Front. Ecol. Evol. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution Front. Ecol. Evol. 2296-701X Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fevo.2021.721241 Ecology and Evolution Original Research Flower Color as Predictor for Nectar Reward Quantity in an Alpine Flower Community Streinzer Martin 1 * Neumayer Johann 2 Spaethe Johannes 3 1Department of Neurobiology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria 2Independent Researcher, Elixhausen, Austria 3Department of Behavioral Physiology and Sociobiology (Zoology II), Biocenter, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany

Edited by: Isabel Marques, University of Lisbon, Portugal

Reviewed by: Jair E. Garcia, RMIT University, Australia; Mario Vallejo-Marin, University of Stirling, United Kingdom

*Correspondence: Martin Streinzer, martin.streinzer@univie.ac.at

These authors have contributed equally to this work

This article was submitted to Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution

09 12 2021 2021 9 721241 06 06 2021 17 11 2021 Copyright © 2021 Streinzer, Neumayer and Spaethe. 2021 Streinzer, Neumayer and Spaethe

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Entomophilous plants have evolved colorful floral displays to attract flower visitors to achieve pollination. Although many insects possess innate preferences for certain colors, the underlying proximate and ultimate causes for this behavior are still not well understood. It has been hypothesized that the floral rewards, e.g., sugar content, of plants belonging to a particular color category correlate with the preference of the flower visitors. However, this hypothesis has been tested only for a subset of plant communities worldwide. Bumble bees are the most important pollinators in alpine environments and show a strong innate preference for (bee) “UV-blue” and “blue” colors. We surveyed plants visited by bumble bees in the subalpine and alpine zones (>1,400 m a.s.l.) of the Austrian Alps and measured nectar reward and spectral reflectance of the flowers. We found that the majority of the 105 plant samples visited by bumble bees fall into the color categories “blue” and “blue-green” of a bee-specific color space. Our study shows that color category is only a weak indicator for nectar reward quantity; and due to the high reward variance within and between categories, we do not consider floral color as a reliable signal for bumble bees in the surveyed habitat. Nevertheless, since mean floral reward quantity differs between categories, naïve bumble bees may benefit from visiting flowers that fall into the innately preferred color category during their first foraging flights.

flower color color preference nectar reward alpine Bombus

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Flower color is a major trait by which plants convey information about their identity and location to a potential visitor. Color is not an intrinsic property of the flower organ, but rather a psychophysical phenomenon that depends on the visual system of the observer that perceives the reflected light spectrum (Kelber and Osorio, 2010; Skorupski and Chittka, 2011). Bees, one of the major pollinator groups of angiosperms, possess three distinct photoreceptor types in their compound eyes, which are most sensitive in the ultraviolet (UV), blue and green part of the light spectrum and enable them to use trichromatic color vision. The number of different photoreceptors and their sensitivity maxima are phylogenetically conserved among most bees (Peitsch et al., 1992; Briscoe and Chittka, 2001) and their origin predates that of angiosperms (Chittka, 1996).

      Color, as other floral traits like scent, shape or size, are used by flower visitors to detect and identify specific plants and thus to associate reward quantity and quality after a visit with a particular floral display. After several visits, pollinators are able to predict the reward probability of a particular flower type in subsequent foraging flights and thus may develop floral (color) preferences based on previous experience. In addition to this learned preference, bees and many other pollinator groups, show an innate preference for certain color(s) or color categories (Giurfa et al., 1995; Lunau and Maier, 1995; Goyret et al., 2008; Streinzer et al., 2019). This preference already exists before any learning took place and is usually overwritten after experience, although it has been shown that bumble bees may revert to their innate preference when confronted with novel floral features (Gumbert, 2000). However, the underlying proximate and ultimate reasons of an innate color preference are currently not well understood. Innate preferences may be adaptive, if they for example allow naïve individuals to better find rewarding food sources as compared to a random search strategy. The preferred flower type may provide a higher reward, an optimal nutritional composition or show a floral morphology that is adapted to the pollinator’s mouthparts and allows an efficient exploitation. Flower-naïve hoverflies (Eristalis tenax), for example, possess an innate preference for yellow and white flowers, and after landing on a flower they reflexively extend their proboscis when confronted with small UV-absorbing yellow spots. This behavior is assumed to help naïve flies to efficiently find and extract the pollen or nectar of a flower (Lunau and Wacht, 1997).

      In bees, several species are shown to have strong preferences for “UV-blue”, “blue” and “blue-green” colors in a bee-specific color space (Giurfa et al., 1995; Gumbert, 2000; Raine and Chittka, 2007b; Dyer et al., 2016). In one of the earliest studies, aiming to link the color of flowers with its predictive value for reward, Giurfa et al. (1995) studied flowering plants in a nature reserve in northern Germany. The authors found that flowers, which fall in the above-mentioned bee-color categories, have, on average, higher reward quantities compared to flowers with unattractive colors. However, similar patterns have not been observed in other world regions, e.g., Australia, where the highest rewards were found in color categories like “green” and “UV-green” (Shrestha et al., 2020). Nevertheless, studies on flower communities, which investigated color frequencies and distribution, generally found a non-uniform distribution of flower colors among the different categories. Distributions were found to be remarkably similar across continents, with a maximum of spectral reflection patterns falling in the “blue-green” and “blue” sectors of a bee-specific color space (Chittka et al., 1994; Dyer et al., 2012; Bischoff et al., 2013; Shrestha et al., 2014; Ortiz et al., 2021).

      In this study, we measured nectar reward quantity (nectar volume and sugar concentration) and spectral reflectance of the majority of bumble bee visited flowering plants in the Eastern Alps in Europe. At high altitudes, bumble bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombus Latreille) are the most important pollinators of bee-visited plant species (Bingham et al., 1998). In contrast to the abundant bumble bees, other bee species found in the same habitat (e.g., Apis mellifera, Andrena rogenhoferi, A. lapponica, and Osmia spp.) are less important due to their lower densities at high elevations with harsh weather conditions (Ebmer, 2003). Based on the finding that bumble bees possess an innate preference for particular flower colors (and thus a higher propensity to visit such flowers), we tested whether the preference is reflected in the nectar reward quantity of flowers of these colors and can thus be considered as being adaptive.

      Materials and Methods Study Region

      All measurements were conducted in the Hohe Tauern National Park located in the main chain of the Eastern Alps in Austria. All sampling sites were located in the subalpine to alpine range between 1,400 and 2,600 m a.s.l. The sites were scattered roughly along the Großglockner panoramic road in the National Park. Experiments were carried out in the years 1994–2020 (nectar measurements: 1994–2020; spectral reflectance measurements: 2006–2020). Research and sampling permits for sites within the National Park core region were issued by the “Land Salzburg” (permit no. 21 301-RI/547/161-2010, to JN).

      Plant Identification

      Plants were either identified in the field or taken to the lab for species-level identification using adequate identification keys. For consistency, all identifications followed the nomenclature by Fischer et al. (2008). Some species could only be identified to the level of a species-group of closely related species, which is indicated by the suffix “agg.” (for aggregate; e.g., Saxifraga oppositifolia agg., Thymus praecox agg.). All sampled species are listed in Supplementary Table 1.

      Visitor Observations

      To include only those plant species that are actually used by bumble bees as a nectar source, we used a database containing museum specimens, observations and literature records pertaining to the majority of field observations made on bumble bees in Austria involving over 42,000 flower visitation records collected between 1848 and 2021. A minimum criterion of five databased observations of bumble bee visits for a certain plant species was set, to include the species in the analysis. For high altitude alpine plants for which the observation density is generally lower (e.g., Phyteuma globulariifolium, Saxifraga spp., alpine Salix spp.), we accepted a threshold of three databased records (in N = 5 plant species). All plant species that failed to meet these criteria were removed from the analysis.

      Nectar Measurements

      Nectar measurements were performed as a series of five measurements during consecutive 2-h intervals, covering the entire day between 7 am and 5 pm CET (7–9 am, 9–11 am, 11 am–1 pm, 1–3 pm, and 3–5 pm). Measurements were only performed on days without precipitation, and with a mean relative humidity below 80% during the day (11 am–4 pm). Since weather conditions are fairly unpredictable in the alpine area, some measurement series were aborted and missing time intervals were completed on the next appropriate day.

      For each series, the nectar volume and concentration were measured in at least 10 flowers (5 in rare species), with a maximum of three different flowers measured per individual plant and time interval. Flowers were randomly selected from the plants/inflorescences. In each measurement series, individuals were investigated from the same population. We then calculated the mean reward quantity for each species and time interval. Two different values were used in the analysis. First, we selected the maximum value across the time series. This value relates to the maximum nectar standing crop of flowers that were shielded from visitors for up to 24 h (in the bagged condition), and thus represents a value that is comparable to previous studies (Shrestha et al., 2020). Secondly, we selected the mean value across the time series, which serves as a more realistic standing crop that would, on average, be available for visitors throughout the day.

      Open flowers are usually depleted by nectar-seeking visitors. To estimate both the intrinsic nectar production of a flower and the nectar standing crop that is actually available for visitors, we recorded series of measurements from flowers, which were covered to exclude visitors, and from unprotected (“open”) flowers. For the intrinsic nectar production, a white plastic mesh was placed around the flowers and inflorescences in the early morning, before the first flower visitors were active (referred to as “bagged” in the text). A mesh width of 0.8–1.5 mm allowed sufficient airflow to minimize the microclimatic influence of solar radiation, extreme temperatures and humidity, which are known to affect nectar amount and concentration, but excluded all potential visitors (Corbet et al., 1979; Kearns and Inouye, 1993). Nectar measurement series on bagged flowers were carried out in parallel to series on open flowers at the same site and day.

      Nectar was extracted from flowers following the protocol of Corbet (2003), with microcapillaries of 5.0 μl (Assistant, Germany), 1.0 μl, 0.5 μl, or 0.2 μl (Drummond, Great Britain) volume. For flowers with several separated nectaries, the nectar from all nectaries was extracted. The length of the liquid column was measured to the nearest 0.5 mm and converted to μl volume. The contained liquid was then expelled onto a hand refractometer suitable for small volumes, and calibrated for concentrations between 0 and 50% sucrose equivalent (Bellingham and Stanley, Great Britain). When a higher nectar concentration was anticipated, the nectar was diluted by dipping the microcapillary into distilled water (Aqua dest.) before the measurement (Corbet, 2003). The nectar volume was then determined before and after dilution, and the ratio was used to calculate the original sugar concentration. For flowers containing very small nectar volumes (<0.05 μl), the nectar was generally diluted with Aqua dest. to allow measurement with the refractometer. Conversion of the volumes was performed as described above. In the first years of the study (1994–2000), measurements on flowers with very small volumes (<0.1 μl) were conducted by pooling the nectar from several flowers. The total volume was then divided by the number of flowers used to reach the threshold volume for measurement with the refractometer. For extremely small nectar volumes (<0.02 μl) concentration measurements failed in a few cases. We then used the average concentration from the other flowers in the same 2 h interval to determine the sugar content of the flower. In open flowers, nectar can concentrate during hot and dry weather conditions. When concentrations exceeded 80%, measurements were excluded from the analysis, since bees do not visit flowers with highly concentrated nectar (own unpublished observations; see also Harder, 1986).

      Since both nectar volume and nectar concentration are strongly influenced by ambient conditions, we calculated the amount of sugar in the nectar (Bertsch, 1983). To calculate the total amount of reward per flower, our measurements were converted to mg sucrose equivalent/flower using standard procedures (Cruden and Hermann, 1983; Kearns and Inouye, 1993; Corbet, 2003). In brief, the measured nectar volume was multiplied by the measured sugar concentration. The sugar concentration measured with the refractometer (calibrated for weight:weight concentration) was corrected to the appropriate unit (weight:volume concentration) using a correction factor according to Cruden and Hermann (1983).

      Since flowers often form units, e.g., inflorescences, flower baskets or synflorescences consisting of individual inflorescences/baskets (Fischer et al., 2008), we also aimed to quantify the nectar reward of the entire flowering units. We first defined a “flowering unit” from a visual perspective. Each entity that constitutes a separate visual cue during approach was treated as a single unit. Parts of inflorescences which stand far apart, form no continuous unit and which force visitors to fly between, were treated as separate units (e.g., Aconitum degenii and Adenostyles alliariae). For each plant species in our dataset, we counted the number of individual flowers per unit in at least 10 plant individuals and calculated the mean.

      Color Measurements and Modeling

      Flowers and inflorescences were collected and brought to the lab for spectral measurements. Spectral measurements were performed by measuring a small (c. 5 mm2) area of a given plant organ that was mounted on black insulation tape. All measurements were performed with either a USB2000 spectrometer equipped with a DH2000 BAL light source, or a JAZ spectrometer unit equipped with a pulsed Xenon light source (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, United States). The spectrometers were calibrated against a white standard (WS-1-SL, Ocean Optics). Measurements were performed with a bifurcated fiber optics probe, with the incident and measuring angle set at 45° with respect to the surface normal, following standard protocols (Chittka and Kevan, 2005). A single measurement was performed on each plant individual. For single flowers or unicolored compound inflorescences (e.g., in Knautia, Scabiosa, and Valeriana) we measured the region of the most prominent flower organ, facing the viewing direction of the visitor (usually the upper surface of a petal). For plants with multicolored flowers or inflorescences, e.g., many Asteraceae, we only measured the part of the inflorescence that occupied the majority (>50%) of the surface (usually the upper surface of the petal lips). Since the flowers of dioecious species (Salix sp.) differ in their reward and appearance and are found on different plant individuals, they were treated as independent data points in our analysis. In Trifolium pratense, two distinct color morphs with a general form appearing pink for a human observer, and an alpine form appearing white, were measured and treated as independent data points. In both cases, nectar and color measurements were also performed separately. Several specimens (between 1 and 127, median 3) were measured per species, depending on availability of the flowers.

      To estimate how bee visitors perceive the flower color, we used the color hexagon (Chittka, 1992), a bee-specific color space that is widely employed in pollinator studies and has been repeatedly tested in laboratory settings (Chittka et al., 1992; Giurfa et al., 1995; Raine and Chittka, 2005; Théry et al., 2005; Dyer et al., 2008, 2012; Leonard et al., 2011). Color loci were calculated according to standard procedures (Chittka and Kevan, 2005) using standard illumination (D65; Wyszecki and Stiles, 1982) and photoreceptor spectral sensitivity functions specific for Bombus terrestris (Skorupski et al., 2007). Hymenopteran photoreceptor sensitivities are phylogenetically conserved and similar among bee species (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001) and were confirmed to be similar across bumble bee species in particular (Peitsch et al., 1992; Briscoe and Chittka, 2001; Skorupski et al., 2007; Skorupski and Chittka, 2010). We used an average reflection spectrum of green foliage as adaptation background (Chittka and Kevan, 2005). For each measurement, we determined the (absolute) green receptor contrast, the position of the locus in the color space, and the color contrast as the Euclidean distance between the hexagon center and the color locus (Spaethe et al., 2001; Chittka and Kevan, 2005). Brightness, considered as the summed response of all three photoreceptors, is used by bees only during phototactic response (Menzel and Greggers, 1985) and is not regarded as an important spectral feature during foraging (Ng et al., 2018).

      Color hue refers to the direction of a locus in the color space and was calculated as the angle between the lines connecting the hexagon center with the blue corner (set as 0°) and the color locus, respectively. Color locus angles are reported as positive values in the clockwise direction with respect to the reference line (see Figure 1A). However, it must be noted that although several studies used angles as a measure for hue, there are no universally accepted standards on how to report it. Different studies used different reference lines and rotation directions (Chittka et al., 1994; Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2014 vs. Tai et al., 2020). For further analysis, the hues (angles) were binned to six categories, which correspond to distinct classes of reflectance functions (Chittka et al., 1994). The categories are referred to as “blue” (B; 330°–30° in the hexagon space), “blue-green” (BG; 30°–90°), “green” (G; 90°–150°), “green-UV” (GU; 150°–210°), “ultraviolet” (UV; 210°–270°), and “UV-blue” (UB; 270°–330°). In addition, we also plotted the hues to a finer scale of 10° bins to make the data comparable to other studies, which used this bin size (e.g., Chittka et al., 1994; Dyer et al., 2012; Shrestha et al., 2014). Flower species with color contrasts < 0.1 hexagon units were assigned as “achromatic” and excluded from further analysis (N = 3 species, see below).

      Flower colors of alpine flowering plants. Mean color loci of bumble bee visited plant species for which full nectar data and spectral measurements were available (N = 105). (A) Color loci plotted in the hexagon color space. (B,C) Frequency of samples in color categories at a rough (B) and fine (C) scale (B, blue; B-G, blue-green; G, green; G–U, green-UV; UV, UV; UV-B, UV-blue). Colors assigned to the categories are for illustrative purposes and are not intended to reflect human or bee-specific perception. The convention for hexagon angle measurement is indicated in panel (A).

      Phylogenetic Reconstruction and Signal

      To test for phylogenetic signal in the color and nectar reward data, we constructed a species-level phylogenetic tree, including all of our studied taxa. We initially used the phylogenetic tree (“ALLOTB” tree) published by Smith and Brown (2018). Tree manipulation was performed in R (version 4.1.1; R Development Core Team, 2021) using the packages “phytools” for R (Version 0.7-80; Revell, 2012), “ape” for R (Version 5.5; Paradis and Schliep, 2019), and “picante” for R (Version 1.8.2; Kembel et al., 2010). The tree was pruned to include only those taxa contained in our dataset. For two taxa which include distinct color morphs (Trifolium pratense) or different sexes (Salix waldsteiniana) of a single species, we introduced a dichotomy with branch length zero. Multitomies in the tree were resolved using the function “multi2di” in the “ape” package. The final tree used in our analysis can be found in the Supplementary Material.

      To test whether color traits, number of flowers/inflorescence or nectar reward of the study species show phylogenetic signal, we calculated Pagel’s λ (Pagel, 1999). For continuous traits, λ calculation and significance tests were performed in the “phytools” package. Since the distribution of nectar reward quantities (i.e., sucrose equivalents) was significantly different from a normal distribution (p < 0.05; Shapiro-Wilks test), all values were log10-transformed for the analysis. For color category, λ was calculated using the “fitDiscrete” function in the “geiger” package. Significant difference between the fitted model and the null model (λ = 0.00; no phylogenetic signal) was tested using a log-likelihood ratio test.

      Data Analysis

      To test whether plant species are uniformly distributed among the color categories, we used a Chi-square test, followed by an analysis of the standardized residuals (Sharpe, 2015). To test whether nectar reward quantity differed significantly between color categories, we performed phylogenetic ANOVA using the package “geiger” for R (Version 2.0.7; Pennell et al., 2014), using log10-transformed nectar values as dependent and hexagon color category as independent variable. Our dataset contained a single species in the “UV”-category (Crepis aurea), which was removed prior to the ANOVA. Independent analyses were performed for the maximum and mean nectar reward and for the “open” and “bagged” treatments. Significant results in the omnibus test were followed by a post-hoc test, comparing all possible combinations and adjusting the p-level using the Bonferroni method.

      Whether nectar reward quantity differed significantly between “open” and “bagged” flowers was tested using a paired t-test. To test the relationship between nectar reward quantity and visual and other traits we used phylogenetic generalized linear mixed models (PGLMM) with a Gaussian distribution. The model included log10-transformed nectar reward data as response variable, species (both as phylogenetic and non-phylogenetic covariate) as random factor, and color contrast, green contrast, brightness and the number of flowers per inflorescence as continuous covariates. All continuous variables were scaled prior to model preparation to facilitate interpretation of the effect sizes (Schielzeth, 2010). Separate models were calculated for each combination of nectar data (maximum & mean) and treatment (open and bagged). Model calculation was performed using the “pglmm” function in the “phyr” package for R (Version 1.1.0.; Li et al., 2020). All analyses were performed using the base version of R (Version 4.1.1; R Development Core Team, 2021) and the cited packages.

      Results Plant Sampling

      We obtained full nectar (both open and bagged flowers) data and spectral measurements from 108 samples. Three species with color contrasts < 0.1 hexagon units were excluded from further analysis (Pedicularis recutita, Salix hastata female, and Vaccinium myrtillus). The remaining 105 samples constituted 103 unique species with two additional samples from a second color morph (Trifolium pratense) and a second sex of a dioecious species (Salix waldsteiniana).

      Of the 112 flowering plant species for which bumble bee visits have been recorded in the study region (local communities of Rauris, Fusch and Heiligenblut above 1,400 m a.s.l.), the analyzed sample comprises 103 species, which accounted for the vast majority (98 %) of all recorded bumble bee visits (N = 4,070) in that region.

      Color Distribution

      The mean color loci of the samples were not distributed uniformly in the color space (Figure 1A), and the distribution of plant colors among the color categories differed significantly from a uniform distribution (N = 105, Chi2 = 125.46, p < 0.05). The majority of samples were found in the “blue-green” category (N = 54), followed by the “blue” category (N = 30; Figure 1B). Analysis of the standardized residuals indicated that the observed frequencies in these two categories are significantly higher than expected by chance, while the categories “UV”, “UV-blue,” and “green-UV” had significantly less observations than expected. Color frequency distribution, when analyzed at a finer scale, showed a pronounced peak at 60°, which corresponds to the central part of the “blue-green” sector in the hexagon (Figure 1). Phylogenetic signal for flower color, calculated as Pagel’s λ was estimated to be λ = 0.87, a value significantly different from λ = 0.00 (p < 0.05; Table 1 and Figure 2).

      Phylogenetic signal for measured traits and rewards in bumble bee visited plant species from the Eastern Alps.

      Trait λ pλ = 0
      log10 (bagged flower maximum) 0.47 <0.05
      log10 (open flower maximum) 0.57 <0.05
      log10 (bagged flower mean) 0.43 <0.05
      log10 (open flower mean) 0.56 <0.05
      Color contrast 0.07 0.38
      Green contrast 0.47 <0.05
      Brightness 0.38 <0.05
      Color category 0.87 <0.05
      Flowers/inflorescence 0.14 <0.05

      Phylogenetic reconstruction of the studied plant species. Phylogenetic tree of the study species, based on the phylogenetic “ALLOTB” tree by Smith and Brown (2018). Colored circles refer to hexagon color category. Scale bar for branch length indicates divergence time in million years.

      Reward Quantity

      Nectar reward quantity (expressed in mg sucrose per flower) varied among species and treatment (Supplementary Table 1). Bagging had a significant effect on the measured reward quantity; flowers shielded from visitors had a significantly higher reward quantity than flowers which could be depleted by visitors (maximum nectar reward: t(104) = 5.57, p < 0.05; mean nectar reward: t(104) = 6.88, p < 0.05). Pagel’s λ for the reward per flowers showed significant phylogenetic signal both in the mean and maximum value and in the “bagged” and “open” treatments (Table 1).

      Reward and Color Category

      Reward quantity of bagged flowers differed significantly between the color categories for the maximum nectar reward values [Phylogenetic ANOVA: F(4,99) = 3.51, p < 0.05; Figure 3A], but just failed significance for the mean nectar reward values [F(4,99) = 2.94, p = 0.07; Supplementary Figure 1A]. While the individual flowers in the “blue” category had, on average, higher reward quantities than in the other categories, pair-wise post hoc comparison did not identify significant differences between any of the combinations after Bonferroni correction.

      Nectar standing crop of alpine flowering plants (individual flowers). Log10-transformed maximum reward quantity, expressed as mg sucrose equivalent per flower for bumble bee visited plant species (N = 105). Reward quantity was measured for (A) bagged flowers and (B) open flowers. Box-plots indicate the median (line) and interquartile range (IQR, i.e., Q25-Q75; box). Lower and upper whiskers indicate Q25-1.5*IQR and Q75+1.5*IQR, respectively. The horizontal dashed line indicates the overall mean. Individual data points have been added with random X-axis jitter. X-axis categories are the five hexagon categories used in the analysis (see Figure 1; UV-B, UV-blue; B, blue; B-G, blue-green; G, green; G-UV, green-UV). For statistics, see text.

      Reward quantities were generally smaller in open flowers. Their distribution across color categories did not differ significantly among categories for the maximum nectar reward values [F(4,99) = 2.18, p = 0.20; Figure 3B] and for the mean nectar reward values [F(4,99) = 2.26, p = 0.18; Supplementary Figure 1B]. When we extrapolated the nectar reward quantity to the entire functional unit (inflorescence), quantities did not differ significantly for both maximum [bagged: F(4,99) = 3.37, p = 0.32; open: F(4,99) = 1.84, p = 0.56; Figure 4] and mean [bagged: F(4,99) = 2.81, p = 0.49; open: F(4,99) = 1.97, p = 0.59; Supplementary Figure 2] quantity measures.

      Nectar standing crop of alpine flowering plants (inflorescences). Log10-transformed maximum reward quantity, expressed as mg sucrose equivalent per inflorescence for bumble bee visited plant species (N = 105). Reward quantity was measured for (A) bagged inflorescences and (B) and open inflorescences. Box-plots indicate the median (line) and interquartile range (IQR, i.e., Q25-Q75; box). Lower and upper whiskers indicate Q25-1.5*IQR and Q75+1.5*IQR, respectively. The horizontal dashed line indicates the overall mean. Individual data points have been added with random X-axis jitter. X-axis categories are the five hexagon categories used in the analysis (see Figure 1; UV-B, UV-blue; B, blue; B-G, blue-green; G, green; G-UV, green-UV). For statistics, see text.

      Phylogenetic generalized linear mixed models for maximum (Tables 2, 3) and mean (Supplementary Tables 2, 3) nectar reward quantities identified a strong influence of species, followed by a smaller effect of the phylogeny-corrected species term. In the fixed effects, we identified equally strong negative effects of color contrast and the number of flowers per inflorescence, as well as small, non-significant effects of all other tested variables (Tables 2, 3 and Supplementary Tables 2, 3). In other words, nectar reward quantity correlated negatively with color contrast (i.e., flowers with higher color contrast contained less nectar) and flower number (i.e., inflorescences with fewer flowers had more nectar per flower).

      PGLMM for maximum nectar reward of single bagged flowers.

      Parameter Variance SD Estimate SE Z P
      Maximum nectar reward (n = 105)
      *Species 0.334 0.578
      *Species_ 0.068 0.261
      Color contrast −0.248 0.091 −2.74 <0.05
      Fl/inflorescence −0.214 0.088 −2.42 <0.05
      Green contrast 0.015 0.102 0.15 0.88
      Brightness 0.059 0.099 0.60 0.55

      *Denotes terms that were entered as random factors; _indicates that a phylogenetic covariance matrix was used in the random term. Continuous parameters were scaled before model generation.

      PGLMM for maximum nectar reward data of single open flowers.

      Parameter Variance SD Estimate SE Z P
      Maximum nectar reward (n = 105)
      *Species 0.238 0.488
      *Species_ 0.075 0.274
      Color contrast −0.164 0.080 −2.04 <0.05
      Fl/Inflorescence −0.184 0.078 −2.37 <0.05
      Green contrast 0.031 0.091 0.34 0.73
      Brightness −0.070 0.088 −0.80 0.42

      *Denotes terms that were entered as random factors; _indicates that a phylogenetic covariance matrix was used in the random term. Continuous parameters were scaled before model generation.

      Discussion

      In our study, we investigated the visual properties and the reward quantity of bumble bee visited flowering plants in an alpine environment. We found significant structure in the color signals, i.e., plant colors were not uniformly distributed across the color categories in a bee-specific color space. The reward quantity differed between the categories in bagged and open single flowers (although the latter was not significant) with higher average rewards in the “blue” and “blue-green” category. For entire flowering units this difference vanished. We hypothesize that naïve bumble bees, when visiting flowers of innately preferred colors, will find on average more reward per flower, although this effect was weak and almost disappeared when flowers were unbagged.

      Color

      Our analysis showed that the color loci of bumble bee visited flowers were scattered throughout the color hexagon, resulting in a variety of hues (angles) and chromatic contrasts to the background (distance to hexagon center). Interestingly, only three species appeared achromatic to bees. Achromatic cues are difficult to detect under natural conditions, and bees may not utilize them for flower detection and identification (Ng et al., 2018). The number of flowers found in each of the major bee-color categories (sensu Chittka et al., 1994) differed significantly from a uniform distribution, with the majority falling into the “blue-green” sector. This pattern, as well as that obtained when analyzed at a finer resolution (Figure 1C), showed remarkable similarity with data from other habitats and locations, e.g., Germany (Giurfa et al., 1995), Australia (Dyer et al., 2012), Nepal (Shrestha et al., 2014), New Zealand (Bischoff et al., 2013), and Taiwan (Tai et al., 2020). Interpretation and comparison of the distribution is problematic as it may depend on sampling strategy, habitat type, pollinator species and the choice of visual system selected for the color modeling (for a discussion, see Shrestha et al., 2019). For instance, most of the above-mentioned studies either combined samples from large regions rather than local communities and/or did not consider the pollinator composition.

      Both abiotic factors and biotic factors are assumed to influence the flower color distribution. Previous studies demonstrated influences of e.g., day length and precipitation (Arista et al., 2013), soil composition (Horovitz, 1976), and vacuole pH (Grotewold, 2006). For the alpine environment, selection pressures for adaptations to cope with temperature extremes and high irradiance are likely to influence the observed color frequencies (van der Kooi et al., 2019; Dalrymple et al., 2020).

      For biotic selection pressures, color frequency differences have been hypothesized as resulting from selection by different pollinator assemblages, but (experimental) proof for this hypothesis is rare. Recently, data from regions that lack bees (Maquarie Island; Shrestha et al., 2016) or social bees (New Zealand; Ishii et al., 2019), show different plant color distributions and thus provide support for this hypothesis. For regions with highly overlapping visitor spectra, like the Alps, comparing color frequencies as function of pollinator group is more complex. Plant-visitor networks showed considerable overlap of visitor groups for most investigated plant species in this ecosystem (Lefebvre et al., 2018). While it can be assumed that many of the flower visitors also serve as pollinators, experimental proof of the actual pollinator identity and its share in the overall pollination of most generalist plant species is largely lacking. These bits of information are, however, crucial in understanding pollinator-mediated selection on traits like e.g., color, since the strength of selection can be assumed to critically depend on the pollination efficiency of the different pollinators of a plant species (Trunschke et al., 2021). To better understand the origin of the color frequency distribution that we observed in our study, we need further detailed information about the base line in the entire community (i.e., spectral reflectance data from all of the c. 400 flowering plant species that occur in the region) and quality information about the predominant pollinator(s) for each of them, which will be a challenge for future generations of pollination ecologists.

      Nectar

      Previous studies, which attempted to link flower color with reward quantity, either used literature data only (Giurfa et al., 1995), extrapolated nectar production rates from short measurement sequences to the entire day (Chittka et al., 2004; Raine and Chittka, 2007a), or measured sugar content of the nectar standing crop of bagged flowers only (Shrestha et al., 2020). In our study, we measured the nectar production capacity of the species (bagged flowers) as well as the nectar standing crop of open flowers, as a more direct measure of what is actually available during a typical day. As expected, the nectar standing crop was lower than the production capacity, due to depletion by visitors. Nectar reward quantities were more similar between color categories in open flowers, suggesting that those flowers that produce more nectar are preferentially depleted in the field under normal conditions. It is unclear whether these higher visitation rates originate initially from random or targeted visits of flower visitors, which have learned that certain flowers are more rewarding than others (Goulson et al., 2007).

      Visitor Color Preferences and the Correlation With Nectar

      Visual signals are used by flowering plants to convey information about their species identity and allow for easier detection in the usually cluttered visual environment. They can hold information about the reward or promote learning of the association between floral traits and the reward. Flower spectral reflectance is a complex mixture of different qualities that can be employed by the bee visual system separately or in combination. These qualities involve color contrast (contrast between the background and the flower color), achromatic contrast (modulation of the green receptor channel), brightness (the sum of the three photoreceptor excitations) and color hue. Aside from brightness, which is sometimes used in bee vision studies but has not been shown to be of importance for bees (Spaethe et al., 2001; Ng et al., 2018) all other signals and cues have been found to be relevant in bee foraging. Color contrast correlates with detection speed (Spaethe et al., 2001; Streinzer et al., 2009) and bees are known to prefer flowers of higher contrast when given a choice (Rohde et al., 2013). Achromatic contrast is used in object detection (Giurfa et al., 1996; Dyer et al., 2008), but is probably not or only rarely used as a sole cue in flower detection (Martínez-Harms et al., 2010; Lunau et al., 2011; Ng et al., 2018). Finally, hue is employed by bees to identify different reflectance spectra independent of lighting conditions and other visual traits (Reser et al., 2012). Bumble bees can learn to discriminate very small differences in hue when trained appropriately (Dyer and Chittka, 2004), but in the real world, such fine discrimination ability is probably of little value, given the existing variation of flower color within species, which sometimes overlaps with color of other species (Jersáková et al., 2016; Garcia et al., 2020).

      Flower visiting insect have been shown to have (species-specific) innate preferences for certain colors (Lunau and Maier, 1995) which have been interpreted to help them find rewarding flowers more quickly during their first foraging flights. In a field study with Bombus terrestris, Raine and Chittka (2007b) first determined the strength of the innate preference for “UV-blue” and then let them forage in the surrounding, where UV-blue flowers were also the most rewarding ones. They found a significantly higher colony-level success of colonies that showed a strong innate color preference, indicating that these preferences may be adaptive if color correlates with reward quantity. Due to the large variation of rewards in our study, color cannot be considered as a reliable signal for nectar reward quantity. After sampling the vast majority of plant species visited by bumble bees in our study region, we, however, found that flowers with colors from the preferred color categories do have, on average, higher reward production at the single flower level (Figure 3A). Smaller differences have also been found for open flowers (Figure 3B) and when comparing entire inflorescences (Figure 4), though the differences were statistically not significant. We thus conclude that color may be a weak (but honest) indicator for reward quantity, and that this overall (small) advantage may indeed allow the bees to increase foraging success, compared with an entirely random search. Similar correlations between the innately preferred color categories of bees and the reward quantity were also found in Central Europe (Giurfa et al., 1995; Raine and Chittka, 2005), but not e.g., in Australia (Shrestha et al., 2020). While in the European region, social bees are assumed to have a large share in the overall pollination of entomophilous plants, in the Australian communities, (social) bees are not the major pollinator guild. Furthermore, pollinator/visitor identity was not investigated in that study, which limits the comparison with our results.

      For a complete understanding of how strongly innate color preferences affect flower color in the Alps, we must know the relative contribution of all flower visitors of a plant species to its pollination success, and to analyze in detail whether different nectar traits (like volume, concentration, sugar content, and sugar composition) differ between major visitor groups and color categories. Our study surprisingly showed a negative correlation between the color contrast and nectar reward, which seems to stand in contrast to the observation that bumble bees prefer flowers of high color contrast (Rohde et al., 2013). However, some previous studies found contrasting results regarding the relationship between color contrast and reward quantity (Kantsa et al., 2017; Shrestha et al., 2020). Color contrast is a highly variable trait in flower communities (Garcia et al., 2021) and it is currently not known how bees use this visual feature while foraging in natural environments.

      Interestingly, we found no statistical difference of nectar rewards among color categories when we calculated the nectar reward of the entire inflorescence (Figure 4 and Supplementary Figure 2). Flower and floral display size have been shown to correlate with nectar reward quantity and may constitute an honest signal of reward quantity for potential visitors (Ortiz et al., 2021). In our study, we found a significant negative relationship between nectar reward quantity of individual flowers and flower number of an inflorescence. From a plant’s perspective, grouping several flowers with smaller reward quantity to larger units would constitute a strategy to attract more potential pollinators due to a larger display size (Spaethe et al., 2001; Wertlen et al., 2008) and thus promote learning through higher reward quantities that can be gathered during a single visit. While larger inflorescences may provide a larger total amount of reward, the energy and time needed to collect these rewards must also be considered in foraging economics (Harder et al., 2001). In future studies, one will need to investigate in more detail how the interplay between nectar reward of individual flowers, variation of inflorescence size (e.g., number of flowers) and spatial distribution of plant individuals within a population affect the foraging economics and color preferences of bumble bees (Geslin et al., 2014).

      Conclusion

      In an alpine community, investigating the majority of flowering plants that are confirmed to be visited by bumble bees, we found evidence that flower color may serve as a weak predictor for reward quantity. Since flowers of innately preferred colors produce either higher, or at least not smaller, reward quantities compared to less favored colors, naïve bumble bees may increase their foraging success by visiting flowers of such categories. Also, experienced foragers may also profit by visiting these flowers, e.g., when previously rewarding flowers become depleted or flowering season has ended. Although our study contributes to a better understanding of the origin and adaptiveness of color preferences of flower visitors, future studies are necessary to gather more quality data on pollination efficiency of the different flower visitors and thus their respective selection force on flower color.

      Data Availability Statement

      The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

      Author Contributions

      JS, JN, and MS designed the study and collected the data. MS analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript writing and editing.

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher’s Note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      Funding

      Nectar data were collected in part during projects supported by Austrian National Park fund (1994) and Glockner Öko-Fonds (2012).

      We would like to thank S. Dötterl for spectral measurements of some flowers, S. Streinzer for help with data preparation, and J. Plant for linguistic improvements. We further wish to express our thanks to the Großglockner-Hochalpenstraßen AG for the gratuitous use of the Großglocker high alpine road and the museum “Haus der Natur” (Salzburg) for the opportunity to use the Eberhard-Stüber alpine research station. Parts of the data were collected during field courses of the University of Vienna. We would also like to thank all participating students. P. Pilsl provided estimates of flowering plant number in the region.

      Supplementary Material

      The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fevo.2021.721241/full#supplementary-material

      References Arista M. Talavera M. Berjano R. Ortiz P. L. (2013). Abiotic factors may explain the geographical distribution of flower colour morphs and the maintenance of colour polymorphism in the scarlet pimpernel. J. Ecol. 101 16131622. 10.1111/1365-2745.12151 Bertsch A. (1983). Nectar production of Epilobium angustifolium L. at different air humidities; nectar sugar in individual flowers and the optimal foraging theory. Oecologia 59 4048. 10.1007/BF00388069 25024144 Bingham R. A. Orthner A. R. Takita T. (1998). Efficient pollination of alpine plants. Nature 391 238239. 10.1111/j.1365-2990.2004.00503.x Bischoff M. Lord J. M. Robertson A. W. Dyer A. G. (2013). Hymenopteran pollinators as agents of selection on flower colour in the New Zealand mountains: salient chromatic signals enhance flower discrimination. N. Z. J. Bot. 51 181193. 10.1080/0028825X.2013.806933 Briscoe A. D. Chittka L. (2001). The evolution of color vision in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46 471510. 10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.471 11112177 Chittka L. (1992). The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170 533543. 10.1007/BF00199331 Chittka L. (1996). Does bee color vision predate the evolution of flower color? Naturwissenschaften 83 136138. 10.1007/s001140050263 Chittka L. Kevan P. G. (2005). “Flower colour as advertisement,” in Practical Pollination Biology, eds Dafni A. Kevan P. G. Husband B. C. (Cambridge, ONT: Enviroquest Ltd), 157230. Chittka L. Beier W. Hertel H. Steinmann E. Menzel R. (1992). Opponent color coding is a universal strategy to evaluate the photoreceptor inputs in Hymenoptera. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170 545563. Chittka L. Ings T. C. Raine N. E. (2004). Chance and adaptation in the evolution of island bumblebee behaviour. Popul. Ecol. 46 243251. 10.1007/s10144-004-0180-1 Chittka L. Shmida A. Troje N. Menzel R. (1994). Ultraviolet as a component of flower reflections, and the colour perception of Hymenoptera. Vis. Res. 34 14891508. 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90151-1 Corbet S. A. (2003). Nectar sugar content: estimating standing crop and secretion rate in the field. Apidologie 34 110. 10.1051/apido:2002049 Corbet S. A. Unwin D. M. Prŷs-Jones O. E. (1979). Humidity, nectar and insect visits to flowers, with special reference to Crataegus, Tilia and Echium. Ecol. Entomol 4 922. 10.1111/j.1365-2311.1979.tb00557.x Cruden R. W. Hermann S. M. (1983). “Studying nectar? Some observations on the art,” in The Biology Of Nectaries, eds Bentley B. L. Elias T. S. (New York, NY: Columbia University Press), 223242. Dalrymple R. L. Kemp D. J. Flores-Moreno H. Laffan S. W. White T. E. Hemmings F. A. (2020). Macroecological patterns in flower colour are shaped by both biotic and abiotic factors. New Phytol. 228 19721985. 10.1111/nph.16737 32533864 Dyer A. G. Chittka L. (2004). Fine colour discrimination requires differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften 91 224227. 10.1007/s00114-004-0508-x 15146269 Dyer A. G. Boyd-Gerny S. McLoughlin S. Rosa M. G. P. Simonov V. Wong B. B. M. (2012). Parallel evolution of angiosperm colour signals: common evolutionary pressures linked to hymenopteran vision. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279 36063615. 10.1098/rspb.2012.0827 22673351 Dyer A. G. Boyd-Gerny S. Shrestha M. Lunau K. Garcia J. E. Koethe S. (2016). Innate colour preferences of the Australian native stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria Sm. J. Comp. Physiol. A 202 603613. 10.1007/s00359-016-1101-4 27316718 Dyer A. G. Spaethe J. Prack S. (2008). Comparative psychophysics of bumblebee and honeybee colour discrimination and object detection. J. Comp. Physiol. A 194 617627. 10.1007/s00359-008-0335-1 18437390 Ebmer P. A. W. (2003). Die Höhenverbreitung der Bienen, ausgenommen Hummeln, im Nationalpark Hohe Tauern, Kärnten. Beitr. Entomofaunistik 4 160164. Fischer M. A. Oswald K. Adler W. (2008). Exkursionsflora für Österreich, Liechtenstein, Südtirol, 3rd Edn. Linz: Biologiezentrum der Oberösterreichischen Landesmuseen. Garcia J. E. Dyer A. G. Burd M. Shrestha M. (2021). Flower colour and size signals differ depending on geographical location and altitude region. Plant Biol. 23 905914. 10.1111/plb.13326 34546624 Garcia J. E. Phillips R. D. Peter C. I. Dyer A. G. (2020). Changing how biologists view flowers—color as a perception not a trait. Front. Plant Sci. 11:601700. 10.3389/fpls.2020.601700 33329670 Geslin B. Baude M. Dajoz I. (2014). Effect of local spatial plant distribution and conspecific density on bumble bee foraging behaviour. Ecol. Entomol. 39 334342. 10.1111/een.12106 Giurfa M. Núñez J. Chittka L. Menzel R. (1995). Color preferences of flower-naive honeybees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 177 247259. Giurfa M. Vorobyev M. Kevan P. Menzel R. (1996). Detection of coloured stimuli by honeybees: minimum visual angles and receptor specific contrasts. J. Comp. Physiol. A 178 699709. 10.1007/BF00227381 Goulson D. Cruise J. L. Sparrow K. R. Harris A. J. Park K. J. Tinsley M. C. (2007). Choosing rewarding flowers; perceptual limitations and innate preferences influence decision making in bumblebees and honeybees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61 15231529. 10.1007/s00265-007-0384-4 Goyret J. Pfaff M. Raguso R. A. Kelber A. (2008). Why do Manduca sexta feed from white flowers? Innate and learnt colour preferences in a hawkmoth. Naturwissenschaften 95 569576. 10.1007/s00114-008-0350-7 18288469 Grotewold E. (2006). The genetics and biochemistry of floral pigments. Annu. Rev. Plant Biol. 57 761780. 10.1146/annurev.arplant.57.032905.105248 16669781 Gumbert A. (2000). Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus terrestris): innate preferences and generalization after learning. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48 3643. 10.1007/s002650000213 Harder L. D. (1986). Effects of nectar concentration and flower depth on flower handling efficiency of bumble bees. Oecologia 69 309315. 10.1007/BF00377639 28311376 Harder L. D. Williams N. M. Jordan C. Y. Nelson W. A. (2001). “The effects of floral design and display on pollinator economics and pollen dispersal,” in Cognitive Ecology of Pollination: Animal Behaviour and Floral Evolution, eds Thomson J. D. Chittka L. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 297317. 10.1017/CBO9780511542268.016 Horovitz A. (1976). Edaphic factors and flower colour distribution in the Anemoneae (Ranunculaceae). Plant Syst. Evol. 126 239242. 10.1007/BF00983363 Ishii H. S. Kubota M. X. Tsujimoto S. G. Kudo G. (2019). Association between community assemblage of flower colours and pollinator fauna: a comparison between Japanese and New Zealand alpine plant communities. Ann. Bot. 123 533541. 10.1093/aob/mcy188 30380008 Jersáková J. Spaethe J. Streinzer M. Neumayer J. Paulus H. Dötterl S. (2016). Does Traunsteinera globosa (the globe orchid) dupe its pollinators through generalized food deception or mimicry? Bot. J. Linn. Soc. 180 269294. 10.1111/boj.12364 Kantsa A. Raguso R. A. Dyer A. G. Sgardelis S. P. Olesen J. M. Petanidou T. (2017). Community-wide integration of floral colour and scent in a Mediterranean scrubland. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1 15021510. 10.1038/s41559-017-0298-0 29185514 Kearns C. A. Inouye D. W. (1993). Techniques For Pollination Biologists. Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado. Kelber A. Osorio D. (2010). From spectral information to animal colour vision: experiments and concepts. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277 16171625. 10.1098/rspb.2009.2118 20164101 Kembel S. W. Cowan P. D. Helmus M. R. Cornwell W. K. Morlon H. Ackerly D. D. (2010). Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics 26 14631464. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq166 20395285 Lefebvre V. Villemant C. Fontaine C. Daugeron C. (2018). Altitudinal, temporal and trophic partitioning of flower-visitors in Alpine communities. Sci. Rep. 8:4706. 10.1038/s41598-018-23210-y 29549294 Leonard A. S. Dornhaus A. Papaj D. R. (2011). Flowers help bees cope with uncertainty: signal detection and the function of floral complexity. J. Exp. Biol. 214 113121. 10.1242/jeb.047407 21147975 Li D. Dinnage R. Nell L. A. Helmus M. R. Ives A. R. (2020). phyr: an R package for phylogenetic species-distribution modelling in ecological communities. Methods Ecol. Evol. 11 14551463. 10.1111/2041-210X.13471 Lunau K. Maier E. J. (1995). Innate colour preferences of flower visitors. J. Comp. Physiol. A 177 119. 10.1007/BF00243394 Lunau K. Wacht S. (1997). Innate flower recognition in the hoverfly Eristalis tenax L. Mitt. Dtsch. Ges. Allg. Angew. Entomol. 11 481484. Lunau K. Papiorek S. Eltz T. Sazima M. (2011). Avoidance of achromatic colours by bees provides a private niche for hummingbirds. J. Exp. Biol. 214 16071612. 10.1242/jeb.052688 21490268 Martínez-Harms J. Palacios A. G. Márquez N. Estay P. Arroyo M. T. K. K. Mpodozis J. (2010). Can red flowers be conspicuous to bees? Bombus dahlbomii and South American temperate forest flowers as a case in point. J. Exp. Biol. 213 564571. 10.1242/jeb.037622 20118307 Menzel R. Greggers U. (1985). Natural phototaxis and its relationship to colour vision in honeybees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 157 311321. 10.1007/BF00618121 Ng L. Garcia J. E. Dyer A. G. (2018). Why colour is complex: evidence that bees perceive neither brightness nor green contrast in colour signal processing. Facets 3 800817. 10.1139/facets-2017-0116 Ortiz P. L. Fernández-Díaz P. Pareja D. Escudero M. Arista M. (2021). Do visual traits honestly signal floral rewards at community level? Funct. Ecol 35 369383. 10.1111/1365-2435.13709 Pagel M. (1999). Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401 877884. Paradis E. Schliep K. (2019). ape 5.0: an environment for modern phylogenetics and evolutionary analyses in R. Bioinformatics 35 526528. Peitsch D. Fietz A. Hertel H. de Souza J. Ventura D. F. Menzel R. (1992). The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 170 2340. 10.1007/BF00190398 1573568 Pennell M. Eastman J. Slater G. Brown J. Uyeda J. Fitzjohn R. (2014). geiger v2.0: an expanded suite of methods for fitting macroevolutionary models to phylogenetic trees. Bioinformatics 30 22162218. R Development Core Team (2021). R: A Language And Environment For Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation statistical Computing. Raine N. E. Chittka L. (2005). Colour preferences in relation to the foraging performance and fitness of the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Uludag Bee J. 5 145150. Raine N. E. Chittka L. (2007b). The adaptive significance of sensory bias in a foraging context: floral colour preferences in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. PLoS One 2:e556. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000556 17579727 Raine N. E. Chittka L. (2007a). Mengen der Nektarerzeugung bei 75 von Hummeln besuchten Blumenarten in einem deutschen Pflanzenbestand (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombus terrestris). Entomol. Gen. 30 191192. 10.1127/entom.gen/30/2007/191 Reser D. H. Wijesekara Witharanage R. Rosa M. G. Dyer A. G. (2012). Honeybees (Apis mellifera) learn color discriminations via differential conditioning independent of long wavelength (green) photoreceptor modulation. PLoS One 7:e48577. 10.1371/journal.pone.0048577 23155394 Revell L. J. (2012). phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3 217223. Rohde K. Papiorek S. Lunau K. (2013). Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) and honeybees (Apis mellifera) prefer similar colours of higher spectral purity over trained colours. J. Comp. Physiol. A 199 197210. 10.1007/s00359-012-0783-5 23224278 Schielzeth H. (2010). Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1 103113. 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00012.x Sharpe D. (2015). Your chi-square test is statistically significant: now what? Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 20 110. 10.7275/tbfa-x148 Shrestha M. Dyer A. G. Bhattarai P. Burd M. (2014). Flower colour and phylogeny along an altitudinal gradient in the Himalayas of Nepal. J. Ecol. 102 126135. 10.1111/1365-2745.12185 Shrestha M. Dyer A. G. Garcia J. E. Burd M. (2019). Floral colour structure in two Australian herbaceous communities: it depends on who is looking. Ann. Bot. 124 221232. 10.1093/aob/mcz043 31008511 Shrestha M. Garcia J. E. Burd M. Dyer A. G. (2020). Australian native flower colours: does nectar reward drive bee pollinator flower preferences? PLoS One 15:e0226469. 10.1371/journal.pone.0226469 32525873 Shrestha M. Lunau K. Dorin A. Schulze B. Bischoff M. Burd M. (2016). Floral colours in a world without birds and bees: the plants of Macquarie Island. Plant Biol. 18 842850. 10.1111/plb.12456 27016399 Skorupski P. Chittka L. (2010). Photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in the bumblebee, Bombus impatiens (Hymenoptera: Apidae). PLoS One 5:e12049. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012049 20711523 Skorupski P. Chittka L. (2011). Is colour cognitive? Opt. Laser Technol. 43 251260. 10.1016/j.optlastec.2008.12.015 Skorupski P. Döring T. F. Chittka L. (2007). Photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in island and mainland populations of the bumblebee, Bombus terrestris. J. Comp. Physiol. A 193 485494. 10.1007/s00359-006-0206-6 17333207 Smith S. A. Brown J. W. (2018). Constructing a broadly inclusive seed plant phylogeny. Am. J. Bot. 105 302314. 10.1002/ajb2.1019 29746720 Spaethe J. Tautz J. Chittka L. (2001). Visual constraints in foraging bumblebees: flower size and color affect search time and flight behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98 38983903. 10.1073/pnas.071053098 11259668 Streinzer M. Paulus H. F. Spaethe J. (2009). Floral colour signal increases short-range detectability of a sexually deceptive orchid to its bee pollinator. J. Exp. Biol. 212 13651370. 10.1242/jeb.027482 19376957 Streinzer M. Roth N. Paulus H. F. Spaethe J. (2019). Color preference and spatial distribution of glaphyrid beetles suggest a key role in the maintenance of the color polymorphism in the peacock anemone (Anemone pavonina, Ranunculaceae) in Northern Greece. J. Comp. Physiol. A 205 735743. 10.1007/s00359-019-01360-2 31338578 Tai K.-C. Shrestha M. Dyer A. G. Yang E.-C. Wang C.-N. (2020). Floral color diversity: how are signals shaped by elevational gradient on the tropical-subtropical mountainous island of Taiwan? Front. Plant Sci. 11:582784. 10.3389/fpls.2020.582784 33391297 Théry M. Debut M. Gomez D. Casas J. (2005). Specific color sensitivities of prey and predator explain camouflage in different visual systems. Behav. Ecol. 16 2529. 10.1093/beheco/arh130 Trunschke J. Lunau K. Pyke G. H. Ren Z.-X. Wang H. (2021). Flower color evolution and the evidence of pollinator-mediated selection. Front. Plant Sci. 12:617851. 10.3389/fpls.2021.617851 34381464 van der Kooi C. J. Kevan P. G. Koski M. H. (2019). The thermal ecology of flowers. Ann. Bot. 124 343353. 10.1093/aob/mcz073 31206146 Wertlen A. M. Niggebrugge C. Vorobyev M. Hempel de Ibarra N. (2008). Detection of patches of coloured discs by bees. J. Exp. Biol. 211 21012104. 10.1242/jeb.014571 18552299 Wyszecki G. Stiles W. S. (1982). Color Science: Concepts And Methods, Quantitative Data And Formulae, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.eilie.com.cn
      lianpei.org.cn
      hisike.com.cn
      hsrjapps.com.cn
      www.qqgjhz.com.cn
      www.naisibo.com.cn
      two-l.net.cn
      www.rgchain.com.cn
      pzswkj.com.cn
      xfhypy.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p