Front. Neuroinform. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics Front. Neuroinform. 1662-5196 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fninf.2023.1233121 Neuroscience Original Research Time to consider animal data governance: perspectives from neuroscience Eke Damian 1 * Ogoh George 2 Knight William 1 Stahl Bernd 2 1Centre for Computing and Social Responsibility, De Montfort University, Leicester, United Kingdom 2School of Computer Science, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom

Edited by: Christian Haselgrove, UMass Chan Medical School, United States

Reviewed by: Dov Greenbaum, Yale University, United States; Orla Shortall, The James Hutton Institute, United Kingdom

*Correspondence: Damian Eke, damian.eke@dmu.ac.uk
29 08 2023 2023 17 1233121 01 06 2023 09 08 2023 Copyright © 2023 Eke, Ogoh, Knight and Stahl. 2023 Eke, Ogoh, Knight and Stahl

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Introduction

Scientific research relies mainly on multimodal, multidimensional big data generated from both animal and human organisms as well as technical data. However, unlike human data that is increasingly regulated at national, regional and international levels, regulatory frameworks that can govern the sharing and reuse of non-human animal data are yet to be established. Whereas the legal and ethical principles that shape animal data generation in many countries and regions differ, the generated data are shared beyond boundaries without any governance mechanism. This paper, through perspectives from neuroscience, shows conceptually and empirically that there is a need for animal data governance that is informed by ethical concerns. There is a plurality of ethical views on the use of animals in scientific research that data governance mechanisms need to consider.

Methods

Semi-structured interviews were used for data collection. Overall, 13 interviews with 12 participants (10 males and 2 females) were conducted. The interviews were transcribed and stored in NviVo 12 where they were thematically analyzed.

Results

The participants shared the view that it is time to consider animal data governance due to factors such as differences in regulations, differences in ethical principles, values and beliefs and data quality concerns. They also provided insights on possible approaches to governance.

Discussion

We therefore conclude that a procedural approach to data governance is needed: an approach that does not prescribe a particular ethical position but allows for a quick understanding of ethical concerns and debate about how different positions differ to facilitate cross-cultural and international collaboration.

animal research animal data neuroscience data governance ethics dumping regulations

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      1. Introduction

      In the last decade, the need to ensure reproducibility of research results and to justify public investment in research has led to increased sharing of research data and the imperative for open sharing. Open data platforms supported by research projects have increasingly become the center piece for facilitating open sharing of research data. In neuroscience, a number of these open platforms exist and share big, multitype and multifunctional data from diverse species of organisms for both research and innovation. As Poldrack and Gorgolewski (2014) pointed out, these platforms not only encourage data re-use and increase statistical power to stimulate translational knowledge but also expand the reach and impact of neuroscience research. A critical implication of this data re-use expansion is that data increasingly interacts with different jurisdictions with different regulatory requirements. While most of these datasets are from human participants, many are generated from animals.

      Unlike human data that is nationally or regionally regulated (e.g., EU General Data Protection Regulation1), there are no established legal frameworks that govern the sharing and re-use of animal data nationally or internationally. One reason for this is that the sharing or use of animal data does not raise the traditional data use concerns associated with human data (Stahl et al., 2019) such as; privacy, fairness, human rights and security. The ethical and legal issues around animal data are usually raised during data creation. The scientific, ethical and legal validity of animal research data are mostly determined by the nature of the research procedure/experiment. The moral and legal questions of animal experiments do not always revolve around the implications of animal data usage but on the ethical and legal permissibility of its scientific generation. Crucially, the regulatory and ethical principles that shape animal data generation in many countries and regions are different while the generated data are shared beyond boundaries without any governance mechanism. This means that animal data generated in less restrictive places are openly shared in countries with very restrictive requirements mostly through open data platforms. Thus, this paper asks the question: is it time to consider animal data governance?

      The paper shows conceptually and empirically that there is a need for animal data governance that is informed by ethical concerns. Animal data raises different ethical concerns from human data and thus needs to be treated differently. There is a plurality of ethical perspectives on the use of animals in research which any data governance regime will need to take into account. This article therefore arrives at the conclusion that a procedural approach to data governance is called for that does not prescribe a particular ethical position but allows for a quick understanding of ethical concerns and debate about how different positions differ to facilitate cross-cultural and international collaboration.

      We organize this paper as follows. We explore the current international data governance ecosystem for responsible biomedical research and innovation; the continued use of animals for neuroscience research, especially non-human primates. We then provide a thematic analysis of interviews conducted with international neuroscientists who conduct animal experiments. On this basis we arrive at the conclusion that a procedural approach to international data governance is called for.

      2. The continued use of animals in neuroscience research

      Despite the increasing requirements to implement the 3Rs (replacement, refinement, and reduction) (Russell and Burch, 1960; Guhad, 2005), the use of animals in research continues in many parts of the world, especially in neuroscience research (perhaps more than in any other field of biomedical research) (Jones, 2021). Although public interest in the use of animals for research has significantly reduced the number of animal experiments in Europe and North America (Lankau et al., 2014; European Commission, 2019), animals continue to be central to scientific research in other parts of the world. The rationale for this is varied. Neuroscience research often involves invasive and non-invasive methods that cannot be conducted with humans because of associated risks and ethical concerns. Thus, neuroscientists turn to other animals such as; rodents, ferrets, dogs, pigs, zebra fish and monkeys whose usage in scientific research comes with considerably fewer ethical concerns. The argument for this, borders on the lack of safe and non-invasive approaches to studying the human brain (Preuss, 2010). Rodents are widely used because of their short gestation periods, their cost-effective production and have proved unquestionably effective (Neuhaus, 2018). Fruit flies (Zweier et al., 2009) and zebrafish (Haesemeyer and Schier, 2015) have also proved to be important research resources for neuroscience research.

      The use of animals in invasive and intrusive neuroscience research is generally informed by the legal and ethical restrictions of such research with human brains. However, many neuroscientists suggest that there are anatomical and genetic limitations/differences that hamper the scope of their use (Garner, 2014; Windisch, 2014). These animal subjects or models present challenges Kaiser and Feng (2015) referred to as the lack of face validity and predictive validity. The delays in the development of new interventions for brain diseases are associated with the differences in pathophysiological mechanisms between rodents and humans (Ting and Feng, 2013). Differences in brain functions and cognitive behavior, difficulties in scalability of dosage regimens, differences in recovery times and differences in the ratio of white to gray matter in the brain are some of the issues that inform this lack of transnationality (Varki, 2000; Weatheall, 2015). For better predictive validity, some neuroscientists assert that non-human primates (NHPs) are particularly better subjects.

      2.1. NHPs in neuroscience

      Available draft genome sequences of primates have shown that there are important similarities between human and NHP genomes. NHPs have, indeed, been revealed as our closest relatives with regard to the DNA sequence of our genome (Li and Saunders, 2005). While the Chimpanzee genome is 98.77% similar to the human genome (Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium, 2005), the rhesus macaque has a 93.5% similarity (Disotell and Tosi, 2007). This phylogenetic proximity to humans and related similarities in anatomy, physiology and behavior form the basis for justification of the use of NHPs in neuroscience experiments (Tardif et al., 2013; Friedman et al., 2017). A further comparative study of primate genomics has also shown that the most significant evolutionary change between primates happened in the brain (Sikela, 2006). These similarities form the basis of justification and a remarkable curiosity to unlock the brain’s complex structure and functions through experiments with NHPs. Today, NHPs are mainly used in basic/fundamental neurobiological research to explore how brain circuits contribute to human brain activities such as; perception, attention, memory and emotion (Bystron et al., 2006). In Europe, some neuroscientists are engaged in this type of research even though the number of invasive/intrusive experiments ongoing is not known (Scientific Committee on Health, Environmental and Emerging Risks [SCHEER], 2017). They are also used in translational and applied neuroscience research (Capitanio and Emborg, 2008) aimed at understanding the causes and development of potential treatments of brain disorders. Even though the scientific and translational validity of NHP experiments have been questioned (Knight, 2007; Bailey and Taylor, 2016), NHP research in neuroscience continues to develop new tools and approaches.

      There is also a steady rise in neurological alterations of NHPs to model human brain diseases/functions and to study the genetic mechanisms that inform human specific neurological changes (Shi et al., 2019). This can be in the form of neural grafting (Bjugstad and Sladek, 2006), transgenesis (Chan, 2014) or other forms of human-NHP chimerism. These experiments significantly alter the neurobiological appearance, behavior or genetic makeup of the NHP causing phenotypic changes. Transgenesis refers to the artificial transfer of a foreign gene into the genome of another organism in order to introduce or delete characteristics of the phenotype (Mepham et al., 1998). In neuroscience, this can involve the use of HLS (human lineage specific) genes to create transgenic NHPs to demonstrate changes in brain structure (like brain size), function (such as high cognition) or to model diseases (like autism, Huntington diseases etc.). These genetic alterations of NHPs are developed with customized mutations and have shown to be able to model human brain disorders like Parkinson’s (Yun et al., 2015), Schizophrenia (Qiu and Li, 2017), Alzheimer’s (Yeo et al., 2015), autism (Cyranoski, 2016; Zhao et al., 2018) and Huntington’s disease (Tomioka et al., 2017). Another invasive NHP experiment in neuroscience involves the transplantation of human-derived neural cells into an NHP to model “human-like behavior”- neural grafting. These and similar neuroscience research experiments with NHPs present unique concerns. The controversy is not confined to the scientific community but extends to the wider public. But the fact that significant neurological similarities between humans and NHPs raise ethical concerns that needs attention and some agreement by many stakeholders (Conlee and Rowan, 2012; Carvalho et al., 2018). In essence, the unique usefulness of NHPs neuroscience research also shapes the unique ethical and legal questions they raise. Overall, this increases the imperative for animal data governance in neuroscience.

      3. Current international data governance ecosystem

      Data governance is defined as “the principles, procedures, frameworks, and policies that ensure acceptable and responsible processing of data at each stage of the data life cycle, from collection, storage, processing, curation, sharing, and use to deletion” (Eke D. O. et al., 2022). The emphasis on the data life cycle demonstrates that data governance is not only required for a specific stage of the life-cycle. It is a robust framework that starts before data collection and continues to the deletion stage. Fothergill et al. (2019) described it as the overall management of the availability, usability, integrity, quality, and security of data in order to ensure that the potential of the data is maximized while regulatory and ethical compliance is achieved within a specific organizational context. This definition introduces ethical compliance as an important aspect of data governance. Data governance is therefore more than legal compliance (Eke D. et al., 2022). It includes adherence to available ethical principles.

      Furthermore, whereas the interpretations of data governance in organizations, disciplines and projects are different (Stahl et al., 2018), its goals and objectives are rooted in available laws and ethics. The question of whose laws and ethical values is determined by the context. Available regulations and ethical values are still jurisdictionally constrained while data continues to cross borders and socio-cultural contexts. For instance, data protection laws are established for specific jurisdictions (e.g., EU GDPR, USA HIPAA2, Canada’s PIPEDA3 etc.). Ethical values and principles that shape data governance also emerge from specific socio-cultural backgrounds. This means that the meaning or interpretations of data ethics principles such as trust, autonomy, privacy and consent are different in different cultures and societies. These inform relative interpretations of data governance in different cultures.

      It is also important to note that established data related regulations and ethical narratives focus mainly on human data. The literature and practice of data ethics and data protection exists to address issues that affect humans in the data processing pipelines. To the best of our knowledge, there is no existing regulation established to address ethical, legal and societal issues related to animal data. This is true for both research and non-research settings. In their systematic literature review of ethical principles that shape data governance discourse in neuroscience, (Ochang et al., 2022) identified a number of ethical principles that often shape data governance discourse in brain research. None of the principles identified touched on animal data concerns. That means that data governance mechanisms often exclude animal data concerns as it relates to ethics and the law. Aspects of technical elements of animal data governance are, however, often included in discussions on findable, accessible, interoperable, reusable (FAIR) data principles. These include aspects of data standardization, integration and interoperability. This falls short of addressing ethical and legal concerns related to the different stages of animal data lifecycle while animal experiments continue to be critical parts of biomedical research.

      4. Emerging interests in animal data governance in neuroscience

      For a number of reasons, including socio-cultural, ethical and legal differences that inform what is considered permissible use of animals in research, there is a growing interest in the governance of animal data (Eke D. O. et al., 2022). Perceptions on ethical concerns about the use of animals, particularly NHPs, in biomedical research are fundamentally shaped by diverse socio-cultural norms, ethical principles and regulatory requirements. This is evident in the fact that while the use of NHPs in research has decreased significantly in Europe and America due to increased social animal welfare activism, NHPs are still the central focus of big neuroscience projects in Asia (Okano et al., 2016; Poo et al., 2016).

      Advancements in genome-editing technologies such as the CRISPR/Cas system and artificial intelligence amplifies the huge possibilities in generating genetically modified NHPs. Although this emerging field (genetically modified NHP) has the “potential to transform the study of higher brain function and dramatically facilitate the development of effective treatment for human brain disorders” (Feng et al., 2020), it raises significant ethical concerns. Responding to the huge potential of genetically modified NHPs, Rommelfanger et al. (2018) raise the concern that researchers may be able to introduce cognitive capabilities that can contribute to blurring the boundaries of personhood and ultimately alter traditional perceptions of animal ethics. In 2019, a group of researchers from the Kunming Institute of Zoology in China claimed to have created transgenic monkeys with improved cognitive capacity (Shi et al., 2019). These modified monkeys were created with human MCPH1 genes and were not modeling any human diseases. They were simply modified to be phenotypically humanlike. During a series of cognitive tests, the researchers reported that these animals displayed better short-term memory than their counterparts in the wild. Basically, their brain development mirrored human brain development in many respects. The underlying logic behind this research, which is to manipulate monkeys to model humanlike capacities, presents a slippery slope concern. The question is where would the line be drawn in the path to generate human-like animals? To say the least, such research will not be permissible in many socio-cultural, ethical and legal contexts where the moral status of NHPs are hotly debated.

      Given these differences in attitudes, values, principles, beliefs, regulations on the use of animals in research, Rommelfanger et al. (2018) further noted that, “sharing of brain data between countries that hold different ethical stances on what is considered appropriate animal experimentation raises additional questions.” One of such questions is; “Should a country accept or use data collected elsewhere in a fashion that is not considered locally ethical?” (Ibid). This is a critical question at the heart of animal data governance consideration. It presents an ethical dilemma many scientists currently face in the neuroscience data sharing ecosystem (Eke D. O. et al., 2022). One project that has stated this as a concern is the PRIMatE Data Exchange (PRIME-DE) Consortium that has highlighted the lack of international standards and regulations as barriers to fostering international NHP data sharing and collaborations (Milham et al., 2020). This increased interest in neuroscience deserves more exploration and hence this paper.

      5. Methodology

      The issue of responsible animal data governance requires multi-stakeholder perspectives and insights (Rommelfanger et al., 2018; Eke D. et al., 2022). It calls for the appreciation of diverse cultural values and beliefs while respecting established ethical frameworks. Thus, a semi-structured interview was selected as the methodological choice. The underlying research philosophy, therefore, is to use social actors to provide in-depth and rich perspectives on the reality of animal data governance. The aim was to provide detailed and reasoned insights rather than objectively generalisable positions.

      The target population included researchers around the globe who have conducted or are conducting animal experiments to answer diverse neuroscience questions. Participants were drawn from active research projects under the International Brain Initiative (IBI). The IBI is the umbrella body for all the large-scale brain research initiatives including the EU Human Brain Project, the US Brain Initiative, Japan Brain/MINDS, Australian Brain Alliance, Korean Brain Initiative, Canadian Brain Research Strategy, and China Brain Project. We also drew participants from Africa and Latin America. Initial list of 37 researchers was compiled and interview invitations extended to all of them. A total of 15 people accepted the invitation and 3 later withdrew citing busy schedules. A structured interview guide that aligns well with the principles of qualitative research design (Ragin and Amoroso, 2011) was developed and tested on two colleagues. Following feedback from these initial tests, the interview protocol was improved before the start of the interviews.

      Ethics approval was obtained for this research from De Montfort University ethics review committee. Information sheet and an informed consent form were then emailed to participants. The information sheet contained comprehensive data on the research including but not limited to the research objectives, expectations from the participants and responsibilities of researchers, potential risks and benefits, the voluntariness of participation and how research data will be used. All participants returned the consent form before the interviews and further verbal consent was sought at the start of the interview to record the session. The interviews were conducted either in person or virtually via Skype or zoom and took approximately 40 min each to complete. These interviews occurred between January 2020 and November 2021. Overall, 13 interviews with 12 participants (10 males and 2 females) were conducted. One participant was interviewed twice because the first interview was cut short due to technical difficulties. A total of 13 interviews were considered sufficient to achieve saturation because according to Guest et al. (2006), theoretical saturation can be achieved even in 12 interviews and basic elements for metathemes can emerge as early as six interviews.

      The interviews were transcribed and stored in NviVo 12 where they were inductively coded for themes by the first author (DE). The inductive coding process involved reading through the transcript and identifying common patterns and themes. Thematically, the coding tree included high level nodes that show participants’ perspectives on why it is time to consider animal data governance or otherwise (such as cultural differences, legal differences etc.). Specific themes that align with high level themes are coded as sub-nodes. For example, ethics dumping was identified as a sub-node under the high-level node of ethical differences.

      6. Key findings

      One of the key results that emerged from the interviews was that all the participants agreed that due to the increasing transfer of animal data across countries, it is time to consider animal data governance for a number of reasons. The reasons provided by the participants are diverse and include: differences in regulations, socio-cultural and ethical values and beliefs, as well as scientific quality (see Table 1).

      Summary of key findings.

      Reasons why it is time to consider animal data governance
      Differences in regulations  ● Animal welfare is regulated differently in different countries and regions.  ● While NHPs remain the most suitable animal for research in Neuroscience, their use in research are legally restricted in many countries.  ● There are no identifiable existing laws in these countries (with restrictive laws) against the use of animal data. However, in countries where animal experiments are legally not allowed, it should not be legal to use data that emerge from such experiments conducted in other countries.
      Differences in socio-cultural values, beliefs, attitudes, and ethics  ● Socio-cultural values, belief systems, attitudes and ethics shape regulations on the use of animals in research.  ● Whereas some cultures allow and even encourage the use of animals in research, other cultures actively work against the use of animals in research.  ● Strict animal welfare regulations and social activism against the use of animals in research are pushing researchers to outsource ethically questionable experiments to countries where there are little or no laws–ethics dumping. Animal data governance will help to prevent ethics dumping and ensure that socio-cultural values and ethics are adhered to.
      Data quality concerns  ● Good science requires good data from reliable experiments.  ● The quality of animal data depends on good animal care (overly stressed animals are bad subjects). Animal data governance will help to ensure good animal care.
      Approaches to animal data governance
       ● Requires a multi-stakeholder dialog for the co-creation of actionable frameworks (involving people from different cultures and animal).  ● Comparative study of existing regulations and the socio-cultural values and beliefs underlying them.  ● Can build on established minimum standards used for journal publications.  ● Requires regulations similar to human data regulations.  ● International research infrastructures where researchers meet data from different regions of the world will play in vital role in achieving effective animal data governance for research and innovation.
      6.1. Differences in regulations

      A number of the participants pointed out legal differences as one of the major reasons why it is time to consider the governance of animal data. For instance, one participant stated that: “although there is no regulation related to the use of animal data that I know of, there should be one since there are differences in regulations for the generation of animal data”- (P2).

      Another participant also confirmed that: “Regulations inside laboratories around the world are not the same, why should the data we generate be treated the same way?” (P1).

      Furthermore, another participant presented these regulatory differences with an example of how it prevents collaborations: “I have colleagues in the UK and we discussed collaborative projects but it turned out to be impossible because the Ethical Research Council’s standards do not match Japanese standards. On another occasion, how to use data obtained from Japan, because it was monkey data, was the major problem” (P5).

      To reiterate this angle of NHPs, one participant stated that: “It is not just that Africa has the animals or the research is cheaper here but it is because that they are allowed to do in some African countries, especially with NHPs, they are not legally allowed to do in their own countries” (P11).

      The mention of non-human primate (NHP) here is critical because as pointed out above, differences in animal welfare are more amplified when it involves NHPs. These legal differences lead to an ethical question of how to share and use data generated from experiments legally not permissible in certain regions of the world.

      Some participants raised further issues and concerns related to power imbalance that may emerge due to non-harmonized regulations. One participant observed that: “If one country has a very loose standard, this country is capable of doing many things that could not be done in some other places. This group will dominate its power in the science of new data maybe. I think this is happening now. If there cannot be harmonized welfare regulations due to many factors, then we should have some open-minded policy discussions on responsible sharing of the datasets” (P1).

      This observation shows that differences in regulation are giving researchers in certain countries (with less strict provisions) scientific advantage over others. In responding to this, another participant stated that: “We ran into that with stem cells, in the United States. And the argument was, ‘well, if you don’t fund it, they’re going to fund it over there, and we’re going to fall behind.’ but you cannot build your industries on the back of something that is ethically wrong” (P3).

      This was an important point since regulations are fundamentally shaped by societal values which are different in different regions. These findings align with evidence that has been demonstrated in literature. Vasbinder and Locke (2016) provided an overview of regulatory frameworks across the globe that demonstrated that whereas there are common standards across different jurisdictions, there are clear differences in how different countries regulate the use of animals in research. Most importantly, they identified that animal research is “performed in African and Middle Eastern countries, but many of these countries have not yet enacted legislation nor established regulatory oversight, policies or guidelines” (Vasbinder and Locke, 2016 p. 263). Mitchell et al. (2021) have also provided detailed insights on the differences in regulatory provisions guiding the use of primates in neuroscience across countries which is the focus of this research. Some of the differences they pointed out include but are not limited to; how NHPs should be generated for use in research (e.g., the use of wild NHPs for research purposes are banned in the EU, in China prior to COVID-19 pandemic this was allowed). Another difference they pointed out is the sizing and flooring of the home enclosures and caging. The EU provides that NHPs should be housed in larger sized enclosures and cages while in China and other Asian countries the cages are smaller (Mitchell et al., 2021). These differences have implications such as forming barriers to effective international collaborations and global data sharing (Rommelfanger et al., 2018; Milham et al., 2020).

      6.2. Differences in ethical principles, values and beliefs

      It has been established in literature that the diverse socio-cultural values, beliefs, attitudes and ethics found in many regions of the world greatly influence the available diverse animal welfare regulations (Masiga and Munyua, 2005; Szucs et al., 2012; Garcia and McGlone, 2022). The participants highlighted the conflicting cultural and religious beliefs that make animal data governance an imperative. As one participant put it:

      I think policy of the animal ethics and welfare differ between countries…there are conflicting concepts and beliefs… and there should be respect for people’s cultures” (P6).

      Another observation was;

      Our cultures are different. In our culture we believe that using animals for a scientific purpose is exactly the same as killing animals to eat to maintain our lives because the scientific research leads to the development of human medicine. That is culturally accepted. However, it is different in Europe” (P5).

      These opinions suggest that the acceptability of generation of animal research data is different in different societal contexts. There are differences in the understanding and conceptualization of ethical concerns associated with animal experiment. One of these concerns involves the idea of inflicting pain on animals. One participant stated that when scientists are inducing psychiatric disorders, they are creating suffering for the animals which is something that should be deeply examined. Reacting to pain associated with inducing psychiatric disorders in animals, a participant observed that; “there are ethical concerns, and I think there should be a very deep and profound discussion about it” (P7).

      However, there was an opinion to always focus on the balance between costs and benefits of research experiments that cause pain. For this participant, “cost and benefit balance is the most critical issue… such a kind of disease model can cause some painful situations in these animals and even this can also be an experiment for the chronic pain you know” (P4).

      Furthermore, the idea of pain and how it affects animals is amplified when NHPs are involved.

      The general belief that monkeys are better animals for experiments because they are closer to humans in cognitive abilities suggests that they will feel more pain than rats and others. This is what we have come to know…I don’t believe researchers in so many other places respect this. We know too much about primates, we know too much about their sociality, we know too much of this for us to use them as our own personal lab rats (P8).

      This is because NHPs are sophisticated, capable entities (P1). Therefore, we should be thinking about minimizing suffering (P1) rather than inflicting pain on them.

      There are also heightened ethical concerns when transgenic NHPs are involved. According to a participant; Technology has advanced and many tools are now available. You don’t want to create monster-killing animals that can be used for military purposes and things like that (P9).

      Similarly, one participant observed that advancement in technologies are improving research but may be used in unethical ways. We do have the tools in our hands to start playing dangerous games. That is definitely the case, not only in non-human primates; even in humans. There are possibilities of creating subjects that we don’t want on earth. These experiments may be culturally allowed in some places and rejected in others. Governance of how the datasets that emerge from such dangerous experiments can deter such research (P3).

      For another participant responding to whether it is ethical to use data from transgenic NHPs, conditions under which such data is generated should be carefully examined because; there are real concerns, especially as you get into transgenic models, to really consider whether or not you want to be seen endorsing a particular approach or not. That depends on where the researcher is working from (P4).

      To support this line of thought, another participant stated that: “…this is a serious ethical concern. The use of the data from research that is not allowed in this country (the participant’s country) can cause reputational damage. I have not thought about it this way but it can” (P10).

      Another argument presented by the participants was that the differences in ethical principles, values and regulations means that some researchers will move to other countries with less restrictive values and regulations or in some cases outsource the research. These are insights provided by the participants:

      We already know that happens. We knew it happened with stem cells, we knew it happened with anything else, “…can’t do it here, I will go and do it in that country, where there are no regulations.” Some researchers can do the research in other countries and then bring back the data to be used here. This is data laundering and it is fraud, right? You are just outsourcing it to somebody else and taking advantage of the data (P3).

      To be fairly honest, I think at some scale that’s already happening. There are people from Europe doing some kind of research in China for Example, which they would have a very difficult time to get easily approved in their own country. The same is true for the United States. So, in theory, yes, this opens the doors for that kind of stuff, but that’s not where we want to go (P1).

      Another participant observed that animal data governance can

      Prevent unnecessary animal research happening in Africa by researchers from Europe and America. So many of these do not follow the same ethical principles they are made to follow in their own countries (P11).

      These perspectives hint at an ethical concern often referred to as ethics dumping which will be explored further in sections below. It also means that while researchers in different regions of the world are required to comply with their regulations, some of these regulations may fall short of acceptable ethical standards in other regions.

      6.3. Data quality concerns

      Another reason the participants believe that it is time for animal data governance is to ensure that the quality of animal data shared is good for scientific purposes. The summary of the opinions here is that data governance can improve the quality of data because good scientific research relies upon high quality laboratory animal care (Friese, 2013, 2019). A harmonized animal data governance can help to improve animal welfare in a way that fewer confounding variables are introduced into research. Some of these opinions are as follows:

      But we also know that overly-stressed animals are bad subjects, you don’t get good data from them, you see mixed effects. So, I think you could make both the ethical and the scientific case that these animals need to be treated well. Governance can help harmonize best practices for animal welfare (P4).

      It will be a con-founding factor. If there are high stress levels on the animals, it will simply provide you with completely wrong measurements of whatever you are doing. Yes, that is what I think. Having some sort of universal principle for the derived data will improve the quality of data shared (P12).

      The quality of data is critical to this discussion. I think in science you want a certain degree of consistency and quality for effective research outcomes. Open data portals need to put mechanisms to ensure that the data they make available come from labs that comply with high standards of animal welfare (P9).

      These views suggest that animal data governance when implemented, particularly by open data repositories/archives, can help to improve the quality of animal data for research. However, without adequate governance mechanisms, low quality animal data may be allowed to permeate within research ecosystems.

      6.4. Approaches to governance

      Beyond providing insights into the reasons why it is time to consider animal data governance, the participants also gave their perspectives on how this can be done. One view that was shared by all the participants is that a harmonized governance framework for animal data requires inclusive discussions involving all relevant stakeholders. In order to appreciate and respect differences in regulations and social-cultural values, animal data governance is a multi-stakeholder endeavor. For instance, one view was that:

      Discussions on governance approaches should be thoughtful and careful and involve researchers that are performing these experiments. It should also involve people from different cultures to understand what is an appropriate framework (P12).

      This is a very important view that can ensure that one region does not dictate for other regions as regards best practices. As a participant mentioned; responsible data governance is an interesting concept but a complex one. Whose understanding of responsibility? Whose values are going to shape this? These are things that need further discussions and understanding (P5).

      Another participant also observed that exclusion or disregard of some cultural values and beliefs will make any developed mechanism unacceptable for many scientists. However, this does not mean that people working in regions with strict regulations should accept anything and everything.

      There was also the feeling that already established standards used for publishing in journals can be a starting point–something to build on.

      Minimum standards of acceptance and rejection for papers in neuroscience journals need to be studied and improved upon. For instance, the implementation of the 3Rs. Maybe a comparative literature about the different ethical standards used in different countries (P2).

      Another researcher further suggested that auditing all data producing sites can be a pragmatic way of understanding the status quo.

      One critical view that was shared by many of the participants is that research infrastructures or open data archives, repositories or portals need to play important roles in providing efficient governance mechanisms for animal data. The argument was that animal data should be governed through international research infrastructures where researchers and research data from different cultures meet (P10).

      Some participants made a case for the establishment of regulations for the use of animal data similar to data protection laws. These can be new regulations or amendments to existing laws to cover the use or application of animal data, particularly NHP data. An EU participant puts it thus:

      The rationale is, of course, that we wouldn’t include data that has been acquired in a way that doesn’t conform to the rules within the EU. That is important. Otherwise, it serves as an incentive, almost, for people to do something elsewhere and then hopefully get the data in (P7).

      7. Discussion and conclusion

      The findings from our empirical work are broadly consistent with our insights from the existing literature. The international neuroscientists we talked to agreed that animal neuroscience research raises ethical concerns and that these concerns have consequences for the way the resulting data can and should be used. The very brief answer to the question in the title to this article whether it is time to consider animal data in data governance is thus a “yes.”

      Our empirical work highlights that this is not a purely theoretical problem but that questions of animal data governance do arise in practical neuroscience work. The global discussion of data governance in health-related research ostensibly has the purpose of facilitating collaboration and exchange of data with a view to support the creation of new knowledge and the resulting consequences. This logic can be extended to animal data which calls for animal data governance.

      There remain, however, fundamental differences between human data and animal data. The very use of human data can raise ethical concerns, for example where patient record privacy is concerned. Animal data does not raise such intrinsic concerns. For animal data the core of most issues is the generation or collection of data. Data use is nevertheless important because a lack of attention to the use of data may facilitate the use of data which is deemed not to be ethically permissible in a particular jurisdiction or cultural context, thereby sidestepping agreed-upon ethical principles.

      A key question is therefore which type of animal research is deemed to be permissible and on what grounds can such value judgments be made. Our interviews showed that neuroscientists are aware of differences with regards to these questions. There seems to be a continuum of ethical severity which starts with cell cultures, moves up via invertebrates, vertebrates, rodents, NHPs and may find its current summit in research on transgenic NHPs. The problem is that the evaluation of these different types of research differ between cultures and jurisdictions and there is no universally agreed position on these questions.

      This lack of agreement points to the lively exchange of ideas between cultures which is probably a good thing. Ethics is a topic that often finds its expressions in dilemmas and disagreements, so the plurality of views is not surprising. It seems plausible that an ethical free-for-all is not desirable, neither in animal research, nor in research more generally. At the same time, a uniform ethical position would suppress legitimate positions and thus be likely unethical in itself. Ethical plurality is thus welcome and can stimulate academic debate, for example in the field of neuroethics, where questions are continually triggered by new technologies and methods. While we thus welcome ethical plurality on animal research, it raises practical questions with regards to what data can and should be used for which purposes.

      This leaves us with the question of how ethical pluralism can be accommodated in data governance. One plausible response to this question could come from procedural approaches to ethics. What this means is that we should not expect to find agreement on the substance of complex ethical questions, but it may be possible to define procedures that support fruitful engagement on these questions. One can argue that most modern western approaches to philosophical ethics follow such a procedural approach. Elsewhere we have suggested that Discourse Ethics may provide a suitable avenue to pursue (Stahl et al., 2019). We believe, without having the space to make this argument in detail, that such a procedural approach could be applied to the problem of animal data governance.

      In practice this would mean that data governance should be designed in a way to facilitate constructive debate about ethical issues and be suitable for supporting ethical consensus, where it exists. This implies that data governance should be used to highlight ethically contentious aspects of animal data. This means that the meta-data of neuroscientific animal research data should clearly show those aspects that we know to be ethically contentious. This would include the species, research question, type of intervention, whether transgenic animals are involved etc.

      The result of such an approach to animal data would be that it would be easy to understand ethical agreements and disagreements. If, for example, a culture has a consensus view that in vivo experiments with NHPs or the use of transgenic animals is not ethically acceptable, then filtering out such data would be easy. Probably more importantly, a strong metadata schema would allow highlighting which aspects of the research and the resulting data may be contentious and thus foster communication around the reasons for disagreements and possible ways to shape research and data in ways that are more broadly deemed to be acceptable.

      This proposal is of course not overwhelmingly novel. Data governance structures already routinely capture some of these items. As our interviews have shown, researchers see data quality as an (ethical) issue and good metadata is recognized as a means to increase transparency of data and to promote the FAIR principles. The novelty of our proposal is that ethics-related aspects of the metadata could be explicitly defined and collected. While many of these will already form part of data governance, a next step would be to more clearly define them in ways that support researchers who generate the scientific data in the first place and ensure that they are aware of relevant metadata requirements.

      This proposal is of course no panacea. There are limitations of our research such as the limited number of respondents and a lack of statistical representativeness of our approach. While we believe that our methodological choices have ensured that we received relevant input, we cannot claim to have represented all possible angles and identified all ethical issues. This article should thus be read as an exploratory study that has confirmed that ethically informed animal data governance is called for. The real work of shaping such a data governance approach will have to follow as a large-scale consultative exercise leading to the co-creation of animal data governance that truly captures ethical issues. This future research can include how these findings individually can or do shape animal data governance.

      It is furthermore worth underlining that the existence of ethically sensitive data governance will not make the underlying ethical issues go away. Many of these issues touch on deep convictions of who we are and what we as humans can or should do. Such convictions do not change quickly and different views will remain. But, as indicated earlier, ethical pluralism does not need to be seen as a problem but can be celebrated as an expression of human diversity. What counts is that we find productive ways of dealing with issues. Ethically informed animal data governance can be one mechanism that allows us as researchers, as citizens of different countries, holders of different convictions to come together and have productive conversations about how to understand and deal with our different worldviews. If it achieves this, then this will not only strengthen neuroscience with all its concomitant benefits but also show how science can play an important role in tackling the broader ethical and social questions that our increasingly globalized world faces.

      Data availability statement

      The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

      Ethics statement

      Ethics approval was obtained for this research from De Montfort University Ethics Review Committee. Information sheet and an informed consent form were then emailed to participants.

      Author contributions

      DE: conceptualization, writing, data analysis, original draft/review, and editing. GO: conceptualization, writing, and data collection. WK: conceptualization and writing. BS: conceptualization, writing, and funding. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

      Funding

      This work was supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Programme for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreements No. 720270 (HBP SGA1), No. 785907 (HBP SGA2), and No. 945539 (HBP SGA3).

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      https://gdpr-info.eu/

      https://www.hhs.gov/hipaa/index.html

      https://www.priv.gc.ca/en/privacy-topics/privacy-laws-in-canada/the-personal-information-protection-and-electronic-documents-act-pipeda/

      References Bailey J. Taylor K. (2016). Non-human primates in neuroscience research: The case against its scientific necessity. Altern. Lab. Anim. 44 4369. 10.1177/026119291604400101 27031602 Bjugstad K. B. Sladek J. R. (2006). “Neural transplantation in the nonhuman primate model of Parkinson’s disease,” in Cell therapy, stem cells, and brain repair, eds Sanberg C. D. Sanberg P. R. (Berlin: Springer), 6182. 10.1007/978-1-59745-147-5_3 Bystron I. Rakic P. Molnár Z. Blakemore C. (2006). The first neurons of the human cerebral cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9 880886. 10.1038/nn1726 16783367 Capitanio J. P. Emborg M. E. (2008). Contributions of non-human primates to neuroscience research. Lancet 371 11261135. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60489-4 18374844 Carvalho C. Gaspar A. Knight A. Vicente L. (2018). Ethical and scientific pitfalls concerning laboratory research with non-human primates, and possible solutions. Animals 9:12. 10.3390/ani9010012 30597951 Chan A. W. (2014). “Production of transgenic nonhuman primates,” in Transgenic animal technology, ed. Pinkert C. A. (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 359385. 10.1016/B978-0-12-410490-7.00014-1 Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium (2005). Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome. Nature 437 6987. 10.1038/nature04072 16136131 Conlee K. M. Rowan A. N. (2012). The case for phasing out experiments on primates. Hastings Center Rep. 42 S31S34. 10.1002/hast.106 23138428 Cyranoski D. (2016). Monkey kingdom. Nature 532 300302. 10.1038/532300a 27111614 Disotell T. R. Tosi A. J. (2007). The monkey’s perspective. Genome biol. 8 14. 10.1186/gb-2007-8-9-226 17903312 Eke D. Ochang P. Ogundele T. Adimula A. Borokini F. Akintoye S. (2022). Responsible Data Governance in Africa: Institutional gaps and capacity needs. Abuja: Centre for the Study of African Economies (CSEA). Eke D. O. Bernard A. Bjaalie J. Chavarriaga R. Hanakawa T. Hannan A. J. (2022). International data governance for neuroscience. Neuron 110 600612. 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.11.017 34914921 European Commission (2019). EU statistical reports on the use of animals for scientific purposes. Brussels: European Commission. Feng G. Jensen F. Greely H. Okano H. Treue S. Roberts A. (2020). Opportunities and limitations of genetically modified nonhuman primate models for neuroscience research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117 2402224031. 10.1073/pnas.2006515117 32817435 Fothergill B. T. Fothergill B. Knight W. Stahl B. Ulnicane I. (2019). Responsible data governance of neuroscience big data. Front. Neuroinform. 13:28. 10.3389/fninf.2019.00028 31110477 Friedman H. Ator N. Haigwood N. Newsome W. Allan J. Golos T. (2017). The critical role of nonhuman primates in medical research. Pathogens Immunity 2 352365. 10.20411/pai.v2i3.186 29034361 Friese C. (2013). Realizing potential in translational medicine: The uncanny emergence of care as science. Curr. Anthropol. 54, S129S138. Friese C. (2019). Intimate entanglements in the animal house: Caring for and about mice. Sociol. Rev. 67, 287298. Garcia A. McGlone J. J. (2022). Animal welfare and the acknowledgment of cultural differences. Animals 12:474. Garner J. P. (2014). The significance of meaning: Why do over 90% of behavioral neuroscience results fail to translate to humans, and what can we do to fix it? ILAR J. 55 438456. 10.1093/ilar/ilu047 25541546 Guest G. Bunce A. Johnson L. (2006). How many interviews are enough?: An experiment with data saturation and variability. Field Methods 18 5982. 10.1177/1525822X05279903 Guhad F. (2005). Introduction to the 3Rs (Refinement, Reduction and Replacement). J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 44 5859. Haesemeyer M. Schier A. F. (2015). The study of psychiatric disease genes and drugs in zebrafish. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 30 122130. 10.1016/j.conb.2014.12.002 25523356 Jones B. (2021). Feature: Why do we need to use animals in neuroscience research? Belek: EARA. Kaiser T. Feng G. (2015). Modeling psychiatric disorders for developing effective treatments. Nat. Med. 21 979988. 10.1038/nm.3935 26340119 Knight A. (2007). The poor contribution of chimpanzee experiments to biomedical progress. J. Appl. Anim. Welfare Sci. 10 281308. 10.1080/10888700701555501 17970631 Lankau E. W. Turner P. Mullan R. Galland G. (2014). Use of nonhuman primates in research in North America. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 53 278282. Li W.-H. Saunders M. A. (2005). The chimpanzee and us. Nature 437 5051. Masiga W. N. Munyua S. J. M. (2005). Global perspectives on animal welfare: Africa. Rev. Sci. Tech. Off. Int. Des Épiz. 24:579. Mepham T. B. Combes R. Balls M. Barbieri O. Blokhuis H. Costa P. (1998). The use of transgenic animals in the European Union: The report and recommendations of ECVAM workshop 28. Altern. Lab. Anim. 26 2143. 10.1177/026119299802600108 11656813 Milham M. Petkov C. I. Margulies D. S. Schroeder C. E. Basso M. A. Belin P. (2020). Accelerating the evolution of nonhuman primate neuroimaging. Neuron 105 600603. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.12.023 32078795 Mitchell A. S. Hartig R. Basso M. Jarrett W. Kastner S. Poirier C. (2021). International primate neuroscience research regulation, public engagement and transparency opportunities. Neuroimage 229:117700. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117700 33418072 Neuhaus C. P. (2018). Ethical issues when modelling brain disorders in non-human primates. J. Med. Ethics 44 323327. 10.1136/medethics-2016-104088 28801311 Ochang P. Stahl B. C. Eke D. (2022). The ethical and legal landscape of brain data governance. PLoS One 17:e0273473. 10.1371/journal.pone.0273473 36580464 Okano H. Sasaki E. Yamamori T. Iriki A. Shimogori T. Yamaguchi Y. (2016). Brain/MINDS: A Japanese national brain project for marmoset neuroscience. Neuron 92 582590. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.018 27809998 Poldrack R. A. Gorgolewski K. J. (2014). Making big data open: Data sharing in neuroimaging. Nat. Neurosci. 17 15101517. 10.1038/nn.3818 25349916 Poo M. Du J. Ip N. Xiong Z. Xu B. Tan T. (2016). China brain project: Basic neuroscience, brain diseases, and brain-inspired computing. Neuron 92 591596. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.050 27809999 Preuss T. M. (2010). Reinventing Primate Neuroscience for the Twenty-First Century. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195326598.003.0022 36389024 Qiu Z. Li X. (2017). Non-human primate models for brain disorders – Towards genetic manipulations via innovative technology. Neurosci. Bull. 33 247250. 10.1007/s12264-017-0115-4 28251519 Ragin C. C. Amoroso L. M. (2011). Constructing social research: The unity and diversity of method. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press. Rommelfanger K. S. Jeong S. Ema A. Fukushi T. Kasai K. Ramos K. (2018). Neuroethics questions to guide ethical research in the international brain initiatives. Neuron 100 1936. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.021 30308169 Russell W. M. S. Burch R. L. (1960). The principles of humane experimental technique. Med. J. Austr. 1 500500. 10.5694/j.1326-5377.1960.tb73127.x 32297660 Scientific Committee on Health, Environmental and Emerging Risks [SCHEER] (2017). Final opinion on “The need for non-human primates in biomedical research, production and testing of products and devises”. Saarbrücken: SCHEER. Shi L. Luo X. Jiang J. Chen Y. Liu C. Hu T. (2019). Transgenic rhesus monkeys carrying the human MCPH1 gene copies show human-like neoteny of brain development. Natl. Sci. Rev. 6 480493. 10.1093/nsr/nwz043 34691896 Sikela J. M. (2006). The jewels of our genome: The search for the genomic changes underlying the evolutionarily unique capacities of the human brain. PLoS Genet. 2:e80. 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020080 16733552 Stahl B. C. Akintoye S. Fothergill B. Guerrero M. Knight W. Ulnicane I. (2019). Beyond research ethics: Dialogues in Neuro-ICT research. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:105. 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00105 30983981 Stahl B. C. Rainey S. Harris E. Fothergill B. T. (2018). The role of ethics in data governance of large neuro-ICT projects. J. Am. Med. Inform. Assoc. 25, 10991107. 10.1093/jamia/ocy040 29767726 Szucs E. Geers R. Jezierski T. Sossidou E. N. Broom D. M. (2012). Animal welfare in different human cultures, traditions and religious faiths. Asian Aust. J. Anim. Sci. 25, 14991506. Tardif S. D. Coleman K. Hobbs T. Lutz C. (2013). IACUC Review of Nonhuman Primate Research. ILAR J. 54 234245. 10.1093/ilar/ilt040 24174445 Ting J. T. Feng G. (2013). Development of transgenic animals for optogenetic manipulation of mammalian nervous system function: Progress and prospects for behavioral neuroscience. Behav. Brain Res. 255 318. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.037 23473879 Tomioka I. Nogami N. Nakatani T. Owari K. Fujita N. Motohashi H. (2017). Generation of transgenic marmosets using a tetracyclin-inducible transgene expression system as a neurodegenerative disease model. Biol. Reprod. 97 772780. 10.1093/biolre/iox129 29045563 Varki A. (2000). A chimpanzee genome project is a biomedical imperative. Genome Res. 10 10651070. 10.1101/gr.10.8.1065 10958623 Vasbinder M. A. Locke P. (2016). Introduction: Global Laws, Regulations, and Standards for Animals in Research. ILAR J. 57 261265. 10.1093/ilar/ilw039 29117408 Weatheall D. (2015). The use of non-human primates in research: A working group report. London: Royal Society. Windisch M. (2014). We can treat Alzheimer’s disease successfully in mice but not in men: Failure in translation? A perspective. Neuro-Degener. Dis. 13 147150. 10.1159/000357568 24401335 Yeo H.-G. Lee Y. Jeon C. Jeong K. Jin Y. Kang P. (2015). Characterization of cerebral damage in a monkey model of Alzheimer’s disease induced by intracerebroventricular injection of streptozotocin. J. Alzheimers Dis. 46 9891005. 10.3233/JAD-143222 25881906 Yun H.-M. Choi D. Oh K. Hong J. (2015). PRDX6 exacerbates dopaminergic neurodegeneration in a MPTP mouse model of Parkinson’s disease. Mol. Neurobiol. 52 422431. 10.1007/s12035-014-8885-4 25193021 Zhao H. Jiang Y.-H. Zhang Y. Q. (2018). Modeling autism in non-human primates: Opportunities and challenges. Autism Res. 11 686694. 10.1002/aur.1945 29573234 Zweier C. de Jong E. Zweier M. Orrico A. Ousager L. Collins A. (2009). CNTNAP2 and NRXN1 are mutated in autosomal-recessive Pitt-Hopkins-like mental retardation and determine the level of a common synaptic protein in Drosophila. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 85 655666. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.10.004 19896112
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016gqnxjtx.com.cn
      lianme.com.cn
      fzzuro.com.cn
      langqinuo.com.cn
      www.qdtqnc.com.cn
      njfi.com.cn
      syzsgl.org.cn
      teasm.com.cn
      pyqdl.com.cn
      www.qesocb.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p