisfactory psychometric properties for the shyness subscale (56). This subscale originally includes five items rated on a 5-point scale (from 1 = not typical to 5 = very typical), but only three questions were included for use in the MoBa questionnaire (i.e., “Is very social,” “Is very friendly with strangers,” both reversed, and “Takes a long time to warm up to strangers”). The Cronbach’s alpha for the shyness subscales was 0.65 at age 18 months, 0.67 at age three years, and 0.71 at age five years.

      2.2.2. Social play behaviors

      At child age five years, ECEC teachers rated the target child’s play behaviors with peers using the social play subscale of the Preschool Play Behavior Scale [PPBS; (57)]. The subscale comprises five items that assess social play in terms of the extent to which the child engages in peer conversation (“Engages in active conversations with other children during play”) and in group interaction [e.g., “Plays in groups with (and not just beside) other children”], with response categories ranging from 1 = never to 5 = very often. The subscale is previously shown to display acceptable psychometric properties, including high internal reliability (α = 0.96) and good construct validity (57). The PPBS instrument has been translated and back-translated and used in several different cultures, including Italy (44), Finland (58), Korea (59), Norway (60), Malaysia (61), and Turkey (62). The internal reliability (α) for the social play subscale in the current study was 0.66.

      2.2.3. Language competencies

      At child age five years, ECEC teachers rated the target child’s language competencies using a combination of items from two language subscales of the Child Development Inventory [CDI; (63)]. Five items were taken from the originally 50-items verbal comprehension subscale [i.e., “Tells where he(she) lives, naming town or city,” “Uses the words “today,” “yesterday,” and “tomorrow” correctly] while four items were taken from the originally 50-items expressive language subscale (i.e., “Asks the meaning of words,” “Uses irregular plurals correctly, for example, says “men,” not “mans”), with dichotomous response categories (1 = no, 2 = yes). Previous studies have demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties of the CDI, including internal reliability and construct and predictive validity (64). The polychoric reliability of the teacher-rated language competence at age five years was 0.69.

      At child age eight years, mothers rated the child’s language competencies using the checklist of 20 statements about language difficulties (65). The checklist is a validated Norwegian instrument used to identify children with receptive, semantic, and expressive language difficulties. Eight of the original 20 items were selected for use in the MoBa (e.g., “Is often struggling finding the right words,” “Has difficulties understanding the meaning of common words”), with statements rated on a 5-point scale (from 1 = “Does not fit the child/absolutely wrong” to 5 = “Fits well with the child, absolutely right”). The polychoric reliability of the mother-rated and teacher-rated language competencies at age eight were 0.85 and 0.78, respectively.

      At child age 11, school teachers rated the target child’s language competencies using the Children’s Communication Checklist [CCC-2; (66)]. The CCC-2 is a screening instrument used to identify language impairment in children that originally includes ten subscales with seven items each. Twelve items were selected for use in the teacher questionnaire, comprising word-finding difficulties, appropriate use of language, coherence, and syntax (e.g., “Forgets words s/he knows,” “Mixes up words of similar meanings,” “Can produce long and complicated sentences such as: When we went to the park I had a go on the swings;” “I saw this man standing on the corner,” “Uses terms like “he” or “it” without making it clear what s/he is talking about”) rated on a 5-point scale (from 1 = “Does not fit the child/never” to 5 = “Fits well with the child/always”). Previous studies have shown the CCC-2 to have good psychometric properties (67). The polychoric reliability of language competencies at age 11 years in this study was 0.84.

      2.2.4. Internalizing problems

      Children’s anxiety symptoms were measured at age eight (mother reports) and at age 11 years (teacher reports) using the short version of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders [SCARED; (68)]. The SCARED is a multidimensional instrument intended to measure anxiety symptoms corresponding to DSM-defined anxiety disorders. The instrument originally contains 41 items covering five subscales, but the five-item short form was selected for use in the MoBa (i.e., “The child gets really frightened for no reason at all,” “People tell the child that he/she worries too much”), with response categories ranging from 1 = Not true to 3 = True. The short form is previously found to have good psychometric properties similar to the full version (69). The polychoric reliability of anxiety symptoms was 0.66 at age eight years and 0.67 at age 11 years.

      Children’s depressive symptoms were measured at age eight (mother reports) and at age 11 (teacher report) using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire [SMFQ; (70)]. The SMFQ consists of 13-items based on DSM-III-R criteria for depression, composed by descriptive phrases regarding how the individual has been feeling or acting recently (e.g., “Didn’t enjoy anything at all,” “Felt s/he was no good anymore”), with response categories ranging from 1 = Not true to 3 = True. The SMFQ is found to have good psychometric properties (71). The polychoric reliability of depressive symptoms was 0.79 at age eight years and 0.76 at age 11 years.

      2.2.5. Covariates

      Gender and maternal education were included as covariates in the analyses. Gender was indexed using birth records of boys and girls (50.1%) from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway. Maternal education was measured using mother’s self-reported level of education derived from the MoBa 15th weeks of pregnancy questionnaire with response categories ranging from nine-years secondary school to University/College over four years. Due to the small number of participants in the lowest categories, the education variable was reduced to three categories scored as: (1) up to high school education (20.4%), (2) higher education college/university up to four years (44.5%), and (3) higher education college/university more than four years (35.1%).

      2.3. Statistical analysis

      All analyses were performed within a structural equation modeling framework using MPlus version 8.2 (72). Full information maximum likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) was used to handle missing data and to correct test statistics and standard errors for non-normality of the observations (73). Model fit was evaluated by values of the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) above 0.95 and by Root Mean Square of Approximation (RMSEA) below 0.05 (74).

      Analyses were carried out in several steps. First, measurement models for all study variables were estimated by using confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) whereby we constructed one latent factor for each of the variables based on their respective indicators. Second, we tested measurement invariance of shyness across the three time points by comparing a baseline model (i.e., configural invariance) against a series of increasingly restricted models (i.e., weak invariance and strong invariance), following Widaman et al. (75). In the configural invariance model, we estimated the first loading and the first intercept, while the corresponding first factor loadings and factor intercepts were constrained to be invariant over time. In the weak invariance model, we added across-time invariance constraints on the remaining factor loadings, and in the strong invariance model, across-time invariance constraints were also placed on the remaining factor intercepts. The model fit of the most restricted invariance model (i.e., strong invariance) was adequate [χ2(21) = 1145.01, RMSEA = 0.025 (95% CI: 0.024, 0.027), CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.98, SRMR = 0.026] and not significantly worse than the less restricted models [Δχ2(2) = 16.71, p > 0.05].

      This baseline measurement model was then used to construct a second order latent childhood shyness factor based on the three latent shyness factors. We established an approximate standard metric by constraining the first factor loading of the shyness factor to its specific value and by setting the mean of the latent shyness factor at the first time point to 0 and the variance of the factor to 1.

      Third, we examined the prospective associations from the early childhood shyness latent factor as well as from the latent social play behavior and language competencies factors at age five years to later language and internalizing problems at age eight and age 11 years by using multiple regression analyses, controlling for gender and maternal education. For the outcomes at age eight years, all the preschool predictor variables were simultaneously included. For the outcomes at age 11 years, we estimated both univariate (including only the preschool predictor variables) and multivariate models (controlling for previous levels of the variable, for instance; anxiety at age eight for anxiety at age 11 years).

      Fourth, we tested whether early social play behaviors and language competencies moderated associations of childhood shyness with language problems and internalizing problems by estimating a series of latent moderation structural equation models [LMS; (76)], one for each outcome variable at child age eight and 11 years. The LMS approach produces estimates of interaction that are not attenuated by measurement errors, which in turn serves to increase power as well as reducing the likelihood of biased estimates (77). In each model, the predictor (latent childhood shyness), the moderator in question (social play behavior, language competence), and the product term of these variables were simultaneously included as predictors of the outcome variables, while also controlling for gender and maternal education. Simple slopes follow-up analyses were then conducted to further probe the association between childhood shyness and the outcome variables at different levels of the moderator variables, specified to low (−1 SD below the mean), average, and high levels (+1 SD above the mean) of the moderator variables. Prior to all analyses, we standardized the latent variables by setting their variances to 1 and freeing their first indicator loadings. We also tested for gender differences in all analyses.

      Finally, we estimated several indirect models for each of the mediator variables, social play behaviors and language competencies, where these variables were defined as putative mediators in the association between early shyness and each of the outcome variables at age eight and 11 years, respectively, controlling for gender and maternal education. For these models, we applied path analyses and estimated the indirect paths and their confidence intervals by using bootstrapping (78), and tested for gender differences by comparing constrained paths with unconstrained pats. In the indirect models for the outcome variables at age 11 years, we also controlled for previous levels of the variable in question.

      3. Results

      Descriptive statistics (i.e., means, standard deviations, and range) and polychoric (i.e., latent) intercorrelations among the study variables are presented in Table 1. The patterns of associations were mostly as expected. There were high intercorrelations between the three shyness measures across time points. Further, shyness across all three time points was positively correlated with both language problems and anxiety at age eight. Language competency at age five years was only negatively associated with shyness at age three years, and social play behaviors at age five years was only negatively associated with shyness at age five years. Shyness at both ages three and five years was positively correlated with depressive symptoms at age eight years and with anxiety at age 11 years.

      Polychoric intercorrelations between study variables and descriptive statistics of variables.

      1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 M (SD) Range
      1. Shyness 1.5 years (m) 2.05 (0.64) 1–5
      2. Shyness 3 years (m) 0.69** 2.21 (0.68) 1–5
      3. Shyness 5 years (m) 0.50** 0.77** 2.09 (0.71) 1–5
      4. Social PB 5 years (kt) -0.01 -0.03 -0.05* 4.41 (0.56) 1–5
      5. Lang. Comp. 5 years (kt) -0.02 -0.05* -0.02 0.39** 1.88 (16) 1–2
      6. Lang. Prob. 8 years (m) 0.07* 0.11** 0.10** -0.26** -0.51** 1.32 (0.48) 1–5
      7. Anxiety 8 years (m) 0.12** 0.21** 0.29** -0.08* -0.06* 0.24** 1.20 (0.24) 1–3
      8. Depressive 8 years (m) 0.01 0.04* 0.07** -0.08** -0.09** 0.25** 0.45** 1.14 (0.19) 1–3
      9. Lang. Prob. 11 years (t) 0.00 0.02 0.01 -0.13** -0.26** 0.25** 0.04 0.09** 1.37 (0.43) 1–4
      10. Anxiety 11 years (t) 0.04 0.09** 0.13** -0.10* -0.12** 0.19** 0.45** 0.22** 0.15** 1.13 (0.22) 1–3
      11. Depressive 11 years (t) -0.04 -0.01 0.01 -0.07* -0.10* 0.09** 0.20** 0.20** 0.12** 0.47** 1.13 (0.28) 1–3
      12. Gender 0.11** 0.07* 0.02 0.18** 0.09** -0.07** 0.05* -0.01 -0.07** 0.04 -0.06*
      13. Mother education -0.03* -0.02 0.00 0.04 0.08** -0.07** -0.03 -0.06** -0.05* -0.05* -0.02

      All variables are latent factors except gender (girls = 1) and mother education, Social PB = social play behavior; Lang. Comp. = language competencies; Lang. Prob. = language problems; (m) = mother reports; (kt) = ECEC teacher reports; (t) = school-teacher reports; M = mean scores; SD = standard deviation, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

      Social play behavior was positively associated with language competencies and negatively associated with anxiety and depressive symptoms at both time points. Language competence at all time points was negatively correlated with anxiety and depressive symptoms at both age eight and 11 years. Gender (i.e., being a girl) was positively correlated with shyness at age 18 months and age three years, social play behaviors at age five, language competence at all time points, and with anxiety at age eight. Higher mother education was positively correlated with language competence at all time points and negatively correlated with depressive symptoms at age eight years.

      3.1. Direct path analyses

      Concerning the outcome variables at age eight years (see Table 2), results from the multiple regression analyses revealed that childhood shyness was positively and significantly predictive of language problems and of anxiety and depressive symptoms. Further, ECEC teacher-reported social play behavior at age five years was negatively predictive of mother-reported language and internalizing difficulties, whereas ECEC teacher-reported language competency in preschool was only negatively predictive of later language problems as reported by mothers.

      Results from univariate and multivariate regression analysis with childhood shyness and social play behaviors and language competencies at age 5 years as predictors of internalizing and language problems at age 8 and 11 years.

      Predictors Childhood shyness age 1.5–5 Social play behaviors age 5 Language competencies age 5 Language problems age 8 Anxiety symptoms age 8 Depressive symptoms age 8
      β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE)
      Outcomes age 8 years
      Language problems 0.09** (0.02) −0.06* (0.02) −0.48** (0.02)
      Anxiety symptoms 0.24** (0.03) −0.07* (0.03) −0.04 (0.03)
      Depressive symptoms 0.05* (0.02) −0.05* (0.02) −0.05 (0.03)
      Outcomes age 11 years Univariate analyses
      Language problems 0.00 (0.02) −0.02 (0.03) −0.24** (0.04)
      Anxiety symptoms 0.09** (0.03) −0.09* (0.04) −0.09* (0.04)
      Depressive symptoms −0.02 (0.03) −0.03 (0.04) −0.09* (0.04)
      Multivariate analyses
      Language problems −0.01 (0.02) −0.01 (0.03) −0.15** (0.04) 0.17** (0.03) −0.02 (0.03) 0.03 (0.03)
      Anxiety symptoms −0.00 (0.03) −0.05 (0.04) −0.04 (0.05) 0.06 (0.04) 0.42** (0.04) −0.00 (0.03)
      Depressive symptoms −0.05 (0.03) −0.02 (0.04) −0.08 (0.05) −0.02 (0.04) 0.14** (0.04) 0.13** (0.03)

      *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001. All analyses controlled for gender and maternal education.

      With regards to the outcome variables at age 11 years, results from the univariate analyses showed mother-rated childhood shyness and ECEC teacher-reported social play behaviors and language competencies in preschool to be significantly predictive of teacher-reported anxiety symptoms, and early language competencies in preschool to be negatively predictive of teacher-reported depressive symptoms. However, these associations were no longer significant when we included previous levels of the outcome variables at age eight in the analyses (see Table 2).

      3.2. Moderation analyses

      Results from the latent moderation analyses showed a few significant interaction effects. First, the childhood shyness x social play behavior interaction effect was significant for mother-rated anxiety symptoms at age eight. The negative coefficient of the interaction term (b = −0.09, p < 0.001) indicates that the otherwise positive association between childhood shyness and later anxiety in school-age decreased at higher levels of social play behaviors in preschool. Second, there was also a significant interaction effect of childhood shyness x language competencies for anxiety symptoms at age eight, where the negative coefficient of the interaction term (b = −0.14, p < 0.001) indicates that the positive association between shyness and anxiety decreased at higher levels of language competencies. Results from simple slopes further confirmed this, by showing that the association with anxiety symptoms steadily decreased at higher levels of the two moderator variables (see Table 3). Figures 1, 2 illustrate the graphical plot of these interaction effects, demonstrating that when early social play behavior increased by one unit, the association between shyness and anxiety became less strong, decreasing by 0.09 standard deviations (Figure 1). Similarly, when language competencies increased by one unit, the association between shyness and anxiety decreased by 0.14 standard deviations (Figure 2). No interaction effects of early social play behaviors or language competencies in preschool were found for the associations between childhood shyness and the other outcome variables (i.e., depressive symptoms and language problems).

      Results from interaction analyses including tests of simple slopes of interaction of the moderators social play behaviors and language competencies with 95% confidence intervals.

      Anxiety symptoms age 8 years
      b [95% CI] β (SE)
      Model 1: Social play behaviors
      Childhood shyness (x) 0.12** [0.098, 0.141] 0.24** (0.04)
      Social play behaviors (w) −0.04** [−0.054, −0.019] −0.07** (0.03)
      Shyness*Social play behaviors (xw) −0.09** [−0.105, −0.068] −0.12** (0.04)
      Simple slopes
      −1 SD below the mean 0.21** [0.171, 0.241]
      Mean 0.12** [0.098, 0.141]
      +1 SD above the mean 0.03** [0.014, 0.052]
      Model 2: Language competence
      Childhood shyness (x) 0.11** [0.082, 0.135] 0.24** (0.05)
      Language competencies (w) −0.06** [−0.091, −0.037] −0.09** (0.05)
      Shyness*Language competencies (xw) −0.14** [−0.173, −0.113] −0.16** (0.05)
      Simple slopes
      −1 SD below the mean 0.25** [0.205, 0.299]
      Mean 0.11** [0.082, 0.135]
      +1 SD above the mean −0.03* [−0.066, −0.002]

      x = predictor variable; w = moderator variable; xw = interaction term; 95% CI = 95% confidence interval; SE = standard error; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. All analyses controlled for gender and maternal education.

      Social play behaviors at age 5 years as a protective factor in the association between childhood shyness and symptoms of anxiety at age 8 years, as reported in standard deviations.

      Language competencies at age 5 years as a protective factor in the association between childhood shyness and anxiety symptoms at age eight, as reported in standard deviations.

      3.3. Indirect path analyses

      Results showed no evidence of indirect effects of social play behaviors or language competencies in preschool in the links from childhood shyness to the three outcome variables at age eight and 11 years. However, results did indicate stability in the indirect pathways from childhood shyness to each of the outcome variables throughout childhood. More specifically, there was a significant indirect path from childhood shyness to teacher-rated anxiety symptoms at age 11 years through mother-reported anxiety symptoms at age eight [β = 0.103, 95% CI (0.072, 0.145), p < 0.001]; from childhood shyness to teacher-rated depressive symptoms at age 11 years through mother-rated depressive symptoms at age eight [β = 0.009, 95% CI (0.003, 0.018), p < 0.001]; and from childhood shyness to teacher-rated language problems at age 11 years through mother-rated language problems at age eight [β = 0.016, 95% CI (0.009, 0.021), p < 0.001].

      4. Discussion

      The overall scope of the current study was to explore the long-term association between shyness in childhood and later adjustment outcomes (language and internalizing difficulties) in school, and secondly, to explore the role of early social play behaviors and language competencies in preschool in these prospective links.

      Among the results, a higher level of shyness in childhood was prospectively predictive of later language problems and symptoms of anxiety and depression in early school age. Second, results showed higher levels of social play behaviors and language competencies in preschool to serve a protective role for shy children’s risk of later internalizing difficulties by significantly moderating (i.e., reducing) the prospective associations from childhood shyness to anxiety symptoms at age eight. In contrast, there were no indications that early social play behaviors and language competencies accounted for (i.e., mediated) the prospective links from early shyness to later adjustment difficulties. A closer elaboration of the results and their implications follow below.

      4.1. The emotional functioning of shy children

      In this study, results revealed that childhood shyness, as measured across 18 months, and three and five years, predicted symptoms of anxiety and depression later in childhood, both directly at age eight (mother-ratings), and indirectly at age 11 (teacher-ratings) through previous symptom levels at age eight. In this sense, the results are suggestive of some degree of stability in the longitudinal pathways from early shyness to later internalizing difficulties throughout the childhood years. However, importantly, the significant association between shyness and later depressive symptoms was weaker than the longitudinal association between shyness and later anxiety symptoms, indicating that shyness represents a greater risk factor for anxiety more than for depression in childhood. This finding aligns with previous studies reporting stronger associations of shyness with anxiety, particularly social anxiety, than other internalizing difficulties (3, 28, 32, 79). In other words, the results of our study bring some support to the notion that temperamental shyness in childhood, characterized by excessive wariness in novel social contexts and with new people, may represent one of the strongest risk factors of anxiety in the subsequent development phases (80). Furthermore, the age period considered in the current study (i.e., school-age) might be an early stage for developing depressive problems, which may emerge in the following developmental periods (3). Thus, identifying shy children at risk in the early phases of development could be helpful to reduce the risk for later internalizing problems, especially concerning anxiety symptoms.

      4.2. The socio-communicative functioning of shy children

      With respect to language problems, our results corroborated with previous research findings demonstrating shy children’s increased risk of language difficulties, including poorer pragmatic, expressive and receptive language abilities (2).

      However, there has been less agreement about the extent to which such difficulties may reflect a “performance deficit,” that is, whereby shy children’s fearful and anxious demeanor may directly inhibit their propensity to speak in social situations (81, 82), or whether such difficulties may rather reflect a “competence deficit,” whereby shy children’s withdrawn and restricted participation in social interaction may influence these children’s opportunities to learn and practice language skills (83). In the former case, one would expect language difficulties to account for (i.e., mediate) the links between shyness and poor adjustment, whereas in the latter case, one would expect language difficulties to influence (i.e., moderate) the strength of such associations.

      In many respects, the results of the current study add important knowledge to this ongoing debate. While there was no evidence of mediation effects of language skills for any of the prospective links, the results showed language competencies in preschool to moderate the longitudinal links between early shyness and later anxiety symptoms. More specifically, this moderating effect suggests that shyness and language skills represent two separate characteristics of the child, which, when co-occurring as high shyness and low language skills, may jointly put the child at risk of anxiety symptoms in the long run. However, when high shyness is accompanied by high language skills, shy children’s risk of later anxiety may decrease significantly. In this sense, our results primarily provide support for the view that language abilities may largely reflect a “competence deficit” rather than a “performance deficit.” Thus, bearing in mind the crucial impact of language skills for shy children’s adjustment in several developmental areas (2, 15, 42), our findings may have important implication for prevention purposes insofar as they highlight the potentially positive benefits of targeting shy children’s language abilities at an early age. Importantly, these results also indicate that language difficulty is not necessarily a defining feature of all shy children, but rather suggest that heterogeneity in shy children’s language abilities is likely to depend on the influence of other individual and/or environmental factors.

      4.3. The social functioning of shy children

      Additionally, the current study also demonstrated the protective benefits of early social play behaviors with peers for shy children’s later adjustment. More specifically, the association between childhood shyness and symptoms of anxiety at age eight was stronger at lower levels of social play behaviors but was found to decrease significantly at higher levels of social play behaviors. Previous research has, indeed, confirmed the benefits of positive social actions for experiencing positive emotions, sense of belonging, and overall well-being (84, 85). As such, this result adds to the growing body of research showing positive social behaviors to buffer against negative adjustment outcomes for shy children, for instance such as peer problems (14, 16) and social anxiety (4, 7).

      Drawing on previous suggestions, a possible mechanism behind such links could be that positive social behaviors and competencies are likely to facilitate more peer liking and more positive interactions with others, which ultimately may help reduce shy children’s risk of socioemotional problems (4). As such, shy children who practice social behaviors and skills may gradually familiarize themselves with others and, consequently, feel better and less socially wary and anxious in social situations. In this sense, the findings of our study add to the growing literature suggesting that when shyness is accompanied with positive behaviors or characteristics, it becomes less strongly associated with anxiety.

      4.4. Gender differences

      Although gender correlated significantly with several of the study variables, we did not find any significant gender differences in any of the associations between the study variables. In past research, there have been contradictory findings concerning this issue, with some studies reporting stronger associations between shyness and internalizing symptoms among boys relative to girls (45), while others find that such associations are stronger for girls than for boys or that there are no gender differences at all (86, 87). Potential reasons for this inconsistency in the findings might be due to differences in how shyness is operationalized and measured (i.e., as social withdrawal, conflicted shyness, or social disinterest) across different studies, as well as in when assessments are made (i.e., early vs. later childhood). For instance, although studies often show that there are no gender differences in shyness overall, there are indications that girls tend to “over-report” their shyness in middle and later childhood (86), which then could mask potential gender differences in associations with outcomes (88). Accordingly, further research into age-specific gender differences in the longitudinal associations between shyness (including different types of shyness) and developmental outcomes (i.e., social anxiety, loneliness) is clearly warranted.

      5. Limitations

      Despite the benefits of a longitudinal design, large sample size, and performing the analyses within a SEM framework, the present study also has limitations.

      First, the correlational nature of this study precludes us from drawing conclusions with respect to causality and direction of associations. For instance, it has been suggested that the direction of influence may not only flow from shyness to poor language, but that lower language abilities may also lead to shyness (2, 35). However, there is limited support for this suggestion with research showing that such patterns may mainly exist among boys but not girls (36).

      Second, we measured shyness by using the short form of the EAS which only includes three of the original five items. This may be problematic with respect to possible discrepancies in how shyness was operationalized and measured in this study. Yet, previous research has shown the short form to have satisfactory psychometric properties, including reliability and validity estimates approaching those of the original scale and with high correlations (r = 0.95) observed between the short-form and the original form (89). As such, these similarities may indicate that the short form, despite its limited number of items, is sufficient in terms of capturing the most essential and core features of the shyness phenomenon among children and youth.

      Third, the measure of anxiety symptoms in the current study taps general anxiety, whereas there is evidence to suggest that shyness is most strongly associated with social anxiety (28). This may partly explain why the direct association between shyness and anxiety at age 11 years was not significant. Thus, future studies should examine longitudinal associations between early shyness and specific types of anxiety symptoms across different phases of development.

      Fourth, the effect sizes for the associations between the study variables were small to moderate, and this warrants that our results should be interpreted with caution. The small effect sizes are problematic because although we demonstrate statistically significant results, this does not necessarily mean that they have practical significance, which is an important issue to consider with regards to intervention purposes. However, given that the assessment points expanded over a long period of time, we did not expect to see large effect sizes in this study. It is possible that the vast number of idiosyncratic experiences occurring within these formative child years may have contributed to the children’s socioemotional adjustment and language development above and beyond the contribution from the included variables in this study. Another possibility is that the over-representation of well-functioning and well-educated families in this study compared to the population in general (90), might have led to an underestimation of true effect sizes.

      Fifth, the validity and generalizability of the study findings may be restricted by general limitations of the MoBa sample, such as attrition, selection, and non-response bias. For instance, problems with self-selection bias and attrition may have resulted in biased estimates of the associations and underestimation of effect sizes (90). Recent research suggests using multiple imputation or inverse probability weighting to account for selection bias present in the MoBa cohort due to loss to follow-up (91). However, others question the appropriateness of using such imputation techniques uncritically as not all associations necessarily are impacted by selection bias (92). In the current study, we applied full information maximum likelihood with robust estimators (MLR) to handle missing data, following recommendations of Lodder et al. (73).

      Sixth, we had a valid ECEC center ID for around 78% of the sample but no equivalent ID at the school level in the study. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility of interdependence between the observations in cases where the same teachers reported on more than one target child. However, the issue on non-independence in observations should not be a substantial concern as the children participating in the MoBa are dispersed across different ECEC centers and schools all over Norway. Furthermore, we have in previous research using the same subsample shown that results remained the same when we adjusted standard errors for clustering at the ECEC level to allow for interdependence in the observations (50). Together, these aspects suggest that the effect of clustering is rather limited.

      Finally, as our investigation involved Norwegian children, their mothers, and teachers, the generalizability of the findings may be limited to Norwegian contexts.

      6. Conclusion

      In sum, the findings of the current study address some of the shortages in the literature concerning which early behavioral child characteristics that may influence the strength and direction of the prospective associations between childhood shyness and later internalizing and language problems. Our results provide empirical support to the theoretical propositions that positive behavioral features, such as social play behaviors and language competencies, may be particularly adaptive for shy children’s socio-emotional functioning and adjustment (13). As such, this study adds to the emerging body of research demonstrating the importance of exploring how traits may operate in an interactive manner in influencing shy children’s developmental outcomes (6, 93). In this sense, the present study offers novel input for both developmental and child personality research by providing evidence in support of a more heterogeneous and nuanced conceptualization of the shyness dimension. Most importantly, the above findings offer some optimistic implications for shy children by showing that positive behavioral assets, such as social play behaviors and language competencies, may buffer against these children’s increased risk of internalizing problems. Future intervention training programs should aim to reinforce shy children’s social behavior and communication skills, both to facilitate and aid their desire to interact and being accepted by others, but also to reduce their inhibited and withdrawn behaviors and thereby their subsequent risk for internalizing difficulties.

      Data availability statement

      The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

      Author contributions

      All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

      The Norwegian Mother, Father, and Child Cohort Study was supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research. We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Crozier WR. Shyness and self-esteem in middle childhood. Br J Educ Psychol. (1995) 65:8595. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1995.tb01133.x 7727270 Coplan RJ Weeks M. Shy and soft-spoken: shyness, pragmatic language, and socio-emotional adjustment in early childhood. Infant Child Dev. (2009) 18:23854. 10.1002/icd.622 Karevold EB Ystrøm E Coplan RJ Sanson AV Mathiesen KS. A prospective longitudinal study of shyness from infancy to adolescence: stability, age-related changes, and prediction of socio-emotional functioning. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2012) 40:116777. 10.1007/s10802-012-9635-6 22527608 Colonnesi C Napoleone E Bögels SM. Positive and negative expressions of shyness in toddlers: are they related to anxiety in the same way? J Pers Soc Psychol. (2014) 106:624. 10.1037/a0035561 24564372 Eggum-Wilkens ND Lemery-Chalfant K Aksan N Goldsmith HH. Self-conscious shyness: growth during toddlerhood, strong role of genetics, and no prediction from fearful shyness. Infancy. (2015) 20:16088. 10.1111/infa.12070 25685095 Poole KL Van Lieshout RJ Schmidt LA. Shyness and sociability beyond emerging adulthood: implications for understanding the developmental sequelae of shyness subtypes. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2017) 36:31634. 10.1521/jscp.2017.36.4.316 Poole KL Schmidt LA. Smiling through the shyness: the adaptive function of positive affect in shy children. Emotion. (2019) 19:160. 10.1037/emo0000426 29565610 White LK McDermott JM Degnan KA Henderson HA Fox NA. Behavioral inhibition and anxiety: the moderating roles of inhibitory control and attention shifting. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2011) 39:73547. 10.1007/s10802-011-9490-x 21301953 Sette S Hipson WE Zava F Baumgartner E Coplan RJ. Linking shyness with social and school adjustment in early childhood: the moderating role of inhibitory control. Early Educ Dev. (2018) 29:67590. 10.1080/10409289.2017.1422230 Hassan R Poole KL Schmidt LA. Revisiting the double-edged sword of self-regulation: linking shyness, attentional shifting, and social behavior in preschoolers. J Exp Child Psychol. (2020) 196:104842. 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104842 32387814 Hassan R Schmidt LA. Longitudinal investigation of shyness and physiological vulnerability: moderating influences of attention biases to threat and safety. Dev Psychobiol. (2021) 63:e22180. 10.1002/dev.22180 34423433 Susa-Erdogan G Benga O Colonnesi C. Expressions of positive and negative shyness in preschool-age children: temperamental correlates and visual attention to emotions. J Exp Child Psychol. (2022) 215:105315. 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105315 34801737 Coplan RJ Baldwin D Wood KR. Shy but getting by: protective factors in the links between childhood shyness and socio-emotional functionings. In: Schmidt LA Poole KL editors. Adaptive Shyness: Multiple Perspectives on Behavior and Development. New York, NY: Springer (2020). p. 6387. Gazelle H. Behavioral profiles of anxious solitary children and heterogeneity in peer relations. Dev Psychol. (2008) 44:160424. 10.1037/a0013303 18999325 Cheung HS Elliott JM. Child shyness and peer likeability: the moderating role of pragmatics and vocabulary. Br J Dev Psychol. (2017) 35:53145. 10.1111/bjdp.12192 28632319 Gazelle H Shell MD. Behavioral profiles of anxious solitary children: predicting peer relations trajectories from Third through Fifth Grades. Merrill Palmer Q. (2017) 63:23781. 10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.63.2.0237 Jahng KE. The moderating effect of children’s language abilities on the relation between their shyness and play behavior during peer play. Early Child Dev Care. (2018) 190:210618. 10.1080/03004430.2018.1560277 Asendorpf JB. Development of inhibited children’s coping with unfamiliarity. Child Dev. (1991) 62:146074. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01618.x Coplan RJ Prakash K O’Neil K Armer M. Do you “want” to play? Distinguishing between conflicted shyness and social disinterest in early childhood. Dev Psychol. (2004) 40:24458. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.244 14979764 Rubin KH Coplan RJ. The Development of Shyness and Social Withdrawal. New York, NY: Guilford (2010). Bohlin G Hagekull B Andersson K. Behavioral inhibition as a precursor of peer social competence in early school age: the interplay with attachment and nonparental care. Merrill Palmer Q. (2005) 51:119. 10.1353/mpq.2005.0001 34409987 Coplan RJ Arbeau KA. The stresses of a “brave new world”: shyness and school adjustment in kindergarten. J Res Child Educ. (2008) 22:37789. 10.1080/02568540809594634 Coplan RJ Rose-Krasnor L Weeks M Kingsbury A Kingsbury M Bullock A. Alone is a crowd: social motivations, social withdrawal, and socioemotional functioning in later childhood. Dev Psychol. (2013) 49:86175. 10.1037/a0028861 22686178 Asendorpf JB. Beyond social withdrawal: shyness, unsociability, and peer avoidance. Hum Dev. (1990) 33:2509. 10.1159/000276522 Schmidt LA Fox NA. Conceptual, biological, and behavioral distinctions among different categories of shy children. In: Schmidt LA Schulkin J editors. Series in Affective Science: Extreme Fear, Shyness, and Social Phobia: Origins, Biological Mechanisms, and Clinical Outcomes. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1999). p. 4766. Jones KM Schulkin J Schmidt LA. Shyness: subtypes, psychosocial correlates, and treatment interventions. Psychology. (2014) 5:24454. 10.4236/psych.2014.53035 Sang B Ding X Coplan RJ Liu J Pan T Feng X. Assessment and implications of social avoidance in Chinese early adolescents. J Early Adolesc. (2016) 38:55473. 10.1177/0272431616678988 Sandstrom A Uher R Pavlova B. Prospective association between childhood behavioral inhibition and anxiety: a meta-analysis. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. (2020) 48:5766. 10.1007/s10802-019-00588-5 31642030 Coplan RJ DeBow A Schneider BH Graham AA. The social behaviours of inhibited children in and out of preschool. Br J Dev Psychol. (2009) 27:891905. 10.1348/026151008X396153 19994485 Sette S Baumgartner E Laghi F Coplan RJ. The role of emotion knowledge in the links between shyness and children’s socio-emotional functioning at preschool. Br J Dev Psychol. (2016) 34:47188. 10.1111/bjdp.12144 27111863 Poole KL Cunningham CE Schmidt LA. Trajectories of observed shyness and psychosocial adjustment in children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2020) 51:63647. 10.1007/s10578-020-00962-1 32020501 Bohlin G Hagekull B. Socio-emotional development: from infancy to young adulthood. Scand J Psychol. (2009) 50:592601. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00787.x 19930258 Crozier WR Badawood A. Shyness, vocabulary and children’s reticence in Saudi Arabian preschools. Infant Child Dev. (2009) 18:25570. 10.1002/icd.623 Van Kleeck A Street R. Does reticence mean just talking less? Qualitative differences in the language of talkative and reticent preschoolers. J Psycholinguist Res. (1982) 11:60929. 10.1007/BF01067615 Coplan RJ Evans MA. At a loss for words? Introduction to the special issue on shyness and language in childhood. Infant Child Dev. (2009) 18:2115. 10.1002/icd.620 Smith Watts AK Patel D Corley RP Friedman NP Hewitt JK Robinson JL Testing alternative hypotheses regarding the association between behavioral inhibition and language development in toddlerhood. Child Dev. (2014) 85:156985. 10.1111/cdev.12219 24499266 Coplan RJ Wichmann C Lagacé-Séguin DG. Solitary-active play behavior: a marker variable for maladjustment in the preschool? J Res Child Educ. (2001) 15:16472. 10.1080/02568540109594957 Findlay LC Coplan RJ. Come out and play: shyness in childhood and the benefits of organized sports participation. Can J Behav Sci. (2008) 40:15361. 10.1037/0008-400X.40.3.153 Karevold EB Coplan RJ Stoolmiller M Mathiesen KS. A longitudinal study of the links between temperamental shyness, activity, and trajectories of internalising problems from infancy to middle childhood. Aust J Psychol. (2011) 63:3643. 10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00005.x Thorell L Bohlin G Rydell AM. Two types of inhibitory control: predictive relations to social functioning. Int J Behav Dev. (2004) 28:193203. 10.1080/01650250344000389 Brooker RJ Kiel EJ Buss KA. Early social fear predicts kindergarteners’ socially anxious behaviors: direct associations, moderation by inhibitory control, and differences from nonsocial fear. Emotion. (2016) 16:9971010. 10.1037/emo0000135 27213729 Coplan RJ Armer M. Talking yourself out of being shy: shyness, expressive vocabulary, and socioemotional adjustment in preschool. Merrill Palmer Q. (2005) 51:2041. 10.1353/mpq.2005.0004 34409987 Baardstu S Coplan RJ Eliassen E Brandlistuen RE Wang MV. Exploring the role of teacher–child relationships in the longitudinal associations between childhood shyness and social functioning at school: a prospective cohort study. School Ment Health. (2022) 14:98496. 10.1007/s12310-022-09518-1 Sette S Zava F Baumgartner E Laghi F Coplan RJ. Exploring the role of play behaviors in the links between preschoolers’ shyness and teacher-child relationships. Early Educ Dev. (2022) 33:187203. 10.1080/10409289.2021.1885237 Doey L Coplan RJ Kingsbury M. Bashful boys and coy girls: a review of gender differences in childhood shyness. Sex Roles. (2014) 70:25566. 10.1007/s11199-013-0317-9 Kopala-Sibley DC Klein DN. Distinguishing types of social withdrawal in children: internalizing and externalizing outcomes of conflicted shyness versus social disinterest across childhood. J Res Pers. (2017) 67:2735. 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.01.003 29622851 Schwartz CE Snidman N Kagan J. Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1999) 38:100815. 10.1097/00004583-199908000-00017 10434493 Bekkhus M McVarnock A Coplan RJ Ulset V Kraft B. Developmental changes in the structure of shyness and internalizing symptoms from early to middle childhood: a network analysis. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. (2022): 10.31234/osf.io/9m68s Letourneau NL Duffett-Leger L Levac L Watson B Young-Morris C. Socioeconomic status and child development: a meta-analysis. J Emot Behav Disord. (2011) 21:21124. 10.1177/1063426611421007 Alexandersen N Zachrisson HD Wilhelmsen T Wang MV Brandlistuen RE. Predicting selection into ECEC of higher quality in a universal context: the role of parental education and income. Early Child Res Q. (2021) 55:33648. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.01.001 Magnus P Irgens LM Haug K Nystad W Skjærven R Stoltenberg C Cohort profile: the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol. (2006) 35:114650. 10.1093/ije/dyl170 16926217 Magnus P Birke C Vejrup K Haugan A Alsaker E Daltveit AK Cohort profile update: the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol. (2016) 45:3828. 10.1093/ije/dyw029 27063603 Irgens LM. The medical birth registry of Norway. Epidemiological research and surveillance throughout 30 years. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. (2000) 79:4359. 10.1034/j.1600-0412.2000.079006435.x 11841302 Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). Oslo: Norwegian Institute of Public Health (2019). Buss AH Plomin R. Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (1984). Walker KL Ammaturo DA Wright KD. Are we assessing temperament appropriately? The emotionality activity sociability and impulsivity (EASI) temperament scale: a systematic psychometric review. Can Psychol. (2017) 58:31632. 10.1037/cap0000108 Coplan RJ Rubin KH. Exploring and assessing nonsocial play in the preschool: the development and validation of the preschool play behavior scale. Soc Dev. (1998) 7:7291. Kesäläinen J Suhonen E Alijoki A Sajaniemi N. Children’s play behaviour, cognitive skills and vocabulary in integrated early childhood special education groups. Int J Inclusive Educ. (2019) 26:117. 10.1080/13603116.2019.1651410 Ho LC. Validation of preschool play behavior scale for korean preschool children (PPBS-K). Life Sci Res. (2021) 25:111. 10.36357/johe.2021.25.3.1 Brandlistuen RE Flatø M Stoltenberg C Helland SS Wang MV. Gender gaps in preschool age: a study of behavior, neurodevelopment and pre-academic skills. Scand J Public Health. (2020) 49:1403494820944740. 10.1177/1403494820944740 32781908 Choo MS Xu Y Haron PF. Subtypes of nonsocial play and psychosocial adjustment in Malaysian preschool children. Soc Dev. (2012) 21:294312. 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00630.x Aslan ÖM. Turkish and American preschoolers’ play, aggression and victimization behaviours in play context. Early Child Dev Care. (2020) 190:34863. 10.1080/03004430.2018.1472088 Ireton H. Child Development Inventory, Manual. Minneapolis, MN: Behavior Science Systems (1992). Chaffee CA Cunningham CE Secord-Gilbert M Elbard H Richards J. Screening effectiveness of the Minnesota child development inventory expressive and receptive language scales: sensitivity, specificity, and predictive value. Psychol Assess. (1990) 2:805. 10.1037/1040-3590.2.1.80 Ottem E. Spørsmål om språkferdigheter: en analyse av sammenhengen mellom observasjonsdata og testdata. Skolepsykologi. (2009) 1:1127. Bishop DV. The Children’s Communication Checklist. London: Psychological Corporation London (2003). Hollund-Møllerhaug L. Children’s communication checklist (CCC-2): en sammenligning av GCC-skårenes persentilfordeling i et britisk og norsk utvalg. Tidsskrift Norsk Psykol. (2010) 47:8246. Birmaher B Khetarpal S Brent D Cully M Balach L Kaufman J The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1997) 36:54553. 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018 9100430 Birmaher B Brent DA Chiappetta L Bridge J Monga S Baugher M. Psychometric properties of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1999) 38:12306. 10.1097/00004583-199910000-00011 10517055 Angold A Costello EJ Messer SC Pickles A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (1995) 5:23749. Fernández-Martínez I Morales A Méndez FX Espada JP Orgilés M. Spanish adaptation and psychometric properties of the parent version of the short mood and feelings questionnaire (SMFQ-P) in a non-clinical sample of young school-aged children. Span J Psychol. (2020) 23:e45. 10.1017/SJP.2020.47 33148355 Muthen L Muthen B. MPlus User’s Guide. 8th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen (2017). Lodder P Denollet J Emons WHM Nefs G Pouwer F Speight J Modeling interactions between latent variables in research on type D personality: a Monte Carlo simulation and clinical study of depression and anxiety. Multivar Behav Res. (2019) 54:63765. 10.1080/00273171.2018.1562863 30977400 Hu L Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. (1999) 6:155. 10.1080/10705519909540118 Widaman KF Ferrer E Conger RD. Factorial invariance within longitudinal structural equation models: measuring the same construct across time. Child Dev Perspect. (2010) 4:108. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00110.x 20369028 Klein A Moosbrugger H. Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction effects with the LMS method. Psychometrika. (2000) 65:45774. 10.1007/BF02296338 Maslowsky J Jager J Hemken D. Estimating and interpreting latent variable interactions: a tutorial for applying the latent moderated structural equations method. Int J Behav Dev. (2015) 39:8796. 10.1177/0165025414552301 26478643 Hayes AF. Beyond baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun Monogr. (2009) 76:40820. 10.1080/03637750903310360 Rubin KH Coplan RJ Bowker JC. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. (2009) 60:14171. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642 18851686 Rapee RM Coplan RJ. Conceptual relations between anxiety disorder and fearful temperament. New Direct Child Adolesc Dev. (2010) 2010:1731. 10.1002/cd.260 20205179 Crozier WR Perkins P. Shyness as a factor when assessing children. Educ Psychol Pract. (2002) 18:23944. 10.1080/0266736022000010267 Spere KA Schmidt LA Theall-Honey LA Martin-Chang S. Expressive and receptive language skills of temperamentally shy preschoolers. Infant Child Dev. (2004) 13:12333. 10.1002/icd.345 Evans MA. Reticent primary grade children and their more talkative peers: verbal, nonverbal, and self-concept characteristics. J Educ Psychol. (1996) 88:73949. 10.1037/0022-0663.88.4.739 Eisenberg N Eggum ND Di Giunta L. Empathy-related responding: associations with prosocial behavior, aggression, and intergroup relations. Soc Issues Policy Rev. (2010) 4:14380. 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2010.01020.x 21221410 Weinstein N Ryan RM. When helping helps: autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2010) 98:22244. 10.1037/a0016984 20085397 Rubin KH Burgess KB Coplan RJ. Social withdrawal and shyness. In: Smith PK Hart CH editors. Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. London: Blackwell (2004). p. 33052. Rubin KH Barstead MG. Gender differences in child and adolescent social withdrawal: a commentary. Sex Roles. (2014) 70:27484. 10.1007/s11199-014-0357-9 25709144 Findlay LC Coplan RJ Bowker A. Keeping it all inside: shyness, internalizing coping strategies and socio-emotional adjustment in middle childhood. Int J Behav Dev. (2009) 33:4754. 10.1177/0165025408098017 Mathiesen KS Tambs K. The EAS temperament questionnaire: factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1999) 40:4319. 10.1111/1469-7610.00460 Biele G Gustavson K Czajkowski NO Nilsen RM Reichborn-Kjennerud T Magnus PM Bias from self selection and loss to follow-up in prospective cohort studies. Eur J Epidemiol. (2019) 34:92738. 10.1007/s10654-019-00550-1 31451995 Vejrup K Magnus P Magnus M. Lost to follow-up in the Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. (2022) 36:3009. 10.1111/ppe.12821 34797579 Hughes RA Heron J Sterne JAC Tilling K. Accounting for missing data in statistical analyses: multiple imputation is not always the answer. Int J Epidemiol. (2019) 48:1294304. 10.1093/ije/dyz032 30879056 Xu Y Farver JAM Yu L Zhang Z. Three types of shyness in Chinese children and the relation to effortful control. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2009) 97:106173. 10.1037/a0016576 19968419
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016gthmjs.com.cn
      www.mallbao.com.cn
      www.fupwdo.com.cn
      iuptnb.com.cn
      hlcitq.com.cn
      www.holdzu.com.cn
      www.gdesigner.com.cn
      sohb.com.cn
      www.nqchain.com.cn
      wjwunu.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p