Edited by: Carmen Moret-Tatay, Catholic University of Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Spain
Reviewed by: Andrés Antonio González-Garrido, University of Guadalajara, Mexico; José Salvador Blasco Magraner, University of Valencia, Spain
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Several studies have implied gender differences in startle reaction to emotional facial expressions. However, few studies have been designed to investigate the difference between responding to emotional female vs. male faces, nor gender differences in responses. The present experiment investigated startle EMG responses to a startle probe while viewing pictures of neutral, happy, angry, fearful, and sad facial expressions presented by female and male models. Participants were divided into female and male groups. Results showed that emotional facial expressions interact with model gender to produce startle potentiation to a probe: greater responses were found while viewing angry expressions by male models, and while viewing happy faces by female models. There were no differences in responses between male and female participants. We argue that these findings underline theimportance of controlling for model gender in research using facial expressions as stimuli.
香京julia种子在线播放
Visual stimuli are one of the most potent sources of information regarding our immediate environment. Emotional visual stimuli give us the information necessary for our well-being, and reactions to these stimuli have been investigated in numerous studies. In research, pictures containing emotional stimuli have been widely used to induce affect. The typical finding is that negative emotional pictures result in stronger reactions compared to neutral and positive images (Vrana et al.,
A relatively simple, but effective, tool for indexing emotional reactions is the startle eyeblink reflex. The startle reflex is a basic defensive reaction to sudden and intense stimuli (e.g., a loud noise). In humans, this involves, and is indexed by, the closure of the eyelids. It has been well documented that the amplitude of the startle eyeblink changes with the emotional stimuli viewed at the same time (Lang et al.,
Several studies have found the mentioned effect of potentiated startle while watching angry facial expressions. Springer et al. (
For the expression of fear, the results are less clear. Anokhin and Golosheykin (
For happy expressions, the results have been even more scarce. Alpers et al. (
A few studies have controlled for gender effects about startle reactions to emotional facial expressions. However, the ones which have included gender as a factor has generally found some effects. Hess et al. (
Anokhin and Golosheykin (
In the present study, we investigated the startle reflex during presentations of pictures displaying neutral, fearful, angry, happy, and sad facial expressions. Also, we assessed the effect of model gender and participant gender. Based on previous studies, we expected increased startle to anger, but possibly only male expressions of anger. For female expressions, we expected greater responses to happy faces compared to male faces with the same emotion. Besides, we controlled for any effects of participant gender by adding observer gender as a factor. In an exploratory part of the experiment, we included the facial expression of sadness. As we are not aware of any previous studies presenting this emotion, we had no specific hypothesis in that regard.
Forty-three individuals (21 men, 22 women, age range 19–35, mean age 23, 5 years) participated in the study. Subjective data were missing for eight participants. Data from two participants were excluded from startle analysis due to small responses. This left 35 participants for the subjective data analysis (18 men and 17 women), and 41 for the startle analysis (20 men and 21 women). All participants reported good health and did not report any consequential hearing problems, previous severe disease, or injury. The participants were instructed not to drink caffeinated beverages or use nicotine-containing substances for three hours before the experiment. They were also told that they could withdraw from the study at any time without giving any reason. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, who were given two lottery tickets (equivalent to 50 NOK) for their participation or course credit for an introductory psychology class. The education level of the participants was not obtained.
The experiment took place in an electrically and acoustically shielded chamber where the temperature was kept at about 20°C. A Bruel and Kjær 2235 Sound Level Precision Meter was used to measure the intensity of auditory stimuli. Programs for experimental control were written by the first author in Coulbourn Human Startle System HSW v. 7.500-00 and run on a Microsoft Windows XP based Dell PC that controlled presentation of experimental stimuli and data acquisition. The same software was used for scoring of startle responses.
Pictures of faces were randomly selected from the Radboud Faces Database (Langner et al.,
Startle-eliciting noise had an intensity of 95 dB (SPL), instantaneous rise time, and a duration of 50 ms. The stimuli were delivered through Sennheiser HD 250 headphones. The startle-eliciting noise was presented between 4,000 and 5,000 ms after picture onset (random for every trial). The startle-eliciting stimulus was presented at every trial (once per picture). Before the presentation of any pictures, five trials of startle-eliciting noise was presented to minimize the effect of habituation in picture trials.
Startle eyeblink electromyographic (EMG) responses were recorded from the right orbicularis oculi (for practical reasons) with two sintered-pellet silver chloride AgCl miniature electrodes (4 mm diameter) filled with Microlyte electrolyte gel (Coulbourn Instruments). Inter-electrode distance was 1.5–2 cm. The ground electrode was placed centrally on the forehead. A factor of 50,000 amplified the EMG signal and filtered (passing 13–1,000 Hz) by a Coulbourn V75-04 bioamplifier. The signal was rectified and integrated with a Coulbourn V76-24 contour-following integrator with a 10-ms time constant, and the output was sent to the PC
For the subjective data, participants used the mouse pointer to indicate the level of valence, arousal, and domination elicited by each picture using a visual analog scale (VAS) on a computer screen. Each participant viewed each picture for as long as they wanted before rating. The instruction read (for valence): “Please mark on the line below how positive or negative you feel after seeing the face;” for arousal, the endpoints were labeled “relaxed,” and “agitated,” and for domination, the endpoints were “submissive” and “dominant.” The response range for both was from 0–100 mm. The program for the VAS was written in, and controlled by, MATLAB version 8.3 with Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard,
After arrival at the laboratory, the participants sat down in a desk chair, read and signed the Informed Consent Form. After that, the participants were lead into the experimental chamber and seated in a reclining chair. The subjects were informed of the general purpose of the study and about the stimuli and procedure. They were also told that they could withdraw from the study without giving any reason at any time. The skin below the participants’ right eye was cleaned with a swab containing alcohol and pumice, and the electrodes for measurement of the startle blink electromyography (EMG) were attached. The headphones were attached, and the experimental procedure was initiated as described in the “Apparatus and Stimuli” section. The door to the experimental chamber was closed during all stimulus presentation.
After the startle session, the participants rated the pictures (valence, arousal, and domination) on a computer in the room adjacent to the experimental chamber (more details in the “Apparatus and Stimuli” section). After the subjective test, the experiment was over, the participants received the lottery tickets and left.
Startle blink reflexes were scored as the difference between the maximum amplitude of the EMG response in the window from 0–200 ms after noise onset, compared to the mean EMG level for the last 100 ms before the onset of the startle-eliciting noise on that trial. Startle amplitude values were T-transformed (Z-scores multiplied by 10 and add 50). The startle baseline on each trial was calculated as the mean EMG activity in the last 100 ms before the startle-eliciting stimulus. Values below 20 A/D-units were scored as a non-response. The average for each picture type excluded values of zeroes (nonresponse trials). Less than 7% of startle responses were scored as nonresponses. Following these criteria, two participants were excluded from startle analysis. One because of nonresponses on more than 50% of the trials and one because of nonresponses on four of five trials to one picture type.
The design was a two model gender (male, female) by five emotion (neutral, fearful, angry, happy, sad) by two participant gender (men, women) mixed design, where the two first factors were within factors and the last was a between factor. Results were considered significant if
There was a significant interaction of Model Gender by Emotion (
Startle response elicited during the viewing of pictures of different facial expressions showed by male and female models. Error bars represent +1 standard error of the mean.
Following up the interaction, contrast analysis revealed the following differences: There was greater startle to male angry facial expressions compared to male happy faces (
Planned comparisons showed greater startle to female happy facial expressions compared to female neutral faces (
Regarding the difference between male and female expressions the following interesting differences was revealed: Greater startle to female happy expressions compared to male happy (
There was a significant main effect of Emotion (
Valence ratings to pictures of different facial expressions showed by male and female models. Error bars represent +1 standard error of the mean.
There were no other significant main effects or interactions.
There was a significant main effect of Model Gender (
Arousal ratings to pictures of different facial expressions showed by male and female models. Error bars represent +1 standard error of the mean.
In addition, there was a significant main effect of Emotion (
There was a significant interaction of Emotion by Participant Gender (
In addition, there was a significant interaction of Model Gender by Emotion by Participant Gender (
There were no other significant main effects or interactions.
There was a significant main effect of Participant Gender (
Two main findings emerged in the present study: Startle was potentiated to angry male faces and happy female faces. Angry male faces produced significantly increased startle compared to happy male faces, and angry female faces. Happy female faces significantly increased startle compared to female neutral, angry, and sad faces. Also, happy female faces produced greater startle compared to happy male faces. This is one of the first studies, to systematically investigate both participant gender and model gender concerning emotional expression in startle responding.
Increased reactions to angry facial expressions are the typical finding in research utilizing startle as the outcome variable (Balaban,
Potentiated startle to angry male faces, but not to the same expressions shown by females, is an interesting finding. The same results were reported by Paulus et al. (
Another possible explanation for the finding that only angry male faces potentiate startle is that angry male faces are perceived as angrier than angry female expressions. That is, maybe only angry male expressions are extreme enough to produce startle potentiation. If so, this would be in line with Dunning et al. (
Startle potentiation to happy faces has been less investigated (Hess et al.,
Facial stimuli also convey social meaning. Hence, it is important to remember that, for the participants, the models are both strangers, and showing fake smiles. Fake smiles in the regard that they are told to smile, and are, probably, not experiencing the joy that merits the facial expression. As such, the participants are reacting to pictures of unknown women who are directing fake smiles their way. However, this would be true for both female and male models, so strangers, showing fake smiles cannot be the whole explanation. Nevertheless, in a study investigating the perceived genuineness of pictures of emotional facial expressions, Dawel et al. (
Another possibility is that there are some differences between male and female smiles in general. What are these differences? For one, females tend to smile more than males (Briton and Hall,
Research in other domains has also singled out reactions to angry male faces and happy female faces. In reaction time studies participants are faster and more accurate detecting angry expressions on male faces and at detecting happy expressions on female faces (e.g., Becker et al.,
The difference between male angry faces and male neutral expressions was not significant, although there was a tendency for greater responses to angry male faces compared to neutral (
This discrepancy concerning model gender and emotional expressions underlines the importance of having expressers gender as a factor. Having both men and women as models, and treat them as identical stimuli, may severely distort the results in research on startle responding to facial stimuli. This may also explain the different results from previous studies. For instance, using an equal number of male and female stimuli would probably cancel out some of the effects seen in the present study, and using unequal numbers would possibly skew the data in the direction of the most typical findings of the overrepresented gender. Model gender should always be controlled for in future studies in this field: A conclusion that was also drawn by Duval et al. (
As mentioned, the different results from the present study compared to earlier studies may be caused by the different pictures utilized. In the present study, pictures were chosen from the Radboud Faces Database (Langner et al.,
One other difference between earlier studies and the present is that the present study had more power, as it was designed to analyze the male and female groups separately. However, as there was no difference in startle reactions between male and female participants, the analysis profited from a large number of participants in the other analysis. Of course, this is also a strong point of the present study, as a larger sample increases the validity of the data.
Potentiated startle to smiling female faces compared to male happy, and angry male faces compared to female angry, stands in contrast to the subjective data. In the subjective data, there were no differences between ratings of valence, arousal, or dominance to these pictures. The reason behind this is not obvious, but it may that subjective data and startle data reflects different levels of processing (Sander et al.,
We did not find any effect of participant gender in the present study, in contrast to Anokhin and Golosheykin (
Another limitation of the present study was the use of only startle eyeblink as a psychophysiological measure. Including other measures could have proven valuable in terms of explaining the results. Asking the participants to rate the genuineness of the stimuli would also have been beneficial. In future research using pictures of emotional facial expressions, this is recommended, as there may very well be a difference in how we react to genuine and fake expressions.
As an exploratory part of the study, we included the facial expression of sadness. Sadness is a negative emotion that is different from fear and anger in that it is much less arousing. It seems that there are stronger responses to male sad faces, compared to females, but this difference is not significant. However, it may be an idea to investigate this further in future studies with other stimuli materials.
In conclusion, the present study revealed that startle potentiation to emotional facial expressions depends on the gender of the model, while there was no effect of participant gender. Moreover, angry male faces potentiated startle responses, while angry female faces did not. For happy faces, the situation was reversed, as female happy faces potentiated startle, while happy male faces did not. These results underline the importance of controlling for model gender in research utilizing facial expressions of emotions as stimuli.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
OÅ developed the theoretical idea. OÅ and MØ planned the experimental protocol. MØ programmed the basic data collection procedure, while OÅ incorporated this into the final experimental setup. Both authors contributed to data analysis, and discussed the results. OÅ wrote the manuscript with support from MØ. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.