Front. Ecol. Evol.Frontiers in Ecology and EvolutionFront. Ecol. Evol.2296-701XFrontiers Media S.A.10.3389/fevo.2015.00147Ecology and EvolutionEditorialEditorial: Ballroom Biology: Recent Insights into Honey Bee Waggle Dance CommunicationsSchürchRoger12*CouvillonMargaret J.1BeekmanMadeleine31Laboratory of Apiculture and Social Insects, University of SussexBrighton, UK2Clinical Trials Unit, University of BernBern, Switzerland3Behaviour and Genetics of Social Insects Lab, School of Biological Sciences, University of SydneySydney, NSW, Australia
Edited and reviewed by: Matjaz Kuntner, Scientific Research Centre of the Slovenian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Slovenia
*Correspondence: Roger Schürch roger.schuerch@ctu.unibe.ch
This article was submitted to Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
The Editorial on the Research Topic Ballroom Biology: Recent Insights into Honey Bee Waggle Dance Communicationswaggle dancehoney beerecruitmentforagerssocial insectsanimal communication
In his 1950 book Bees: Their Vison, Chemical Senses, and Language, Karl von Frisch recounts his decades of research into how honey bees perceive the world, concluding “The bee's life is like a magic well: the more you draw from it, the more it fills with water.” (von Frisch, 1950). Sixty-five years on, science continues to draw from that magical well, and we have learned a tremendous amount about honey bee foraging, nest site selection and recruitment. We now understand a great deal about how the decisions made by individual bees, which usually have limited information, translate into colony-level decisions in an unpredictable environment. When studying honey bee behavior, it is impossible to ignore the role the unique waggle dance plays in the life of the bee. Honey bees use the dance to communicate the location of resources. The discovery and subsequent decoding of the waggle dance would earn Karl von Frisch a Nobel Prize in 1973.
The waggle dance is the inspiration behind the nine contributions, spanning from original experimental research to emerging methods, which we present here in this special Frontiers issue.
Available information for honey bees
We begin with an exploration of the information available to honey bee foragers. Rivera et al. used the duration of trophallaxis as a proxy for that individual's experience of resource quality, where a decrease in trophallaxis duration from a marked forager may suggest a decrease in forage quality. They also used forager wait time as a proxy for social information, where increased wait time for the marked returning nectar forager indicates that the colony no longer requires that resource so acutely. However, the authors did not find an effect of either on an individual bee's decision to stop foraging, reflecting that honey bee decision-making in a natural environment remains poorly understood. We do know that honey bees forage at long distances (von Frisch, 1967; Seeley, 1995). Ratnieks and Shackleton conclude that these distances, which may be up to 10–14 kilometers (Waddington et al., 1994; Beekman and Ratnieks, 2000; Couvillon et al., 2014), are exceptional, given the bee's body size. Ratnieks and Shackleton hypothesize it is the dance language that makes long distance foraging possible. The question of exactly how much information is conveyed by the waggle dance is examined directly in the contribution by Schürch and Ratnieks. By reanalyzing the information content in both vector components (direction and distance) of the dance, they demonstrate that 2.9 and 4.5 bits of information are conveyed respectively, showing that a little bit of information can go a long way if it is the right information (Schürch and Ratnieks). l'Anson Price and Grüter then review what is currently known about the circumstances that have led to the evolution of dance communication. They conclude that the dance information is most useful in an environment where resources are clustered, difficult to find, and of variable quality and when the environment itself is relatively stable for periods of time. Lastly, Avarguès-Weber et al. take a slightly different focus by looking not at the details conveyed from specialized signals such as the dance, but at the indirect, environmental information that bees may obtain. In doing so, they draw from work on both bees and fruit flies and discuss the evidence for observational learning and the neural circuits that may underpin it.
Within the dance: intra-dance variation and bee stop signals
Two contributions deal with specific aspects of the dance itself. Firstly, Beekman et al. examine intra-dance variation among Apis species by analyzing the dance precision of A. florea, A. dorsata, and A. mellifera in the contexts of both foraging and swarming. They find that all three species improve the precision of the dance with increasing distance to the source but that dance precision levels off. Their data supports the idea that intra-dance variation reflects an unavoidable constraint in the bees' ability to communicate more precisely. Kietzman and Visscher examine the bee's stop signal, which counters the waggle dance by providing negative feedback toward an advertised location. The combination of both positive feedback from the waggle dance and negative feedback from the stop signal, allows the colony to more rapidly respond to changes in the foraging environment.
Dancing into the future
Lastly, two contributions look ahead toward potential research avenues that pertain to the bees' communication system. Couvillon and Ratnieks argue that the waggle dance, specifically the ability of researchers to decode the dances to determine where honey bees are foraging, may be applied as a future tool for ecology to inform on a range of ecological, conservation, and land management issues. The authors concede, however, that decoding dances by hand, which is the current methodology to extract the spatial information from the dance, is time-consuming, an issue that may be helped by the contribution of Wario et al., where they present in their methods article the full specifications of the recording setup and the software for automatic recognition of individually tagged bees and the decoding of dances. With the development of this technology, it may be possible to track all individuals of a honey bee colony and to detect and to decode the communication dances automatically.
Conclusion
The diverse nature of the contributions in this special issue illustrates how, 93 years after von Frisch's first description of the honey bee's waggle dance (von Frisch, 1923), there still remains much to be learned. We hope that this eBook will continue to motivate scientists to draw from the well that is the bees' lives and further elucidate how such a tiny brain in a small insect can generate such amazingly complex behaviors as the waggle dance communication.
Author contributions
RS and MC came up with the topic idea, and MB contributed to its description and to the overall organization of the special topic. All editors contributed to the overseeing of reviews for this special issue and wrote the cover editorial.
Funding
MB is funded by the Australian Research Council (FT120100120 and DP140100560).
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
ReferencesBeekmanM.RatnieksF. L. W. (2000). Long-range foraging by the honey-bee, Apis mellifera L. Funct. Ecol.14, 490–496. 10.1046/j.1365-2435.2000.00443.xCouvillonM. J.SchürchR.RatnieksF. L. W. (2014). Waggle dance distances as integrative indicators of seasonal foraging challenges. PLoS ONE9:e93495. 10.1371/journal.pone.009349524695678SeeleyT. D. (1995). The Wisdom of the Hive.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.von FrischK. (1923). Über die “Spräche” der Bienen, eine tier-psychologische UntersuchungZool. Jahrb40, 1–186.von FrischK. (1950). Bees: Their Vision, Chemical Senses, and Language.Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.von FrischK. (1967). The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.WaddingtonK. D.HerbertT. J.VisscherP. K.RichterM. R. (1994). Comparisons of forager distributions from matched honey bee colonies in suburban environments. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.35, 423–429. 10.1007/BF00165845‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放
ENTER NUMBET 0016griven.com.cn www.lvtop.com.cn superong.org.cn skicms.com.cn www.ruuyue.net.cn www.nyriff.com.cn qzjkkj.org.cn www.slchain.com.cn www.suotai.net.cn ubermoney.com.cn