Front. Mar. Sci. Frontiers in Marine Science Front. Mar. Sci. 2296-7745 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fmars.2022.897298 Marine Science Original Research North Pacific minke whales call rapidly when calling conspecifics are nearby Martin Cameron R. 1 * Guazzo Regina A. 1 Helble Tyler A. 1 Alongi Gabriela C. 2 Durbach Ian N. 3 Martin Stephen W. 2 Matsuyama Brian M. 2 Henderson E. Elizabeth 1 1 Naval Information Warfare Center Pacific, San Diego, CA, United States 2 National Marine Mammal Foundation, San Diego, CA, United States 3 Centre for Research into Ecological and Environmental Modelling, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, United Kingdom

Edited by: Ana M. M. Sequeira, University of Western Australia, Australia

Reviewed by: Denise Risch, Scottish Association For Marine Science, United Kingdom; David Moretti, Naval Undersea Warfare Center (NUWC), United States

*Correspondence: Cameron R. Martin, cameron.r.martin9.civ@us.navy.mil

This article was submitted to Marine Megafauna, a section of the journal Frontiers in Marine Science

22 08 2022 2022 9 897298 16 03 2022 28 07 2022 Copyright © 2022 Martin, Guazzo, Helble, Alongi, Durbach, Martin, Matsuyama and Henderson 2022 Martin, Guazzo, Helble, Alongi, Durbach, Martin, Matsuyama and Henderson

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

North Pacific minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) boing calls are commonly detected in Hawaiian waters. When producing boing vocalizations, minke whales seem to be in one of two calling behavioral states. Most often minke whales produce boings with inter-call intervals of several minutes, but sometimes minke whales call rapidly with inter-call intervals of less than a minute. Since minke whales are difficult to detect visually, cue-rate-based density estimation using passive acoustic monitoring has been proposed. However, the variables that influence cue rate or calling rate are poorly understood in most whales, including minke whales. We collected passive acoustic recordings from 47 bottom-mounted hydrophones at the Pacific Missile Range Facility’s instrumented range off the coast of Kauaʻi, Hawaiʻi to test the hypothesis that minke whales call more rapidly when closer in proximity to other calling conspecifics. A total of 599 days of data were recorded between August 2012 and July 2017 and were automatically post-processed to detect, classify, and localize calls. Localized calls were grouped into tracks and manually validated, resulting in 509 individual tracks composed of 36,033 calls within a 16 x 39 km focal study area. Tracked minke whales exhibited a strong bimodal call rate with means of one call every 6.85 min (σ= 2.54 min) and 0.63 min (σ= 0.36 min). We ran hidden Markov models to quantify the relationship between call rate and the distance to the nearest calling conspecific. Overall, the probability of the higher call rate occurring increased as the distance to the nearest conspecific decreased, and the probability of the lower call rate occurring increased as the distance to the nearest conspecific increased. We also examined individual track data and found that minke whales may also exhibit other responses (i.e. increased speed, changes in heading, and cessation of calling) when calling conspecifics are nearby. These findings provide new information about minke whale calling behavior in what is likely a breeding area.

minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) inter-call interval (ICI) calling rate cue rate bioacoustics passive acoustic monitoring animal behavior marine ecology

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      1 Introduction

      North Pacific minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) boing calls are commonly detected in Hawaiian waters from fall to spring, but the animals that produce them have rarely been sighted and little is known about their behavior in this area. The minke whale boing call was first detailed by Wenz (1964) using recordings from submarines operating off the coast of San Diego, California and in Hawaiian waters and also from two bottom-mounted hydrophones off the coast of Kāneʻohe, Hawaiʻi. Although it was suspected that the boing was produced by a whale species (Thompson and Friedl, 1982), it took about 40 years until the source of the boing was determined to be from a minke whale (Rankin and Barlow, 2005). Minke whales are difficult to detect visually, so passive acoustic monitoring (PAM) is an important methodology for learning about the behavior and life history of this species.

      Thompson and Friedl (1982) recorded boing sounds from two bottom-mounted hydrophones off the coast of Oʻahu. They observed two calling states with distinct inter-call intervals (ICIs). They proposed that the whales produced calls with ICIs of approximately 30 s when in acoustic contact with another calling whale, and 6 min when not in acoustic contact with another calling whale, but this study was limited by a low sample size and the inability to localize calls and measure distances between individuals (Thompson and Friedl, 1982). The significance of the boing call is currently unknown. However, since humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in Hawaiian waters use song for breeding purposes (Tyack, 1981) and have a similar seasonal and spatial overlap with minke whales, past researchers have suggested that the minke whale boing is also a mating call (Oswald et al., 2011; Martin et al., 2015), but this hypothesis is yet to be tested. Learning more about the calling rate of whales in different contexts can help us better understand the function of calls in whale behavior.

      The central North Pacific boing call produced by minke whales in Hawaiian waters (as opposed to the eastern North Pacific boing call, which has a slightly different pulse repetition rate and call duration) has an onset transient component followed by a long call (mean duration of 2.6 sec) with both frequency and amplitude modulation (Rankin and Barlow, 2005). The 1,350 to 1,440 Hz band contains the majority of energy from the boing call with 115 Hz pulse repetition rate harmonic bands (Wenz, 1964; Rankin and Barlow, 2005; Martin et al., 2015). The peak frequency in this band has been termed the dominant signal component (DSC) (Martin, 2009), and has been identified as a feature to attribute calls to individual whales in some situations (Martin et al., 2013).

      The life history of minke whales that spend time in Hawaiian waters is unknown, but acoustic detections and visual sightings in other areas can give clues about their migratory patterns. In the Chukchi Sea, Delarue et al. (2012) detected a small number of central North Pacific boings in the early fall (n=44), and boings resembling the eastern North Pacific type in the late summer and fall (n=10). In the North Pacific, pulse trains and downsweep calls have been documented in the presence of minke whales during the summer in Cormorant Channel, British Columbia, Canada which is likely a feeding area (Nikolich and Towers, 2020). Minke whales sighted off Vancouver Island and central British Columbia often have scars characteristic of cookiecutter sharks (Isistius brasiliensis) which are only in tropical and subtropical waters (Towers et al., 2013). Based on these observations and acoustic detections, the minke whales detected in Hawaiian waters probably migrate north to arctic or subarctic waters to feed, similar to the migration patterns of other large baleen whales.

      In addition to helping us understand whale behavior, calling rate, or cue rate, is a necessary variable for PAM cue-rate-based density estimation (Marques et al., 2009). Visual surveys are currently the primary method to estimate marine mammal abundance, but minke whales are difficult to see due to their small size, minimal surface cues, and tendency to be alone, and so it is very challenging to estimate their abundance precisely (Zerbini et al., 2006). In addition, the visual ship-based surveys for marine mammals in Hawaiian waters are usually conducted during summer and fall, and since minke whales are rarely visually or acoustically detected during this time, no total abundance estimate exists for minke whales in this region (Carretta et al., 2014). Quantifying the minke whale cue rate and the variables that influence it may make it possible to estimate the abundance of minke whales using PAM for applications when tracking individuals is not possible.

      In this paper, we investigate the hypothesis proposed by Thompson and Friedl (1982) that minke whales call more rapidly when closer to other calling minke whales. We recorded and localized boing calls off of Kauaʻi, Hawaiʻi between fall and spring for 5 years, and spatio-temporally grouped these localized calls into acoustic tracks. Using these tracks, we quantified individual minke whale ICIs and modeled ICI as a function of distance to the nearest tracked calling conspecific. This paper summarizes the calling behavior of minke whales using 36,033 boing calls from 509 tracks.

      2 Methods 2.1 Study area and data description

      The Pacific Missile Range Facility (PMRF) is a U.S. Navy training and testing area northwest of the island of Kauaʻi, Hawaiʻi. For this analysis, we analyzed recordings from 47 bottom-mounted hydrophones spread over a grid approximately 20 km to the east-west and 58 km to the north-south ( Figure 1 ) to detect, classify, and localize minke whale boing calls ( Figure 2 ) from data collected between August 2012 and July 2017. This period from 2012 to 2017 was chosen because the broadband hydrophone array configuration was consistent during this time. As detailed by Martin et al. (2015), the depths of the hydrophones ranged from 650 m to 4,700 m and had a suitable frequency response to detect calls from minke whales. A custom personal computer-based recorder described by Martin et al. (2013) and updated with a second 32-channel analog-to-digital converter was used to record a total of 62 hydrophones and two inter-range instrumentation group time code channels. Full bandwidth data were collected at a 96 kHz sample rate with 16-bit samples. Starting in August 2014, the collection of additional long-term recordings at a 6 kHz sample rate increased the recording effort to monitor low-frequency whale calls, including the minke whale boing call. Recordings at the 96 kHz sample rate typically had maximum durations of 1.9 days of continuous data, while recordings at the 6 kHz sample rate had maximum durations of 10.9 days. The recording limit was a function of sample rate, bit depth, the number of channels recorded, and a maximum hard disk drive capacity of 2 terabytes supported by the recorder. No recordings during known U.S. Navy training or testing activities were included in the analysis. This work only analyzed tracks from unclassified recordings. Of the focal tracks analyzed, two started within 24 hours after U.S. Navy training and testing activities ended. Based on previous work by Durbach et al. (2021), between 24 hours and 4 days after the last sonar transmission, the kinematic behavior of minke whales on PMRF is similar to baseline behavior.

      Map of the approximate locations of the 47 hydrophones used in this analysis (white circles) at the Pacific Missile Range Facility (PMRF) off Kauaʻi, Hawaiʻi, as shown by the red box in the inset map. The focal study area of the array extends approximately 16 km to the east-west and 39 km to the north-south (boundaries shown by a solid white box). The 15 km search area outside the focal area is shown with a dashed white box. The depth contours are every 1,000 m (1 km spatial resolution, Hawaii Mapping Research Group, The School of Ocean and Earth Science and Technology, the University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa, http://www.soest.hawaii.edu/hmrg/multibeam/bathymetry.php).

      Example minke whale boing spectrogram (upper) and time series (lower) recorded at the Pacific Missile Range Facility (PMRF). Color in the spectrogram represents received sound pressure level in dB re 1µPa.

      In order to investigate the impact of conspecifics on minke whale calling rate, we had to ensure that if a minke whale was vocalizing within a certain distance from another whale, both whales would be detected, localized, and tracked in all expected noise conditions. Based on the geometry of the PMRF array and the probability of detection of minke whale calls as a function of location, we designated an inner rectangular focal area where, if a whale called from a distance of 15 km or less from the focal area boundaries, we would be able to detect, localize, and track it. In other words, we could detect nearest calling conspecifics for whales tracked within the focal area out to 15 km. The focal area extended approximately 16 km to the east-west and 39 km to the north-south (indicated by the solid white box in Figure 1 ) with approximate latitude boundaries of 22.38°N and 22.74°N, and longitude boundaries of 160.02°W and 159.87°W. The 15 km search area was smaller than that defined by Harris et al. (2019) which conservatively encompassed the area where minke whale calls could be accurately detected, localized, and tracked based on expected noise conditions, source levels, and transmission properties of the boing call.

      2.2 Post-processing and validation

      The automated minke whale boing call detection and classification algorithms developed to process recordings collected at the PMRF instrumented range have been previously described (Mellinger et al., 2011; Martin et al., 2013; Martin et al., 2015) and are briefly reiterated here. The central North Pacific minke whale boing call detection and classification process operated in multiple stages. The first stage detected the near-continuous wave portion of the call by measuring if the energy in the detection band (1,320–1,450 Hz) exceeded a long-term spectral average noise estimate in the detection band for more than 0.8 sec. The second stage computed a spectral correlation to locate the onset time of the frequency modulated portion of call, and was used as the measured time of arrival (TOA) in the model-based localizer (discussed in the next paragraph). The third stage reprocessed the raw time series data with a longer fast Fourier transform (FFT) length to produce an FFT bin resolution of less than 1 Hz. The average power in the 1,350–1,440 Hz range over the duration of the signal was calculated and compared to the peak power in the same frequency and time range. The resulting value was termed a quasi-signal-to-noise ratio (QSNR) since the average power in the 1,350–1,440 Hz range also included the peak signal. If the QSNR exceeded a user-defined value of 2.0, the signal was classified as a boing call detection, and a high resolution measurement of the DSC was made. To our knowledge, the central North Pacific boing call is the only call associated with minke whales that has been recorded on PMRF.

      The model-based localization algorithm for data processing was previously described by Martin et al. (2015) and is briefly described here. To localize calls, we compared the measured time difference of arrivals (TDOAs) of each call across the hydrophones with the modeled TDOAs. The onset times for automatic detections on multiple hydrophones were the measured TOAs, and were subtracted from each other to calculate measured TDOAs. Modeled TDOAs were calculated from theoretical source locations. An iterative spatial gridding process minimized the weighted least square error between measured and modeled TDOAs to spatially locate a call. The least square error was weighted by order of TOA with earlier arrivals weighted more than later arrivals, and was also normalized by the number of hydrophones in the localization solution. Only localizations with a weighted least square error value between measured and modeled TDOAs of less than or equal to 0.075 sec were used in this analysis. In addition, the DSCs of the first four detections for a candidate localization were required to be within 5 Hz to initiate grouping of detections. Currently, ground-truth whale positions to determine localization accuracy do not exist. However, Martin et al. (2015) noted that surface ship hull-mounted mid-frequency active sonar transmissions within the hydrophone array at PMRF that were localized with the same algorithm were typically within 50 m of ship positional data. The accuracy with which the start of a signal is detected influences the accuracy of localization. Despite mid-frequency active sonar transmissions being a higher frequency than minke whale boings, the start of both signals have an onset transient with a sharp rise time that is detected well.

      An automatic tracking algorithm (previously described by Klay et al. (2015) and used by Harris et al. (2019); Helble et al. (2020b); Durbach et al. (2021) to track minke whales) spatially and temporally grouped localized calls into individual tracks by recursively examining distance and time between successive candidate localizations. The following user-defined values were tuned for tracking minke whales: a track had to be composed of 12 or more localizations, a localization solution required automatic detections from 8 or more hydrophones, the distance between localizations had to be less than or equal to 0.06 decimal degrees in both latitude and longitude (approximately 6.7 km), and the maximum time allowed between any two localizations before terminating a track was set to 40 min. Gaps between recordings could vary from minutes to weeks. In this study, we did not group calls into tracks across individual recordings.

      For every localized call, we searched for the closest localized call from another track within the past 10, 20, and 30 min. If at least one other vocalizing minke whale was present during that time window and within the 15 km search area from the focal study area, we calculated the straight-line horizontal distance between the focal track’s call and the neighboring track’s calls. The distance to the nearest calling conspecific or “nearest neighbor” was the distance to the closest localized call from another track during that time window. For statistical analysis, nearest neighbor distances were assigned to categories of 0–5 km, 5–10 km, 10–15 km, or 15+ km. If there were no other tracks within the search area and time window, then the nearest neighbor for the focal track was assigned to the 15+ km category given that there were additional whales beyond 15 km. Grouping nearest neighbor distances into categories allowed us to keep observations from all tracks, including those that did not have a detectable nearest neighbor, without assuming an arbitrary large value for its nearest neighbor distance. The 5 km distance bins were small enough to see changes in calling behavior across distances, but large enough to have large sample sizes and small confidence intervals for each category. We only searched for nearest neighbors within the same recording as that containing the focal track. We tested multiple nearest neighbor time windows (10, 20, and 30 min) to account for variability in call timing and location (e.g. if an animal skipped calls or if some calls from the nearest neighbor were outside of the study area). Since the maximum time between subsequent calls before terminating a track was 40 min, we did not test window sizes larger than 30 min to avoid potentially declaring a whale as its own nearest neighbor.

      We manually validated all tracks to ensure that a track was generated by an individual whale since spurious tracks could be generated under certain conditions. Specifically, a single track could be classified as two or more tracks in close proximity to each other when the ICI decreased or the number of co-occurring tracks in the study area increased. During the validation process, it was important to inspect and rectify potential duplicate tracks by removing or combining them since they could produce artificial minimum distances between conspecifics (i.e. a whale could be its own nearest neighbor). An interactive program was used to systematically examine the movement, DSC, and ICI of tracks in close proximity to each other. We combined tracks if the overall track path appeared to be a single animal which was evidenced by a relatively stable DSC and a call rate distribution that fell within the expected bimodal call rate distribution. Alternatively, we removed a track if it did not satisfy these criteria and appeared to be produced by multiple animals. No tracks were split since parsing calls from an individual track to multiple individuals is difficult and subjective without other data from tags or visual sightings. The validation process reduced the total number of focal tracks by 15.7%. Using unmodified tracks has the advantage of adding no subjectivity from the analyst, but a fraction of these automatic tracks are likely spurious or split from the same animal. To test the sensitivity of the results to the validation process and ensure call rate results were not biased by combining or removing tracks, both validated and unvalidated tracks were run through the same statistical analyses and the overall trend of the results were similar. From this analysis we determined that any subjectivity introduced from the validation process was minimal and using validated tracks for the statistical analyses provided the most accurate information on minke whale calling behavior and distances to conspecifics.

      In addition to validating how calls were grouped into tracks, we also quantified the performance of the entire detection, classification, localization, and tracking process in a way similar to Helble et al. (2020a). We randomly selected 5% of all tracks from the full range of seasons and years. The corresponding raw acoustic data during the times of these tracks from four hydrophones centered and evenly spaced in the study area were imported into Raven Pro (Center for Conservation Bioacoustics, 2019) to generate spectrograms. The TOAs for tracked calls were also imported and colored boxes were overlaid on the signals in the spectrogram, with each color associated with an individual whale track. Sequential calls from the same individual had similar delay patterns across the four hydrophones. If an unmarked call recorded on the four channels had the same delay pattern as other calls along a known track, the call was counted as a missed localization. A missed localization means a call along a track should have been detected, localized, and tracked, but was missed or excluded in one of the automated stages. If detections coincidentally arrived at the correct delay patterns to warrant a localization but did not appear to be a minke boing or if boings were detected but suspected to be from multiple minke sources (based on the signal characteristics and/or source level), they were marked as false localizations. Any missed or false localizations were noted from each of these tracks. In reality, there was no evidence that false localizations occurred along a track, which is not surprising because calls from different whales would need to arrive in near-perfect delay sequences on 8 or more hydrophones to be considered a valid localization. Missed localizations did occasionally occur, and so a “stress test” dataset was created to determine how sensitive subsequent analyses were to missed localizations. To test this sensitivity, 30% of all calls along each track were randomly deleted, and this decimated dataset was also used to see if any changes in the calling behavior statistical analysis could be mistakenly skewed by any missed localizations. In practice, the average missed localization rate was nearly an order of magnitude smaller than 30%, and so the stress test marked a worse-case scenario for any missed localizations to skew the behavioral statistical analysis.

      2.3 Changes in calling behavior statistical analysis

      We used hidden Markov models (HMMs) to quantify the relationship between calling rate and distance to the closest calling conspecific. For each localized call, we measured the time difference between this call and the next call in the track and saved this value as the ICI. Only tracks or segments of tracks that were within the focal area were modeled in the HMMs. For tracks with multiple segments within the focal area, we only kept the longest segment. This segment was required to have at least 4 localizations within the focal area.

      Since minke whales in Hawaiʻi have two calling states, a rapid and nominal calling state, and we hypothesized that the likelihood of being in each is affected by the distance to the closest calling conspecific, HMMs were appropriate for modeling. We modeled the minke whale ICI as HMMs that were functions of distance to the nearest calling neighbor. The ICI was modeled as a gamma distribution with initial values of 5 min for State 1 (representing the nominal calling rate) and 0.5 min for State 2 (representing the rapid calling rate) with standard deviations of 1 min for both. These initial values were chosen based on the observed distribution of data and the calling rates reported by Thompson and Friedl (1982), but the results presented were not sensitive to these initial values as initial values ±25% were tested and the best models did not change. We used the Akaike information criterion (AIC) (Akaike, 1974) to determine which nearest neighbor window size was best (10 min, 20 min, or 30 min). Once the best model was selected, we used the Viterbi algorithm to group each localized call into one of the two calling states (Langrock et al., 2012; McClintock and Michelot, 2018). To calculate the amount of time spent in each state, we added up the ICIs for the calls assigned to each state. HMM analyses for this study were conducted in R (v 4.0.3, R Core Team, 2020) using the momentuHMM package (McClintock and Michelot, 2018).

      3 Results

      Between August 2012 and July 2017, 599 days of data were recorded on each hydrophone. During this time, 2,245 individual minke whale tracks were observed and contained 223,732 calls. Of these tracks, 509 were located within the focal area at least part of the time and so were included as “focal tracks”. The subset of focal tracks contained 36,033 localized calls. Although we recorded throughout the year, minke whale boing vocalizations were only present between October and May ( Figure 3 ). The increase in recording effort from collecting long-term data at the 6 kHz sample rate (initiated in August 2014) is evident from the lower cyan bars in Figure 3 starting in September 2014.

      The number of localized minke whale calls that were tracked within the focal area per month (upper blue bars), and hours of recording effort per month (lower cyan bars) shown as a function of time from August 2012–July 2017.

      Manual review of 5% of the 509 focal tracks (26 tracks) resulted in a mean missed localization rate of 4.6%. We noted that missed calls were almost always detected on 8 or more hydrophones, but were sometimes discarded by the automated localization process because the least square error value exceeded the user-defined threshold. No false localizations were noted in the manual review process. Relaxing the least square error threshold would result in fewer missed localizations, but would come at the expense of false localizations that can confuse the tracking algorithm. The 4.6% missed localization rate was acceptable and did not affect the outcome of determining the behavioral state, as demonstrated by the stress test dataset (details presented with the modeling results).

      Minke whale boing ICIs followed a bimodal distribution (gray bars, Figure 4 ). Two calling states based on ICI were identified by the HMM and their probability density functions followed the observed ICI distribution well (blue and teal curves, Figure 4 ). Whales in State 1 had an mean ICI of approximately 6.85 min and whales in State 2 had an mean ICI of approximately 0.63 min ( Table 1 ).

      Distribution of observed minke whale boing inter-call intervals from 509 focal tracks at PMRF. The gray histogram shows the observed values normalized by the area of each bar, the blue curve shows the probability density function of State 1, and the teal curve shows the probability density function of State 2. These calling states were estimated using a hidden Markov model with distances grouped into 5 km bins and nearest neighbor determined using a 30 min window.

      Inter-call interval distribution parameters for hidden Markov model states modeling minke whale calling behavior as a function of distance to the nearest calling conspecific within the past 30 min.

      State 1 State 2
      Inter-Call Interval
      Mean 6.85 min 0.63 min
      Standard Deviation 2.54 min 0.36 min

      We compared three different time windows to search for the nearest calling conspecific. The 30 min time window performed the best, followed by 20 min, and 10 min, which all performed better than the null model that tested the hypothesis that minke whale inter-call interval was not a function of distance to the nearest neighbor ( Table 2 ). Because the inter-call interval changed by an order of magnitude between State 1 and State 2, these results were not sensitive to occasional missed localizations along a track. In the stress test dataset, 30% of calls were randomly dropped from a track, but the model ranking and stationary state probability trend remained the same (although the ICIs for the two states increased, as expected since there were fewer calls).

      Comparison of models using three different time windows to search for the nearest calling conspecific ranked by the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and AIC weights (Akaike, 1974; Burnham and Anderson, 2002) H0 represents the null model that calling rate is not a function of nearest calling conspecific.

      Time Window AIC AIC Weight
      30 min 98687 1.0
      20 min 98701 1.1 x 10–3
      10 min 98728 1.6 x 10–9
      H0 98868 5.8 x 10–40

      Distance was modeled as a categorical variable in 5 km bins.

      The probability of a minke whale being in State 1 and calling at a nominal rate increased as its distance to the nearest calling conspecific increased and the probability of a minke whale being in State 2 and calling at a rapid rate increased as its distance to the nearest calling conspecific decreased ( Figure 5 ). Since each observation is a localized call, the plotted probabilities are the probability of a call being in a given state and not the length of time that a whale is in a state ( Figure 5 upper). When a whale is calling rapidly, more localizations are available for a given time than when a whale is calling nominally. Of the 36,033 localized calls that were part of focal tracks, 49% were categorized as being in State 1 and 51% were categorized as being in State 2. However, these tracked whales spent a total of 84.7 days calling nominally (State 1) and 8.0 days calling rapidly (State 2), which is equal to 91% of the time in State 1 and 9% of the time in State 2. Therefore, regardless of the distance to the closest calling conspecific, at any given time, minke whales are most likely in State 1 ( Figure 5 lower). Calling rapidly has a higher probability when whales are closer together, but it is never more likely than calling nominally.

      The probability of a minke whale call being a part of State 1 or 2 as a function of its distance to the nearest calling minke whale (upper plot) and the proportion of time in each of the states (lower plot). The blue and teal points show the stationary state probabilities of State 1 and State 2, respectively. The error bounds show the 95% confidence intervals. The blue and teal bars show the proportion of time that the focal animals were calling in State 1 and 2, respectively. The nearest neighbor distance is binned in 5 km intervals. The distances to the nearest neighbor are the distances to the closest calling whale within the past 30 min.

      We selected two interactions between calling minke whales to illustrate these behaviors with individual whales. The first example occurred on 7 Feb 2017 ( Figure 6 ). The minke whale that generated the primary track was calling entirely in State 1 and changed its heading and speed, and ultimately ceased calling during an encounter with a whale that generated the nearest track and was mainly in State 2. At the start of the primary track it was steadily calling and moving east-southeast, and had a distant nearest neighbor that was outside the search area. The whale that generated the track labeled as the “nearest track” in Figure 6 started calling at 18:31 UTC, almost an hour and 15 min after the start of the primary track. When the nearest track started calling it was 11.8 km away from the primary track and it quickly switched into State 2 and traveled southeast. At about the same time, the primary track turned to a south-southeast heading and increased its median speed before it stopped calling. While both animals were calling, the distance between the primary track and the nearest track varied between 10.8–11.8 km.

      Example minke whale boing tracks at PMRF on 7 Feb 2017. The left panel shows a map of the interaction between two whales. Marker color indicates time and shape indicates calling state, with circles representing the nominal calling state (State 1) and triangles representing the rapid calling state (State 2). The dotted and solid lines connect the localized calls for the primary and nearest track, respectively. The right panel shows the distance to the primary track’s nearest neighbor. Time and calling state are again indicated by color and shape of the markers, respectively. Note that the nearest track between 17:00 and 18:30 UTC was outside of the 15 km search area and is a different track than the “nearest track” depicted in the map. This earlier track is not depicted in the left panel.

      The second example of an interaction between two calling minke whales occurred on 8 Nov 2013 ( Figure 7 ). During this encounter, the minke whale that generated the primary track switched to a rapid calling behavioral state when the whale that generated the nearest track and was also calling rapidly headed towards it. At the start of these tracks, the primary track was traveling southwest and the nearest track was traveling northeast and then north, with the distance between the two tracks decreasing. The primary track switched to calling mainly in State 2 at 6:27 UTC (indicated by arrow in Figure 7 ) when the whales were about 6.9 km apart. At the same time, the nearest track’s heading became more northeast, a reciprocal heading of the primary track. Before the tracks intersected, the whales paused calling for 36 min (primary track) and 15 min (nearest track). The nearest track started calling again first and was traveling northwest. When the primary track started calling it was about 2.7 km southwest of the nearest track and milled in that area while still calling in State 2. The nearest track then turned and headed south towards the primary track. The whale generating the primary track ceased calling when the nearest track was 900 m away.

      Example minke whale boing tracks at PMRF on 8 Nov 2013. The left panel shows a map of the interaction between two whales. Marker color indicates time and shape indicates calling state, with circles representing the nominal calling state (State 1) and triangles representing the rapid calling state (State 2). The arrow indicates when the primary track’s state changed from mainly State 1 to mainly State 2. The dotted and solid lines connect the localized calls for the primary and nearest track, respectively. The right panel shows the distance to the primary track’s nearest neighbor. The time and calling state are again indicated by the color and shape of the markers, respectively.

      4 Discussion

      Minke whales were more likely to call at an increased calling rate when in close proximity to another calling minke whale. They increased their calling rate by an order of magnitude between State 1 and State 2. Converting the ICIs reported in the Results section, the mean calling rate for State 1 was 8.7 calls/whale/hour and the mean calling rate for State 2 was 95.2 calls/whale/hour. The overall mean calling rate for minke whales on PMRF was 15.7 calls/whales/hour. Since calling rate is dependent on distance to the nearest neighbor, calling rate would be expected to be higher in areas where minke whale density is higher and lower in areas where minke whale density is lower.

      The findings in this paper add complication to PAM cue-rate-based density estimation since calling rate is needed and is dependent on the number of animals in an area. In contrast, using PAM tracks of localized calls for a census-type density estimation (counting the number of tracks) is less complicated than density estimation methods using only calling rate, but is only available in places where the number and spacing of hydrophones allow for tracking (requirements for tracking are discussed in detail in Nosal, 2013; Helble et al., 2015). For researchers that are unable to localize and track calls, this work provides information on minke whale calling dynamics that are important to consider when studying calling behavior and density estimation. Although we present a cue rate for minke whale boing calls in this paper, this rate should be applied with caution to density estimation equations because we also found that calling rate is density dependent. In addition, this calling rate is based only on regularly vocalizing animals and we do not know the proportion of silent minke whales. Due to the complicated use of vocalizations by mysticetes, deploying multiple hydrophones to track animals may allow for more accurate PAM-based density estimation. Then the “cue” would be a track and the “cue rate” would be related to the probability of tracking a whale, which could be estimated with passive acoustic tagging studies. However, these tagging studies might show that the probability of acoustically tracking whales is also density dependent, which was shown with humpback whales migrating off the coast of Australia by Noad et al. (2017). Even so, deploying multiple hydrophones to localize and track whales is likely more stable for density estimation because it is easier to define a study area and tracking is less impacted by changes in calling rate.

      Martin et al. (2015) hypothesized that the minke whale boing call is a mating call. Since minke whales increase their calling rate when in close proximity to other calling minke whales, we are hypothesizing that the boing call also communicates territoriality when the calling rate increases. Minke whales might call slowly to advertise their position and keep space between other vocalizing whales. When another whale gets too close, they begin to call rapidly, possibly as a sign of aggression. Territoriality is expressed acoustically in many other animal taxa. For example, territorial male Galápagos sea lions (Zalophus wollebaeki) and greater sac-winged bats (Saccopteryx bilineata) both increased their vocalization rates as the density of conspecifics around them increased (Kunc and Wolf, 2008; Eckenweber and Knörnschild, 2013). Other species including male green frogs (Rana clamitans) and ring-billed gulls (Larus delawarensis) use a specific call type to signal aggression and produced those calls when intruders entered their territory (Wells, 1978; Southern, 1981).

      The minke whale boing call might have a dual purpose of being a mating call and a territorial call. Other species also use the same calls for both aggression and mating. Anurans, birds, and even fish species have been observed to produce the same calls while defending their territory from an intruder as they would if they were trying to attract a mate, although their calling rate may help signify the intent in these different scenarios (e.g. Wiewandt, 1969; Wingfield, 1994; Borgia and Coleman, 2000; Vasconcelos et al., 2010; Baker et al., 2012). However, aggression and mating may not be independent events. Species that use vocalizations to simultaneously deter competitors and attract mates include male midwife toads (Alytes obstetricans and Alytes cisternasii), red deer (Cervus elaphus), and fallow bucks (Dama dama) (McComb, 1991; Bosch and Márquez, 1996; McElligott and Hayden, 1999). These previous studies have used the proximity of non-calling conspecifics (specifically female conspecifics) to help determine the significance of calls, but in our study, we only know the positions of vocal whales and do not know how non-vocal whales are behaving and whether they are attracted or repelled from the boing calls.

      Even if vocalizations are associated with mating behavior, their purpose may not be to attract mates. Sometimes other humpback whale males have approached singers, but singers have also been observed to sing when alone and when escorting mother-calf pairs (e.g. Darling and Bérubé, 2001; Smith et al., 2008). Male-male interactions were not always agonistic and Darling and Bérubé (2001) suggested that they might help in social ordering. Similar to minke whales, singing humpbacks might also change their singing behavior when in close proximity to another singing male and often stop singing if the approach gets very close, regardless of whether or not the other male is singing (Darling and Bérubé, 2001; Smith et al., 2008; Cholewiak et al., 2018). Our understanding of baleen whale behavior has advanced more slowly than other taxa due to their vast communication space and difficulties with visual observations, but studies like these can help to increase our knowledge of these protected species.

      Although the seasonality of the minke whale boing in Hawaiian waters is similar to that of male songs from humpback whales and fin whales (Balaenoptera physalus) (e.g. Guazzo et al., 2020; Helble et al., 2020a), no biopsies have been collected from calling minke whales to provide evidence of their sex (unlike with humpback and fin whales, e.g. Darling and Bérubé, 2001; Croll et al., 2002; Smith et al., 2008). In other taxa, both males and females have been observed producing territorial calls, so our hypothesis that the minke whale call is used to signal aggression does not also imply the sex of the calling animals. For example, both male and female little blue penguins (Eudyptula minor) defended nest sites by vocalizing and/or moving toward intruders and these vocalizations may help them to signal their aggression in the low-visibility nighttime conditions when they are most active (Waas, 1991). Minke whales also live in low-visibility conditions, so vocalizations are more effective than visual signals to communicate a message over long distances. Since territoriality and aggression are associated with mating in many taxa, it is possible that the minke whale boing functions as a mating call when emitted at the lower calling rate and as a territorial or aggressive call at the higher calling rate.

      An alternative hypothesis for the minke whale boing call is that this call is a social call not used for mating, and whales are increasing their calling rate to better communicate location and maintain cohesion with another nearby minke whale. The rarity of boing calls reported at higher latitudes during the summer and fall (Delarue et al., 2012) in potential feeding areas suggests that if the boing call is a social call, it seems to be used primarily in tropical waters from fall to spring. More information about the sex of calling minke whales and the reaction of conspecifics to the boing call would help differentiate between hypotheses.

      Minke whale vocal activity is likely influenced by other factors not tested in this analysis. If the boing call is part of mating behavior, then calling behavior may be influenced by hormone levels and time of year (proposed for humpback whales by Clark and Clapham, 2004; Vu et al., 2012; Vu et al., 2014). The time of day can also affect whale behavior, including vocalization behavior (observed in gray whales (Eschrichtius robustus) by Guazzo et al., 2017). In addition, background noise level might affect the vocalization rate (Helble et al., 2020b; Guazzo et al., 2020). Minke whales might call more if noise levels are higher due to other calling conspecifics in the area or they might call more out of a defensive posture due to reduced communication space. Conversely, minke whales might call less if the communication space is reduced and they cannot hear nearby conspecifics (various responses to increased noise level are reviewed in Brumm and Zollinger, 2011). These variables should be tested with future research.

      Ideally, similar studies should be performed in other locations to assess how minke whales and other species vocalize as a function of density. However, very few hydrophone set-ups currently available would be able to replicate the study described in this paper. PMRF is a unique area in that we were able to guarantee detection and localization of calling minke whales within a large (over 3,100 km2) search area. A large search area is required to be able to test for behavioral responses based on distance to a conspecific when conspecifics are spread out on the order of multiple kilometers.

      In conclusion, minke whales producing boing calls in Hawaiian waters are more likely to call at a rapid rate when they are closer to other vocalizing conspecifics. Many questions still remain about the function of these boing calls, but the unique hydrophone array set-up at PMRF combined with the statistical tools applied here allowed for the analysis of more than 500 minke whale tracks containing tens of thousands of calls. Since the calling rate is dependent on the distance to the nearest neighbor, the calling rate would be expected to be higher in areas where the density of minke whales is higher and lower in areas where the density of minke whales is lower, adding complication to density estimation using passive acoustic monitoring. Despite this added difficulty when estimating abundance, these findings provide new information about minke whale calling behavior on what is probably a breeding area.

      Data availability statement

      The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://seamap.env.duke.edu/dataset/2190.

      Ethics statement

      Ethical review and approval was not required for the animal study because this study was conducted using passive acoustic monitoring of wild animals with underwater microphones (i.e. hydrophones) and has no impact on animals’ lifecycle.

      Author contributions

      CM and SM formulated the idea for this study and CM performed the initial analysis. CM, SM, BM, and EH collected the data. GA and BM programmed the detection, classification, and localization algorithms. GA managed and post-processed the data. SM developed the initial tracking algorithm and CM, GA, and SM tuned the tracking algorithm for minke whale boings. ID assisted with the HMM statistical analysis. CM, RG, and TH validated and analyzed the data and wrote the draft manuscript. All authors discussed the results and contributed to the final manuscript.

      Funding

      This research was supported by Commander, U.S. Pacific Fleet (Code N465JR, AwardNumber N0007020WR0EP8F) and tool development utilized for this analysis was supported by the U.S. Navy’s Living Marine Resources Program (Award Number N0002520WR0141R).

      Acknowledgments

      The authors thank the personnel at PMRF, especially Eliseo Boloson, Mike Dick, Jim Hager, Robin Higuchi, Bryson Kurokawa, Jon Winsley, and Jeffrey Yates, for obtaining recordings of acoustic data, as without the acoustic data this type of analysis would not be possible.

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Akaike H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Automatic Control 19, 716723. doi: 10.1109/tac.1974.1100705 Baker T. M. Wilson D. R. Mennill D. J. (2012). Vocal signals predict attack during aggressive interactions in black-capped chickadees. Anim. Behav. 84, 965974. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.022 Borgia G. Coleman S. W. (2000). Co-Option of male courtship signals from aggressive display in bowerbirds. Proc. R Soc. Lond B Biol. Sci. 267, 17351740. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1203 Bosch J. Márquez R. (1996). Acoustic competition in male midwife toads Alytes obstetricans and Alytes cisternasii: response to neighbor size and calling rate. implications for female choice. Ethology 102, 841855. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01205.x Brumm H. Zollinger S. A. (2011). The evolution of the Lombard effect: 100 years of psychoacoustic research. Behaviour 148, 11731198. doi: 10.1163/000579511x605759 Burnham K. P. Anderson D. R. (2002). Model selection and multimodal inference. 2 edn (New York: Springer-Verlag). doi: 10.1007/b97636 Carretta J. V. Oleson E. M. Weller D. W. Lang A. R. Forney K. A. Baker J. . (2014). U.S. Pacific marine mammal stock assessments: 2013. NOAA Technical Memorandum, NOAA-TM-NMFS-SWFSC-532. (Department of Commerce). Center for Conservation Bioacoustics (2019). Raven Pro: Interactive sound analysis software (Version 1.6.1) (Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology). Available at: http://ravensoundsoftware.com/. Cholewiak D. M. Cerchio S. Jacobsen J. K. Urbán-R. J. Clark C. W. (2018). Songbird dynamics under the sea: acoustic interactions between humpback whales suggest song mediates male interactions. R. Soc Open Sci. 5, 171298. doi: 10.1098/rsos.171298 Clark C. W. Clapham P. J. (2004). Acoustic monitoring on a humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) feeding ground shows continual singing into late spring. Proc. R Soc. Lond B Biol. Sci. 271, 10511057. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2699 Croll D. A. Clark C. W. Acevedo A. Tershy B. Flores S. Gedamke J. . (2002). Only male fin whales sing loud songs. Nature 417, 809. doi: 10.1038/417809a Darling J. D. Bérubé M. (2001). Interactions of singing humpback whales with other males. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 17, 570584. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2001.tb01005.x Delarue J. Martin B. Hannay D. (2012). Minke whale boing sound detections in the northeastern chukchi Sea. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 29, E333E341. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2012.00611.x Durbach I. N. Harris C. M. Martin C. Helble T. A. Henderson E. E. Ierley G. . (2021). Changes in the movement and calling behaviour of minke whales (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) in response to Navy training. Front. Mar. Sci. 8. doi: 10.3389/fmars.2021.660122 Eckenweber M. Knörnschild M. (2013). Social influences on territorial signaling in male greater sac-winged bats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 639648. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1483-z Guazzo R. A. Helble T. A. Alongi G. C. Durbach I. N. Martin C. R. Martin S. W. . (2020). Lombard Effect: humpback whale song source levels increase as ambient ocean noise levels increase. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 148, 542555. doi: 10.1121/10.0001669 Guazzo R. A. Helble T. A. D’Spain G. L. Weller D. W. Wiggins S. M. Hildebrand J. A. (2017). Migratory behavior of eastern north Pacific gray whales tracked using a hydrophone array. PloS One 12, e0185585. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185585 Harris C. M. Martin S. W. Martin C. Helble T. A. Henderson E. E. Paxton C. G. M. . (2019). Changes in the spatial distribution of acoustically derived minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) tracks in response to Navy training. Aquat. Mamm. 45, 661674. doi: 10.1578/am.45.6.2019.661 Helble T. A. Guazzo R. A. Alongi G. C. Martin C. R. Martin S. W. Henderson E. E. (2020a). Fin whale song patterns shift over time in the central north Pacific. Front. Mar. Sci. 7. doi: 10.3389/fmars.2020.587110 Helble T. A. Guazzo R. A. Martin C. R. Durbach I. N. Alongi G. C. Martin S. W. . (2020b). Lombard Effect: minke whale call source levels vary with natural variations in ocean noise. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 147, 698712. doi: 10.1121/10.0000596 Helble T. A. Ierley G. R. D’Spain G. L. Martin S. W. (2015). Automated acoustic localization and call association for vocalizing humpback whales on the navy’s Pacific missile range facility. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 137, 1121. doi: 10.1121/1.4904505 Klay J. Mellinger D. K. Moretti D. J. Martin S. W. Roch M. A. (2015). Advanced methods for passive acoustic detection, classification, and localization of marine mammals. Tech. rep., Office of Naval Research, Arlington, VA. Last accessed 22 January 2020. Available at: https://www.onr.navy.mil/reports/FY15/mbklay.pdf. Kunc H. P. Wolf J. B. W. (2008). Seasonal changes of vocal rates and their relation to territorial status in male galápagos sea lions (Zalophus wollebaeki). Ethology 114, 381388. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01484.x Langrock R. King R. Matthiopoulos J. Thomas L. Fortin D. Morales J. M. (2012). Flexible and practical modeling of animal telemetry data: hidden Markov models and extensions. Ecology 93, 23362342. doi: 10.1890/11-2241.1 Marques T. A. Thomas L. Ward J. DiMarzio N. Tyack P. L. (2009). Estimating cetacean population density using fixed passive acoustic sensors: an example with Blainville’s beaked whales. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 125, 19821994. doi: 10.1121/1.3089590 Martin S. W. (2009). The ecology and acoustic behavior of wintering minke whales in the Hawaiian and Pacific islands. techreport N0001409WX20964 (San Diego, CA: SPAWAR Systems Center Pacific). Martin S. W. Marques T. A. Thomas L. Morrissey R. P. Jarvis S. DiMarzio N. . (2013). Estimating minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) boing sound density using passive acoustic sensors. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 29, 142158. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2011.00561.x Martin S. W. Martin C. R. Matsuyama B. M. Henderson E. E. (2015). Minke whales (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) respond to Navy training. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 137, 25332541. doi: 10.1121/1.4919319 McClintock B. T. Michelot T. (2018). momentuHMM: R package for generalized hidden Markov models of animal movement. Methods Ecol. Evol. 9, 15181530. doi: 10.1111/2041-210x.12995 McComb K. E. (1991). Female choice for high roaring rates in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Anim. Behav. 41, 7988. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80504-4 McElligott A. G. Hayden T. J. (1999). Context-related vocalization rates of fallow bucks, Dama dama. Anim. Behav. 58, 10951104. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1237 Mellinger D. K. Martin S. W. Morrissey R. P. Thomas L. Yosco J. J. (2011). A method for detecting whistles, moans, and other frequency contour sounds. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 129, 40554061. doi: 10.1121/1.3531926 Nikolich K. Towers J. R. (2020). Vocalizations of common minke whales (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) in an eastern north Pacific feeding ground. Bioacoustics 29, 97108. doi: 10.1080/09524622.2018.1555716 Noad M. J. Dunlop R. A. Mack A. K. (2017). Changes in humpback whale singing behavior with abundance: implications for the development of acoustic surveys of cetaceans. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 142, 16111618. doi: 10.1121/1.5001502 Nosal E.-M. (2013). Methods for tracking multiple marine mammals with wide-baseline passive acoustic arrays. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 134, 23832392. doi: 10.1121/1.4816549 Oswald J. N. Au W. W. L. Duennebier F. (2011). Minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) boings detected at the station ALOHA cabled observatory. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 129, 33533360. doi: 10.1121/1.3575555 Rankin S. Barlow J. (2005). Source of the north Pacific “boing” sound attributed to minke whales. J. Acoust. Soc Am. 118, 33463351. doi: 10.1121/1.2046747 R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing). Smith J. N. Goldizen A. W. Dunlop R. A. Noad M. J. (2008). Songs of male humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, are involved in intersexual interactions. Anim. Behav. 76, 467477. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.02.013 Southern L. K. (1981). Sex-related differences in territorial aggression by ring-billed gulls. Auk 98, 179181. http://www.jstor.org/stable/4085622 Thompson P. O. Friedl W. A. (1982). A long term study of low frequency sounds from several species of whales off Oahu, Hawaii. Cetology 45, 119. Towers J. R. McMillan C. J. Malleson M. Hildering J. Ford J. K. B. Ellis G. M. (2013). Seasonal movements and ecological markers as evidence for migration of common minke whales photo-identified in the eastern north Pacific. J. Cetacean Res. Manage. 13, 221229. Tyack P. (1981). Interactions between singing Hawaiian humpback whales and conspecifics nearby. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 8, 105116. doi: 10.1007/BF00300822 Vasconcelos R. O. Simões J. M. Almada V. C. Fonseca P. J. Amorim M. C. P. (2010). Vocal behavior during territorial intrusions in the Lusitanian toadfish: boatwhistles also function as territorial ‘keep-out’ signals. Ethology 116, 155165. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01722.x Vu E. T. Clark C. Catelani K. Kellar N. M. Calambokidis J. (2014). Seasonal blubber testosterone concentrations of male humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). Mar. Mamm. Sci. 31, 12581264. doi: 10.1111/mms.12191 Vu E. T. Risch D. Clark C. W. Gaylord S. Hatch L. T. Thompson M. A. . (2012). Humpback whale song occurs extensively on feeding grounds in the western north Atlantic ocean. Aquat. Biol. 14, 175183. doi: 10.3354/ab00390 Waas J. R. (1991). The risks and benefits of signalling aggressive motivation: a study of cave-dwelling little blue penguins. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 139146. doi: 10.1007/bf00166489 Wells K. D. (1978). Territoriality in the green frog (Rana clamitans): vocalizations and agonistic behaviour. Anim. Behav. 26, 10511063. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(78)90094-5 Wenz G. M. (1964). Curious noises and the sonic environment in the ocean. Mar. Bio-acoustics 1, 101119. Wiewandt T. A. (1969). Vocalization, aggressive behavior, and territoriality in the bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana. Copeia 1969, 276. doi: 10.2307/1442074 Wingfield J. C. (1994). Control of territorial aggression in a changing environment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 19, 709721. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)90052-3 Zerbini A. N. Waite J. M. Laake J. L. Wade P. R. (2006). Abundance, trends and distribution of baleen whales off western Alaska and the central Aleutian islands. Deep Sea Res. Part I 53, 17721790. doi: 10.1016/j.dsr.2006.08.009
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016feelface.com.cn
      griven.com.cn
      www.lrvrtm.org.cn
      hnzz666.org.cn
      tsfhc.org.cn
      www.nz8news.com.cn
      www.orpxg7.com.cn
      www.muzt.com.cn
      www.ptbick.com.cn
      www.wyao58.org.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p