Front. Ecol. Evol. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution Front. Ecol. Evol. 2296-701X Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/fevo.2019.00480 Ecology and Evolution Original Research Nonchalant Flight in Tiger Moths (Erebidae: Arctiinae) Is Correlated With Unpalatability Dowdy Nicolas J. 1 2 * Conner William E. 1 1Department of Biology, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC, United States 2Department of Zoology, Milwaukee Public Museum, Milwaukee, WI, United States

Edited by: Piotr Jablonski, Seoul National University, South Korea

Reviewed by: Changku Kang, Carleton University, Canada; Ryo Nakano, National Agriculture and Food Research Organization (NARO), Japan

*Correspondence: Nicolas J. Dowdy njdowdy@gmail.com

This article was submitted to Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution

16 12 2019 2019 7 480 22 08 2019 25 11 2019 Copyright © 2019 Dowdy and Conner. 2019 Dowdy and Conner

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Many aposematic animals are well-known to exhibit generally sluggish movements. However, less is known about their escape responses when under direct threat of predation. In this study, we characterize the anti-bat escape responses of 5 species of tiger moth (Erebidae: Arctiinae), a subfamily of Lepidoptera which possess ultrasound-sensitive ears. These ears act as an early-warning system which can detect the ultrasonic cries of nearby echolocating bats, allowing the moths to enact evasive flight behaviors in an effort to escape predation. We examine the role that unpalatability plays in predicting the likelihood that individuals of a given species will enact escape behaviors in response to predation. We hypothesized that more unpalatable species would be less likely to exhibit escape maneuvers (i.e., more nonchalant) than their less unpalatable counterparts. Our results demonstrate significant interspecific variation in the degree to which tiger moths utilize evasive flight behaviors to escape bat predators as well as in their degree of unpalatability. We provide evidence for the existence of a nonchalance continuum of anti-bat evasive flight response among tiger moths and show that species are arrayed along this continuum based on their relative unpalatability to bat predators. Relatively unpalatable prey more often exhibit nonchalant flight behaviors whereas palatable prey more often employ evasive dives. Our findings demonstrate that the degree to which certain animals are protected by potent chemical defenses can influence the likelihood that they will exhibit evasive escape behaviors. Further, we argue that the bat-moth predator-prey system is an ideal model for future studies of escape behaviors of prey which overcomes some of the limitations inherent to current model systems.

nonchalance escape behavior anti-predator defense palatability Lepidoptera DBI-1811897 IOS 0951160 National Science Foundation10.13039/100000001

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Aposematic animals have long been recognized to exhibit sluggish movements. Bates was the first to observe this, describing several strikingly colored butterflies as possessing “weak, slow flight” (Bates, 1862; Poulton, 1890). Wallace later wrote of the chemically defended, black-and-white striped skunk, “Its consciousness that it needs only to be seen to be avoided gives it that slowness of motion and fearlessness of aspect which are characteristic of most creatures so protected.” (Wallace, 1889). The phenomenon of sluggishness was later incorporated into the “chemical defense syndrome” (CDS), a general suite of characteristics commonly exhibited by chemically protected animals (Whitman et al., 1985). There are several hypotheses which seek to explain why aposematic animals exhibit these sluggish behaviors. One such hypothesis is that these slow movements evolved because they avoid triggering the attack responses of motion-oriented, ambush predators like frogs and praying mantids (Hatle and Faragher, 1998; Hatle et al., 2002). Another possibility is that these conspicuous, slow movements are themselves aposematic, acting as an additional signal of unprofitability to would-be predators. Expanding on Bates' early observations, a comparative study of butterfly species has quantitatively shown that distasteful species fly relatively more slowly than palatable species, indicating that sluggish flight can honestly signal prey defenses (i.e., “locomotor mimicry”; Chai and Srygley, 1990; Srygley, 1994, 2004). Another non-mutually exclusive hypothesis posits that these slow movements may make visual aposematic signals more apparent to predators, increasing the effectiveness of such signals (i.e., “raised exposure”; Speed et al., 2010).

      We reserve “sluggishness” as a description of general locomotory movements, with “more sluggish” behavior indicating slower, seemingly careless movements, as it has traditionally been defined (Hatle and Faragher, 1998; Hatle and Whitman, 2001). However, animals can alter their movements in different contexts. When animals are actively pursued by predators they will often exhibit escape maneuvers. We define a seemingly careless demeanor in response to an immediate threat as “nonchalant.” “More nonchalant” indicates slower, less complex, delayed, and/or more rarely enacted escape responses in response to a potential predatory threat (Dowdy and Conner, 2016). Because of inherent costs of fleeing, prey are expected to balance perceived predation risk with the costs of fleeing such that they initiate escape at a distance which maximizes their fitness at the end of an encounter with a predator (i.e., “Optimal Escape Model”; Ydenberg and Dill, 1986; Cooper and Frederick, 2007). Flight initiation distance (FID) is known to vary depending on a variety of factors (e.g., body condition, distance to refugia) which likely influence prey assessment of predation risk and/or the costs associated with escape (Stankowich and Blumstein, 2005; Samia et al., 2016; Cooper, 2018). Chemical defenses may be one such factor influencing FID, however the escape responses of chemically defended prey have not been well-studied, and it is not clear whether sluggish animals should be expected to remain so when directly threatened. To our knowledge, the escape responses of aposematic animals in response to the threat of predation have only been investigated among dendrobatid frogs. Generally, these studies have found that aposematic frogs retain escape responses but delay fleeing relative to cryptic species, though variation in FID exists between aposematic species and based on the context of the encounter (Cooper et al., 2009a,b; Ozel and Stynoski, 2011; Dugas et al., 2015; Blanchette et al., 2017). Unfortunately, due to methodological limitations, the results of these studies confound escape decisions based on predation avoidance with trample avoidance (i.e., avoidance of being crushed), limiting the usefulness of their conclusions.

      The bat-moth predator-prey system is an ideal system in which to study the escape behaviors of aposematic prey. Many moth species are equipped with ultrasound-sensitive ears which they use to detect bat predators. The ability of insects to detect the ultrasonic cries of bats led to their development of avoidance behaviors such as negative phonotaxis, spiraling erratic flight, and powered dives (Roeder, 1962). These maneuvers have been shown to be an effective means of dodging bat attacks (Acharya and Fenton, 1999; Triblehorn et al., 2008). Additionally, some moths possess secondary defenses which may alter their risk of predation. Many tiger moths (Erebidae: Arctiinae) sequester defensive toxins from their host plants and produce ultrasound in response to bat echolocation, advertising their unpalatability (i.e., “acoustic aposematism”; Acharya and Fenton, 1992; Dunning et al., 1992; Hristov and Conner, 2005; Barber et al., 2009; Dowdy and Conner, 2016). Unpalatability varies between species and may relate to the concentration and type of chemical compounds they have sequestered.

      Interestingly, some tiger moths have been noted to lack any significant evasive flight response to bat attacks, even though they possess and utilize ultrasonic hearing (Goldman and Henson, 1977; Acharya and Fenton, 1992; Dunning et al., 1992; Dowdy and Conner, 2016). Field experiments with certain species of tiger moths have uncovered variation in escape behaviors in response to bat attacks (Dowdy and Conner, 2016). Two sympatric tiger moth species, Pygarctia roseicapitis and Cisthene martini, differed in both the likelihood of enacting evasive dives as well as in their unpalatability. The more unpalatable C. martini was significantly more nonchalant, rarely performing diving escape maneuvers. In contrast, the less unpalatable P. roseicapitis was less nonchalant, diving much more frequently when attacked by bats, indicating that a nonchalance continuum among aposematic species may exist.

      In this study we examine the role that unpalatability plays in predicting the likelihood that individuals of a given species will enact escape behaviors in response to the threat of predation. Chemical defenses have not yet been recognized as a key factor affecting escape behaviors (Stankowich and Blumstein, 2005; Samia et al., 2016; Cooper, 2018). We hypothesized that more unpalatable tiger moth species would be less likely to exhibit escape maneuvers (i.e., more nonchalant) than their less unpalatable counterparts. This is the first study to comparatively examine the escape behaviors of aposematic insects in a natural context, compare the likelihood of escape between both aposematic and mimetic species, and construct a predictive model of escape behavior.

      Materials and Methods Ethics Statement

      Most of the data presented here involves free-flying bats in their natural habitats, apart from some additional palatability data for P. rosecapitis and C. martini. Bats included in palatability tests were captured and released under USFWS permit TE01690A-0. The methods of this study were approved by the Wake Forest University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol #A12-048). This work was performed with permission on private property.

      Field Site

      Field experiments were conducted at the Southwestern Research Station (SWRS) operated by the American Museum of Natural History. SWRS is located in Cochise County approximately 7 km southwest of Portal, Arizona, United States. The GPS coordinates of the field site are: 31°53'00.30” N 109°12'27.20” W; elevation: 1,650 m. This site was chosen for its high diversity of both bats and moths. The field trials were performed between July 18th and August 10th during 2011, 2012, and 2013.

      Moth Collection and Manipulation

      Moths were collected on station grounds from sheets illuminated with 15 Watt ultraviolet “quantum” lights (Leptraps.com; F15T8QBL). Moths identified as either Carales arizonensis, C. martini, Pygarctia murina, or P. roseicapitis were stored individually for up to 24 h in 30 mL plastic containers at ambient temperatures. Moths were randomly placed into one of three treatment groups: Tymbals Intact (T+), Tymbals Removed (T–), and Sham Control (S). Individuals from all three treatment groups were chilled for 5 min in an ice bath prior to surgery. Individuals assigned to the T+ group were removed from the ice bath and no further manipulations were performed. Individuals assigned to the T- group had their tymbal organs ablated with curved forceps by removing the cuticular surface of the organ. This ablation does not cause any discernable injury or loss of haemolymph. Impairment of sound production was verified by manipulating the individual while monitoring with an ultrasonic detector (Pettersson Model D-100). Individuals in the S group had their tymbals left intact, however scales near the tymbal organs were removed using curved forceps to simulate experimental manipulation.

      Outdoor Flight Arena

      Two ultraviolet lights were placed ~5 m off the ground and set 4 m apart in the center of a large, open field (~600 m2). These lights increased the general insect abundance within the flight arena, luring in free-flying bats which then began to forage reliably at this location. Moths included in this study were released at this site, one at a time, starting after sunset (21:00) for 6 h (03:00) or until we ran out of moths to run in trials. Most releases involved the moths taking off shortly after being released from their containers. In a few cases we released the moths by tossing them up in the air. In these cases, we did not collect data for 5–10 s or until it was clear the moth was flying under its own power and would be able to react normally to any bat attacks. Bat-moth interactions were recorded using high-speed, infrared cameras and ultrasound-sensitive microphones. Using these tools, we were able to document the evasive and acoustic responses of moths in response to natural interactions with their bat predators. To reduce the effects of pseudo-replication, we used only the first interaction for each individual moth included in our study. The number of individual bats within the calibrated space at any given time varied from one to six. This resulted in some unavoidable pseudo-replication across individual bats which could not be accounted for. However, this study was conducted over a period of 3 years, increasing the likelihood that unique bats were included.

      Audio Recording and Analysis

      Audio of the bat-moth interactions was recorded using three Avisoft Bioacoustics CM16/CMPA ultrasonic microphones (Berlin, Germany) with an Avisoft Ultrasound Gate 416H recording interface. Two microphones were placed ~1.5 m high and 4 m apart, near the edges of the recording volume, pointed up toward the interaction space. A third microphone was placed on the end of a pole which was held close (between 1 and 3 m) to the moth's location.

      Video Recording

      In 2011–2012, three Basler AG Scout infrared cameras (Model scA640-120gc; Ahrensburg, Germany) were used to record bat-moth interactions at 60 frames*sec-1 with 640 × 480 resolution. In 2013, three Basler Ace acA-2000-50 gm infrared cameras capable of recording up to 80 frames*sec-1 with 1,024 × 720 resolution were substituted. The cameras were synchronized with the audio recordings using custom hardware (Innovision Systems, Columbiaville, MI, USA). Video was acquired with MaxTraq2D software (Innovision Systems) and two Intel PRO/1000 PT Dual Server Adapters (Intel, Model: EXPI9402PT) installed in a PC running Windows 7. Six Wildlife Engineering IR-Lamp6 lights (Tucson, AZ, USA), two Bosch UFLED20-8BD illuminators (Farmington Hills, MI, USA) and two Raytec Raymax 200 platinum illuminators (Ashington, UK) provided infrared illumination in the flight arena.

      Evasive Flight Behavior

      We recorded interactions of moths with free-flying bats to determine how frequently they utilized evasive flight maneuvers. We included data from 345 bat-moth interactions across 5 species of tiger moth, P. murina, Bertholdia trigona, P. roseicapitis, C. arizonensis, and C. martini. Field trials included moths from experimental groups which could normally produce sound (i.e., T+, S) as well as individuals which had their sound-producing structures removed via ablation (i.e., T-). We included data about evasive flight responses from both clicking and silenced moths in this study. We manually classified each interaction and scored them as either “Turn Away,” “Dives,” or “No Evasion” as defined in classic studies of moth evasive flight (Roeder, 1962). However, we restricted our analysis to include only moths exhibiting “Dives” (renamed “Evasion”) or “No Evasion,” as we observed “Turn Away” flight in fewer than 10% of interactions. Examples of these behaviors can be seen in Supplemental Videos 13 as well as Figures 6A,B from Dowdy and Conner (2016).

      Unpalatability

      All moths species included in this study were previously known to produce ultrasound in response to bat echolocation (Corcoran et al., 2010; Dowdy, unpublished). Because of this, all of the palatability data reported in this study was measured from moths which were experimentally silenced (i.e., T–) to avoid confounding palatability with potentially deterrent effects of moth clicks. This data was derived from 118 interactions with wild, free-flying bats, which are also represented in the evasive flight behavior dataset. When captured by bats, moths were either dropped immediately (“Capture, Drop”—unpalatable) or not at all (“Consume”—palatable). In addition, we included data from 14 hand-feedings of silenced P. roseicapitis and 10 hand-feedings of silenced C. martini to captive big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). These 24 moths were presented to captive bats in a random order, interspersed with palatable moth species from the family Noctuidae. When moths were presented to these bats they were either rejected (unpalatable) or consumed (palatable) within 5–30 s of their first presentation. Trials continued until bats showed signs of satiation, which was defined as two sequential and complete rejections of control moths. Trials began again after 1–2 h until all tiger moths had been tested.

      Bertholdia Trigona Data

      Data for B. trigona (79 of our total 349 interactions) come from a previous study using the same experimental methods and location as those used in this study (Corcoran and Conner, 2012). Palatability and evasive diving flight data for B. trigona come from Figures 2B, 4A in Corcoran and Conner (2012), respectively. We included only data about diving evasive behavior from this study. Palatability data was taken only for silenced moths and was measured as the number of captured individuals that were not consumed.

      <italic>Pygarctia roseicapitis</italic> and <italic>Cisthene martini</italic> Data

      Data for P. roseicapitis and C. martini (87 of our total 349 interactions) come from a previous publication using the same experimental methods and location as those used in this study (Dowdy and Conner, 2016). Palatability and evasive diving flight data for these species come from Figures 2, 7 in Dowdy and Conner (2016), respectively, excluding some additional, new palatability data for these species which is presented here for the first time.

      Statistics

      Statistical analyses were performed in R version 3.3.2 (R Core Team, 2016) utilizing the dplyr package to prepare the data (Wickham et al., 2019). We implemented the glm function of the base R stats package to create a generalized linear model (GLM) with a binomial variance function and logit link function. This approach was used to compare the proportion of nonchalant flight behavior between species as well as the proportion of unpalatability between species. Using the relevel function within the base R stats package, we constructed a set of level contrasts for the species factor and applied them to our model in order to more easily compare the model results among all species. For each species, 95% confidence intervals for the binomial probabilities of nonchalance and unpalatability were constructed using the binconf function of the Hmisc package (Harrell, 2019) using the Wilson interval method. We used these proportions as a property of each species and examined how unpalatability predicted nonchalant flight using linear regression. Because we were interested in predicting the frequency of nonchalant evasive responses exhibited by species given their unpalatability, we constructed 95% prediction intervals for nonchalant flight using the predict function in the base R stats package. LaTeX tables were prepared using the texreg package (Leifeld, 2013). Plots were generated using ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016) with additional functions from the scales (Wickham, 2018) and gridExtra packages (Auguie, 2017). All R scripts are available via online repository (see Data Availability Statement).

      Results

      We found significant intraspecific variation in nonchalance and unpalatability (Figure 1). Statistical significance, level contrasts, and relative coefficients are given in Tables 1, 2. P. murina was significantly less nonchalant than all other species in our analysis. The prevalence of nonchalance between B. trigona and P. roseicapitis was not significantly different, though B. trigona was significantly less nonchalant as compared to C. arizonensis and C. martini. P. roseicapitis and C. arizonensis were not significantly different, however P. roseicapitis was significantly less nonchalant than C. martini. Finally, C. arizonenesis and C. martini were not significantly different.

      Interspecific variation in nonchalant flight behavior and unpalatability. Sample sizes for each species and the percentage exhibiting either nonchalant flight or unpalatability are given within each bar. Significantly different groups within each plot are indicated by different letters (see Tables 1, 2 for more info).

      Generalized linear model of nonchalant flight behavior.

      Relative nonchalance Contrast 1 Contrast 2 Contrast 3 Contrast 4 Contrast 5
      (Intercept) 1.23*** 0.23 −0.28 −0.82*** −1.57**
      Pygarctia murina 1.00** 1.50** 2.05*** 2.80***
      (0.33) (0.36) (0.34) (0.55)
      (0.24) (0.23) (0.27) (0.24) (0.49)
      Bertholdia trigona −1.00** 0.51 1.05** 1.80***
      (0.33) (0.35) (0.33) (0.54)
      Pygarctia roseicapitis −1.50*** −0.51 0.55 1.29*
      (0.36) (0.35) (0.36) (0.56)
      Carales arizonensis −2.05*** −1.05** −0.55 0.74
      (0.34) (0.33) (0.36) (0.55)
      Cisthene martini −2.80*** −1.80*** −1.29* −0.74
      (0.55) (0.54) (0.56) (0.55)
      AIC 429.17 429.17 429.17 429.17 429.17
      BIC 448.38 448.38 448.38 448.38 448.38
      Log likelihood −209.58 −209.58 −209.58 −209.58 −209.58
      Deviance 419.17 419.17 419.17 419.17 419.17
      Num. obs. 345 345 345 345 345

      p < 0.001,

      p < 0.01,

      p < 0.05.

      The coefficient estimates listed for each species give the relative log-odds difference between a given species and the unlisted species, with p-values for pairwise comparisons given as *'s. Higher-valued negative relative log-odds indicate greater likelihood of employing nonchalant flight relative to the unlisted species. Higher-valued positive relative log-odds indicate lower likelihood of employing nonchalant flight relative to the unlisted species. Values in parentheses are standard errors on coefficient estimates.

      Generalized linear model of unpalatability.

      Relative unpalatability Contrast 1 Contrast 2 Contrast 3 Contrast 4 Contrast 5
      (Intercept) 1.32* 0.69 −0.82 −2.77*** −2.48***
      Pygarctia murina 0.63 2.14** 4.09*** 3.81***
      (0.70) (0.67) (0.92) (0.93)
      (0.56) (0.41) (0.36) (0.73) (0.74)
      Bertholdia trigona −0.63 0.51** 3.47*** 3.18***
      (0.70) (0.55) (0.84) (0.84)
      Pygarctia roseicapitis −2.14** −1.51** 1.95 1.66*
      (0.67) (0.55) (0.81) (0.82)
      Carales arizonensis −4.09*** −3.47*** −1.95* −0.29
      (0.92) (0.84) (0.81) (1.04)
      Cisthene martini −3.81*** −3.18*** −1.66* −0.29
      (0.93) (0.84) (0.82) (1.04)
      AIC 137.56 137.56 137.56 137.56 137.56
      BIC 152.34 152.34 152.34 152.34 152.34
      Log Likelihood −63.78 −63.78 −63.78 −63.78 −63.78
      Deviance 127.56 127.56 127.56 127.56 127.56
      Num. obs. 142 142 142 142 142

      p < 0.001,

      p < 0.01,

      p < 0.05.

      The coefficient estimates listed for each species give the relative log-odds difference between a given species and the unlisted species, with p-values for pairwise comparisons given as *'s. Higher-valued negative relative log-odds indicate greater likelihood of being unpalatable relative to the unlisted species. Higher-valued positive relative log-odds indicate lower likelihood of being unpalatable relative to the unlisted species. Values in parentheses are standard errors on coefficient estimates.

      Compared to all other species in our analysis, P. murina and B. trigona were significantly less unpalatable, whereas C. arizonensis and C. martini were significantly more unpalatable than all other species in our analysis. P. roseicapitis was significantly different from both of these groups, exhibiting an intermediate level of unpalatability.

      Modeling nonchalance from unpalatability yielded a significantly strong, positive relationship (p < 0.05, Adj. R2: 0.87; Table 3). However, because we could only sample 5 species, our 95% prediction intervals are large, limiting our current ability to confidently estimate nonchalance from unpalatability (Figure 2). A summary of the estimated nonchalance and unpalatability for each species, along with their 95% confidence intervals, can be found in Table 4.

      Linear model of nonchalance from unpalatability.

      Nonchalance
      (Intercept) 0.15
      (0.09)
      Unpalatability 0.66*
      (0.12)
      R2 0.90
      Adj. R2 0.87
      Num. obs. 5
      RMSE 0.08

      *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01,

      p < 0.05.

      Nonchalance and unpalatability estimate values are proportions of individuals within each species exhibiting each trait. Nonchalant flight behavior is strongly and significantly associated with unpalatability. For every 1.0% increase in unpalatability, the likelihood of nonchalant flight is expected to increase by ~0.66%. Unpalatability explains a large portion of the variation in nonchalance among species (Adj. R2 = 0.87). Values in parentheses are standard errors on coefficient estimates.

      Plot of linear model of nonchalance. Gray ribbon represents the 95% prediction interval for the linear model relative nonchalant flight behavior and unpalatability (Table 3). For a species with a given Unpalatability, Nonchalance is expected to fall within these intervals in 95% of cases. For each species, point estimates (unique symbols) and 95% confidence intervals (line segments) for the binomial probabilities for both nonchalance and unpalatability are plotted.

      Summary of nonchalance and unpalatability estimates.

      Nonchalance Count Lower 95% CI % Nonchalant Upper 95% CI
      Cisthene martini 29 65.5 82.8 92.4
      Carales arizonensis 82 58.9 69.5 78.4
      Pygarctia roseicapitis 58 44.1 56.9 68.8
      Bertholdia trigona 79 33.9 44.3 55.3
      Pygarctia murina 97 15.5 22.7 32.0
      Noctuidae 0 NA NA NA
      Unpalatability Count Lower 95% CI % Unpalatable Upper 95% CI
      Cisthene martini 26 75.9 92.3 97.9
      Carales arizonensis 34 80.9 94.1 98.4
      Pygarctia roseicapitis 36 53.1 69.4 82.0
      Bertholdia trigona 27 18.6 33.3 52.2
      Pygarctia murina 19 8.5 21.1 43.3
      Noctuidae 114 0.0 0.9 4.8

      For each species, estimated nonchalance (“%Nonchalant”; upper table) and palatability (“%Unpalatable”; lower table) are summarized, along with the lower and upper bounds of their 95% confidence intervals as well as the sample size (“Count”) for each estimate. Data from hand-fed control moths within the family Noctuidae (“Noctuidae Control”) are included with the unpalatability data as a benchmark of palatability.

      Discussion

      Our results demonstrate that, at least among tiger moths, significant interspecific variation in the likelihood of enacting evasive maneuvers in response to bat echolocation exists. This phenomenon forms a nonchalance continuum, with some species enacting escape maneuvers often, some rarely, and others lying somewhere between. In addition, we found significant interspecific differences in palatability among tiger moths. Variation in palatability has been noted in other insects, driven mainly by the host plants which they have fed on, and is generally known as a “palatability spectrum” (Brower et al., 1968, 1972; Turner, 1984). Further, we have shown that the position of tiger moth species along the nonchalance continuum and the palatability spectrum are strongly correlated. In other words, moths with more potent chemical defenses are more likely to display nonchalant flight. Interestingly, this is apparently independent of anti-bat strategy. Two species included in our analysis, B. trigona and C. arizonensis, produce clicks at very high rates and have been hypothesized or demonstrated to be capable of jamming bat sonar (Corcoran et al., 2010; Conner and Corcoran, 2012). Despite this, they each occupy significantly different places along the nonchalance continuum. However, consistent with our model, the large difference in unpalatability between these two species appears to be an important factor in determining their anti-bat evasive flight responses, with the more unpalatable C. arizonensis exhibiting nonchalant responses more frequently. Less unpalatable moths face an increased risk of consumption if captured by bat predators, and so they utilize evasive diving maneuvers more frequently, likely offsetting some of this risk. Additionally, more unpalatable prey may benefit from nonchalant flight by avoiding some of the potential costs of evasive maneuvers, including energy expenditure, opportunity costs related to hostplant- or mate-finding, or even increased exposure to terrestrial or aquatic predators (Guignion and Fullard, 2004; Yager, 2011). Our findings lend further support to an earlier study which demonstrated that palatability relates to evasive behaviors enacted from rest on substrates (e.g., flying away, dropping from vegetation) when exposed to tactile stimulation (Evans, 1983). These observations describe unpalatable prey exhibiting behaviors that could also be considered more nonchalant.

      The relatively palatable P. murina produces anti-bat ultrasound with acoustic characteristics similar to those of its more unpalatable, sympatric congener P. roseicapitis (Dowdy and Conner, 2016; Dowdy, unpublished). This is similar to the relationship between Cycnia tenera and Euchaetes egle, two sympatric tiger moths native to eastern North America. Lab experiments with naïve bats have shown that the palatable E. egle was protected from predation by Eptesicus fuscus bats, acting as an acoustic Batesian mimic of C. tenera (Barber and Conner, 2007). It is likely that P. murina is an acoustic Batesian mimic of P. roseicapitis. It is intriguing then, that P. murina is less nonchalant than its more toxic model. It is possible that Batesian mimics will often “play it safe” by enacting escape responses more frequently than their more honestly signaling counterparts. This should be explored further with experiments confirming model-mimic relationships followed by a comparison of the frequency of evasive behaviors between members of these groups.

      Of course, tiger moths are not the only chemically protected insects that employ evasive flight to contend with bat predation. Ears tuned to ultrasonic frequencies can be found in at least 5 orders of insects (i.e., Coleoptera, Lepidoptera, Mantodea, Neuroptera, and Orthoptera) and have evolved independently at least 14 times (Dawson et al., 2004; Greenfield, 2016). All 5 orders of ultrasound-sensitive insects have been reported to exhibit evasive flight responses in response to bat echolocation (Roeder, 1962; Miller and Oleson, 1979; Spangler, 1988; Yager et al., 1990; Dawson et al., 2004). Each of these orders, with the exception of Mantodea, contains examples of species utilizing a chemical defense of some kind (Eisner et al., 2005). Intraspecific variation in host plant chemical defenses as well as multiple host plant affiliations in some groups could generate intraspecific differences in chemical protection, potentially explaining some of the variation in nonchalance both among and within a variety of insect species, such as the recently reported nonchalant flight in fireflies in response to foraging bats (Leavell et al., 2018).

      By gathering more evasive flight and palatability data for more species, we could also better predict nonchalance from palatability data. This is useful, as measuring palatability can be much simpler than making detailed observations of evasive flight. Additionally, if palatability could be confidently inferred from preserved museum specimens using analytical chemistry methods such as mass spectroscopy, then these traits could be determined on an unprecedented scale (Bowers, 2009; Anderson et al., 2016). Gaining a better understanding of the variation in nonchalance will help us better characterize how animals balance the potential costs of escape maneuvers with the potential benefits gained from eluding predators.

      The bat-moth predator-prey system is a useful model for examining the escape behaviors of prey, both aposematic and non-aposematic. The sensory system that moths use to detect bat predators and their acoustic aposematic signals can be ablated by simple surgical impairment without compromising their overall physical integrity, their remaining behavioral repertoire, nor other cues produced by the specimen. Additionally, moth unpalatability can be modulated by rearing individuals on diets that are lacking defensive compounds. With this approach, the nonchalance, unpalatability, and/or aposematic signals of individual moths can be experimentally manipulated to yield greater insights into the costs, benefits, and factors influencing escape behavior. We believe the bat-moth predator-prey system is a promising new modality for understanding how the escape behaviors of prey are enacted and evolve.

      Data Availability Statement

      The datasets analyzed for this study and the associated R scripts can be found in the FigShare repositories: Evasive Flight Data (10.6084/m9.figshare.9705788), Palatability Data (10.6084/m9.figshare.9706187), R Script (10.6084/m9.figshare.9706280), Figures and Tables (10.6084/m9.figshare.10318622), Tables in LaTeX format (10.6084/m9.figshare.10318604), and Supplemental Videos (10.6084/m9.figshare.10115180; 10.6084/m9.figshare.10115204; 10.6084/m9.figshare.10115207).

      Author Contributions

      ND conceived and designed experiments and performed and analyzed experiments. ND and WC wrote the manuscript.

      Conflict of Interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      We would like to thank Wake Forest undergraduates Kaitlyn Roman, Deanna Margius, Meredith Shaw, and Celia Spell and Dr. Aaron J. Corcoran for their assistance in the lab and field. We also thank the staff of The Southwestern Research Station of the American Museum of Natural History. Finally, we would like to thank Yohami Fernández Delgado and two reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions.

      Supplementary Material

      The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: /articles/10.3389/fevo.2019.00480/full#supplementary-material

      References Acharya L. Fenton M. B. (1992). Echolocation behaviour of vespertilionid bats (Lasiurus cinereus and Lasiurus borealis) attacking airborne targets including arctiids moths. Can. J. Zool. 70, 12921298. 10.1139/z92-180 Acharya L. Fenton M. B. (1999). Bat attacks and moth defensive behavior around street lights. Can. J. Zool. 77, 2733. 10.1139/z98-202 Anderson T. J. Wagner D. L. Cooper B. R. McCarty M. E. Zaspel J. M. (2016). HPLC-MS analysis of Lichen-derived metabolites in the life stages of Crambidia cephalica (Grote and Robinson). J. Chem. Ecol. 43, 6674. 10.1007/s10886-016-0799-327966190 Auguie B. (2017). gridExtra: Miscellaneous Functions for Grid Graphics. R package version 2.3. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gridExtra (accessed November 18, 2019). Barber J. R. Chadwell B. A. Garrett N. Schmidt-French B. Conner W. E. (2009). Naïve bats discriminate arctiid moth warning sounds but generalize their aposematic meaning. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 21412148. 10.1242/jeb.02999119561203 Barber J. R. Conner W. E. (2007). Acoustic mimicry in a predator-prey interaction. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 104, 93319334. 10.1073/pnas.070362710417517637 Bates H. W. (1862). XXXII. Contributions to an Insect Fauna of the Amazon Valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidæ. Trans. Linn. Soc. Lond. 23, 495566. 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1860.tb00146.x Blanchette A. Becza N. Saporito R. A. (2017). Escape behavior of aposematic (Oophaga pumilio) and cryptic (Craugastor sp.) frogs in response to simulated predator approach. J. Trop. Ecol. 33, 165169. 10.1017/S0266467417000037 Bowers M. D. (2009). Chemical defenses in woolly bears: sequestration and efficacy against predators and parasitoids, in Tiger Moths and Wooly Bears: Behavior, Ecology, and Evolution of the Arctiidae, ed Conner W. E. (New York: Oxford University Press), 83101. Brower L. P. McEvoy P. B. Williamson K. L. Flannery M. A. (1972). Variation in cardiac glycoside content of monarch butterflies from natural populations in eastern North America. Science 177, 426429. 10.1126/science.177.4047.4265043141 Brower L. P. Ryerson W. N. Coppinger L. L. Glazier S. C. (1968). Ecological chemistry and the palatability spectrum. Science 161, 13491350. 10.1126/science.161.3848.134917831347 Chai P. Srygley R. B. (1990). Predation and the flight, morphology, and temperature of neotropical rain-forest butterflies. Am. Nat. 135, 748765. 10.1086/285072 Conner W. E. Corcoran A. J. (2012). Sound strategies: the 65-million-year-old battle between bats and insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 57, 2139. 10.1146/annurev-ento-121510-13353721888517 Cooper W. E. Jr. (2018). Escape from predators, in Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, 2nd Edn, ed Choe J. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 349360. Cooper W. E. Jr. Caldwell J. Vitt L. (2009a). Conspicuousness and vestigial escape behaviour by two dendrobatid frogs, Dendrobates auratus and Oophaga pumilio. Behaviour 146, 325349. 10.1163/156853909X410946 Cooper W. E. Jr. Caldwell J. Vitt L. (2009b). Risk assessment and withdrawal behavior by two species of aposematic poison frogs, Dendrobates auratus and Oophaga pumilio, on forest trails. Ethology 115, 311320. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01615.x Cooper W. E. Jr. Frederick W. G. (2007). Optimal flight initiation distance. J. Theor. Biol. 244, 5967. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.07.01116949619 Corcoran A.J. Conner W. E. Barber J. R. (2010). Anti-bat tiger moth sounds: form and function. Curr. Zool. 56, 358369. 10.1093/czoolo/56.3.358 Corcoran A. J. Conner W. E. (2012). Sonar-jamming in the field: effectiveness and behavior of a unique prey defense. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 42784287. 10.1242/jeb.07694323175526 Dawson J. W. Kutsch W. Robertson R. M. (2004). Auditory-evoked evasive manoeuvres in free-flying locusts and moths. J. Comp. Physiol. A 190, 6984. 10.1007/s00359-003-0474-314655020 Dowdy N. J. Conner W. E. (2016). Acoustic aposematism and evasive action in select chemically defended arctiine (Lepidoptera: Erebidae) species: nonchalant or not? PLoS ONE 11:e0152981. 10.1371/journal.pone.0152981 Dugas M. Halbrook S. R. Killius A. M. del Sol J. F. Richards-Zawacki C. L. (2015). Colour and escape behaviour in polymorphic populations of an aposematic poison frog. Ethology 121, 813822. 10.1111/eth.12396 Dunning D. C. Acharya L. Merriman C. B. Ferro L. D. (1992). Interactions between bats and arctiid moths. Can. J. Biol. 70, 22182223. 10.1139/z92-298 Eisner T. Eisner M. Siegler M. (2005). Secret Weapons: Defenses of Insects, Spiders, Scorpions, and Other Many-Legged Creatures. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Evans D. L. (1983). Relative defensive behavior of some moths and the implication to predator-prey interactions. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 33, 103111. 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1983.tb03240.x Goldman L. J. Henson O. W. (1977). Prey recognition and selection by the constant frequency bat, Pteronotus p. parnellii. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2, 411419. 10.1007/BF00299509 Greenfield M. D. (2016). Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Insects, in Insect Hearing, eds Pollack A. C. Mason A. N. Popper R. R. Fay (Switzerland: Springer), 1747. Guignion C. Fullard J. H. (2004). A potential cost of responding to bats for moths flying over water. Can. J. Zool. 82, 529532. 10.1139/z04-015 Harrell F. E. Jr. (2019). Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous. R package version 4.2-0. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc (accessed November 18, 2019). Hatle J. D. Faragher S. G. (1998). Slow movement increases the survivorship of a chemically defended grasshopper in predatory encounters. Oecologia 115, 260267. 10.1007/s00442005051528308461 Hatle J. D. Salazar B. A. Whitman D. W. (2002). Survival advantage of sluggish individuals in aggregations of aposematic prey, during encounters with ambush predators. Evol. Ecol. 16, 415431. 10.1023/A:1020814110102 Hatle J. D. Whitman D. W. (2001). Sluggish movement of conspicuous insects as a defense mechanism against motion-oriented predators, in Insects and Plant Defence Dynamics, ed Ananthakrishnan T. N. (Enfield, NH: Science Publishers), 209228. Hristov N. I. Conner W. E. (2005). Sound strategy: acoustic aposematism in the bat-moth arms race. Naturwiss 92, 164169. 10.1007/s00114-005-0611-7 Leavell B. C. Rubin J. J. McClure C. J. W. Miner K. A. Branham M. A. Barber J. R. (2018). Fireflies thwart bat attack with multisensory warnings. Sci. Adv. 4:eaat6601. 10.1126/sciadv.aat660130140743 Leifeld P. (2013). texreg: conversion of Statistical Model Output in R to LaTeX and HTML Tables. J. Stat. Softw. 55, 124. 10.18637/jss.v055.i08 Miller L. A. Oleson J. (1979). Avoidance behavior in green lacewings – I. Behavior of free flying green lacewings to hunting bats and ultrasound. J. Comp. Physiol. A 131, 113120. 10.1007/BF00619071 Ozel L. D. Stynoski J. L. (2011). Differences in escape behavior between a cryptic and an aposematic litter frog. J. Herp. 45, 395398. 10.1670/10-249.1 Poulton E. B. (1890). The Colours of Animals: Their Meaning and Use, Especially Considered in the Case Of Insects. (New York, NY: D. Appleton), 1360. 10.5962/bhl.title.11353 R Core Team (2016). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. 30628467 Roeder K. D. (1962). The behaviour of free flying moths in the presence of artificial ultrasonic pulses. Anim. Behav. 10, 300304. 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90053-2 Samia D. S. M. Blumstein D. T. Stankowich T. Cooper W. E Jr. (2016). Fifty years of chasing lizards: new insights advance optimal escape theory. Biol. Rev. 91, 349366. 10.1111/brv.1217325620002 Spangler H. G. (1988). Hearing in tiger beetles (Cicindelidae). Physiol. Entomol. 13, 447452. 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1988.tb01129.x Speed M. P. Brockhurst M. A. Ruxton G. D. (2010). The dual benefits of aposematism: predator avoidance and enhanced resource collection. Evolution 64, 16221633. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00931.x20050915 Srygley R. B. (1994). Locomotor mimicry in butterflies? The associations of positions of centres of mass among groups of mimetic, unprofitable prey. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 343, 145155. 10.1098/rstb.1994.0017 Srygley R. B. (2004). The aerodynamic costs of warning signals in palatable mimetic butterflies and their distasteful models. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 589594. 10.1098/rspb.2003.262715156916 Stankowich T. Blumstein D. T. (2005). Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 26272634. 10.1098/rspb.2005.325116321785 Triblehorn J. D. Ghose K. Bohn K. Moss C. F. Yager D. D. (2008). Free-flight encounters between praying mantids (Parasphendale agrionina) and bats (Eptesicus fuscus). J. Exp. Biol. 211, 555562. 10.1242/jeb.00573618245632 Turner J. R. G. (1984). Mimicry: the palatability spectrum and its consequences, in The Biology of Butterflies, eds Vane-Wright R. I. Ackery P. R. (London: Academic Press), 141161. Wallace A. R. (1889). Darwinism – An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection With Some of Its Applications. London: MacMillan and Co. Whitman D. W. Blum M. S. Jones C. G. (1985). Chemical defense in Taeniopoda eques (Orthoptera: Acrididae): role of the metathoracic secretion. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 78, 451455. 10.1093/aesa/78.4.451 Wickham H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. Wickham H. (2018). scales: Scale Functions for Visualization. R package version 1.0.0. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=scales (accessed November 18, 2019). Wickham H. François R. Henry L. Müller K. (2019). dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation. R package version 0.8.3. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr (accessed November 18, 2019). Yager D. D. (2011). Predator detection and evasion by flying insects. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 17. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.01122226428 Yager D. D. May M. L. Fenton M. B. (1990). Ultrasound-triggered, flight-gated evasive maneuvers in the praying mantis Parasphendale agrionina I. free flight. J. Exp. Biol. 152, 1739. 2230635 Ydenberg R. C. Dill L. M. (1986). The Economics of fleeing predators. Adv. Study Behav. 16, 229249. 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60192-8

      Funding. This work was supported by a National Science Foundation grant to WC (IOS 0951160) and an NSF PRFB grant (DBI-1811897) to ND.

      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.hbstkjyxgs.com.cn
      gkchain.com.cn
      www.khtwti.com.cn
      www.ksfyjcz.com.cn
      wbit.org.cn
      valassis.com.cn
      ruuyue.com.cn
      www.mqchain.com.cn
      usmpbo.com.cn
      www.ww8news.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p