Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2813-4540 Frontiers Media S.A. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1330331 Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Original Research Effects of digital and non-digital parental distraction on parent-child interaction and communication ChamamSouhir 1 2 ForcellaAlexia 1 2 MusioNadia 1 2 QuinodozFlorence 1 2 DimitrovaNevena 1 * Faculty of Social Work (HETSL | HESSO), University of Applied Sciences and Arts Western Switzerland, Lausanne, Switzerland Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland

Edited by: Margarete Ingrid Bolten, University of Basel, Switzerland

Reviewed by: Veronika Konok, Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary

Tapan Parikh, Ann & Robert H. Lurie Children’s Hospital of Chicago, United States

Correspondence: Nevena Dimitrova nevena.dimitrova@hetsl.ch
21052024 2024 31330331 30102023 09042024 © 2024 Chamam, Forcella, Musio, Quinodoz and Dimitrova. 2024Chamam, Forcella, Musio, Quinodoz and Dimitrova

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

Technoference, namely parental screen use in the presence of a child, is a widespread phenomenon that has negative effects on parent-child interaction and communication. When parents use screens around their children there are fewer interactions and parents are less contingent and responsive to the child. Additionally, children show more negative behaviors, such as whining, frustration, and outbursts. Communication is also affected—parents speak and gesture less towards their children and, in turn, children are less likely to develop their language abilities. It remains unclear, however, if parental distraction due to screen use affects parent-child interaction and communication more negatively compared to non-digital parental distraction. Fifty-two parent-child dyads (mean child age = 22 months, range 12–36 months) first played for 5 min (Time 1); then (Time 2), the parent was asked to fill out a questionnaire on a tablet (screen condition), on a printed form (paper-pen condition) or was not interrupted (control condition). Interactive quality was assessed at Time 1 and Time 2 using the Coding Interactive Behavior scale. Communication was assessed by coding the number of word tokens and types during Time 1 and Time 2; child gestures were also coded. Results revealed that when parents were distracted—either by the paper-pen or the screen questionnaire—the quality of the interaction significantly deteriorated (ps ≤ .01) and the quantity of parental communication significantly declined (ps ≤ .012). Importantly, the nature of the distraction did not matter: there were no significant differences between the paper-pen and the screen distraction conditions across Time 2 (ps ≥ .59). Findings suggest that parental distraction matters for the quality of interaction and the amount of communicative bids, independently on whether parents were distracted by a digital or non-digital activity. These findings likely relate to complex factors related to young children's experiences and habits with parental screen use.

screen technoference toddlers interaction language Foundation Action Innocence (Switzerland) section-at-acceptanceChild Mental Health and Interventions

香京julia种子在线播放

    1. <form id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></form>
      <address id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv><nobr id=HxFbUHhlv></nobr></nobr></address>

      Introduction

      Young children rely on interactions with their parents in order to learn and develop their social and emotional skills. When parent-child interactions are disrupted—such as when the parent is being distracted by using a screen—the interaction is negatively affected. In the current study, we test how different parental distractions may disrupt the quality and quantity of interactions between parents and their toddlers. Specifically, we ask whether parental distraction from a screen activity leads to lower quality of the interaction and to less communicative exchanges between the parent and the child than parental distraction from the same activity on paper-pencil.

      Technoference

      Digital devices are ubiquitous: people across all ages, all cultures and all socioeconomic backgrounds are using digital media on an everyday basis. For smartphones only, 82% of French families (1) and 97% of the Swiss population (2) own at least one device. Digital devices are not only widespread; they are also highly used. Accordingly, French adults use digital media for 5 h/day [France (3)], whereas this duration almost doubles for American parents [i.e., 9 h/day (4)].

      Yet, when a parent uses a digital device in the presence of a child, the nature and quality of the parent-child interaction are impacted (5). In fact, interactions are frequently interrupted when parents use a screen technology. Radesky and colleagues were the first to operationalize parents’ level of device use during parent–child interaction as “the extent to which the primary focus of the caregiver's attention and engagement was with the [digital] device rather than the child” (2014, p. 845). Later on, McDaniel (6) coined the term “technoference” or “technology interference” to describe the situations when digital media intrude and interrupt parent-child interactions and communications. In an American survey, 68% of parents indicated that they feel distracted by their smartphone when spending time with their children (7). Such self-reports are supported by systematic observations: 73% of parents have used their smartphone while in a fast food restaurant with their children (8).

      Given that technoference is not an isolated phenomenon, studies have examined the implications of technoference on children. Beyond issues related to safety concerns (9), there is emerging evidence that parental screen use around children might negatively influence child's development. A particular emphasis is put on young children (0–3 years) given the importance of parent-child interactions on early psychological development.

      Effects of technoference on parent-child interactions

      Interactions between parents and infants play a crucial role in supporting various aspects of the child’s development (10). These interactions enable young children to develop their social awareness through continuous and mutually responsive exchanges with their parents (11, 12). Additionally, sensitive parent-infant interactions lay the groundwork for forming secure attachment bonds (13, 14). As a result, the children’s growing social awareness and attachment provide the basis and incentive for them to explore and learn about the world, foster healthy social and emotional growth (15), acquire language and communication skills (16, 17), gain insights into themselves and others, and establish positive social connections.

      A crucial aspect of the quality of parent-child interactions is the ability of parents to detect, acknowledge and respond to child's behavior and communicative bids [i.e., parental sensitivity (18, 19)]. There is broad consensus that parental sensitivity is crucial for child development (20). However, when parents’ focus of attention shifts to a digital device instead of the child (i.e., technoference), their ability to be sensitive and responsive to their children is negatively impacted.

      Research shows that when parents use screens in the presence of their child, there are fewer interactions (21, 22) and parents are less contingent and responsive to the child’s behavior (9, 2327). For example, studies looking at parents in public places show that lack of sensitivity to the child, such as not noticing signs of emotional distress, increases in parents who use a smartphone, compared with those who don't (28, 29). A similar finding, but relating to duration of use, is reported in studies by Tharner and colleagues (30) and Wolfers and colleagues (31). In these studies the authors show that the longer parents use their smartphones, the less sensitive they are to their child. Additionally, technoference is related to increases in the number of conflicts with the child, the number of negative behaviors towards the child (8) and the dissatisfaction of the time spent with the child (21).

      Parental screen use during parent-child interactions also affects the child. Technoference is associated with both more internalized behaviors, such as whining or pouting, and more externalized behaviors, such as agitation, frustration and outbursts (3234). Children show more negative affect and less positive affect when their mother uses a screen during interaction (35). In addition, children show more behaviors to attract their mother's attention during an interrupted mother-child interaction with a screen compared to an uninterrupted interaction (36). A recent experimental study shows that technoference affects infants’ physiological reactivity (i.e., increased heart rate), suggesting that this may be a stressful context (37).

      Effects of technoference on parent-child communication

      In the first years of life, parent-child interactions provide the foundations of young children's communicative development. Stemming from the Transactional Theory in child developmental (38), the importance of face-to-face parent–child interactions in the development of communication in early childhood is widely recognized (3942). Mounting evidence establishes direct links between the parental communicative input and young children's both verbal and non-verbal communication abilities. Namely, individual differences in maternal gesture rates correlate with their infants’ own gesture (43, 44) and parents who direct more speech to their children have children showing faster and larger vocabulary growth (45, 46).

      There is empirical evidence showing that technoference affects parental communicative input towards young children (47). In a study with 6-year-old children, Radesky et al. (22) found that mothers who used their phone during the observation session spoke less and made fewer nonverbal gestures to their children (80% of the utterances and 61% of the nonverbal gestures compared to those who did not use phones). Importantly, findings of Reed et al.'s laboratory study showed that technoference affected vocabulary acquisition in toddlers. Specifically, 2-year-olds were less likely to learn a novel word taught by parents when they were distracted by a 30-sec phone call compared to peers whose parents were not interrupted (48). More recently, it has been shown that the amount of audible notifications parents report receiving per hour was negatively associated with infants’ vocabulary in controlled observations of 18- to 25-month-olds from New Zealand (49).

      The current study

      Existing evidence shows that technoference is a widespread phenomenon that affects young children's development. Most research, however, has studied parental distraction by a screen compared to situations when parents are not distracted. While these studies highlight issues regarding screen use during parent-child interactions, it is clear that parents are oftentimes distracted in many other ways during interactions with their child, such as attending to another sibling or finishing cooking a meal. In order to determine the effect of parental distraction from using a screen, it is crucial to provide evidence from experimental conditions when parents are distracted from a similar but non-digital activity. To our knowledge, the empirical evidence on the differential effect of technoference compared to other parental distractions is very scarce. In a U.S. study on question-asking during parent-child interactions, Gaudreau et al. (50) found that only for information-seeking questions of parents—but not of children, nor for responsiveness to questions—distraction from a cell phone showed more negative impact than distraction from a non-digital activity. This finding points to the importance of controlling for parental distraction in order to clarify whether technoference affects parent-child interactions above and beyond non-digital parental distractions.

      Accordingly, in this study we ask whether technoference affects the interaction and communication skills of parents and young children above and beyond parental distraction from a paper-pen activity. Based on the literature, we made the following hypotheses:

      Both interaction and communication will be negatively affected when parents are distracted compared to when parents are not being distracted.

      Both interaction and communication will be negatively affected when parents are being distracted in the screen condition compared to when parents are being distracted in the paper-pen condition, given that digital devices may be especially distracting to parents (23, 51).

      Methods Population

      Fifty-two parent-child dyads were invited to participated in this study. Two dyads were excluded because of technical problems preventing from coding the data. The final sample consisted of 50 parent-child dyads; however, interaction scores are missing for one dyad due to disturbances during data collection. The G*Power software indicated that the obtained power for correctly rejecting the null hypothesis (1-β err prob) given an effect size of .25 and the study's sample size is .95.

      Mean child age was 22 months (min = 12, max = 36, SD = 7.37 months); 26 girls (52%) participated. Mean parental age was 34 years (min = 27, max = 49, SD = 4.72 years); 45 mothers (90%) participated. Parents were predominantly married or in a couple (88%), mostly highly educated (67% held a university degree) and the large majority had a professional occupation (68%). Participants were recruited in the metropolitan area of a large French-speaking city in Switzerland by posting flyers in day-care centers and pediatric practices, by word-of-mouth, and through advertisement on social networks.

      Procedure

      Data was collected in a quiet laboratory room over the summer of 2021. Upon arrival of the parent-child dyads, an experimenter explained that the purpose of the study is to evaluate parent-child interaction (without mentioning parental distraction), answered eventual questions and obtained written consent from parents. Each parent-child dyad was invited to sit on a foam tile carpet on the floor; several cushions were supplied. A seek-and-find book as well as a wooden bear puzzle were provided for the interactive play session (see Figure 1). All parent-child interactions were videotaped by a static camera. Before leaving the room, the experimenter reminded participants that the interaction was being filmed and asked them to remain facing the camera.

      Materials used for the interactive play session.

      All dyads were instructed to play for 10 min as they usually would do in a quiet room. Participating dyads were randomly assigned into one of three condition. In the first condition, parent-child dyads interacted for 10 min without distraction (“control group”, 16 dyads, or 32%). In a second condition (“paper-pen distraction”, 17 dyads, 34%), each dyad interacted for 5 min without distraction (Time 1), then was interrupted by the experimenter who asked the parent to fill in a demographic questionnaire on paper while continuing to interact with the child for additional 5 min (Time 2); the experimenter then left the room. In the third condition (“screen condition, 16 dyads, 32%), each dyad interacted for 5 min without distraction (Time 1), then was interrupted by the experimenter who asked the parent to fill in a demographic questionnaire on a digital tablet while continuing to interact with the child for additional 5 min (Time 2); the experimenter then left the room.

      In all conditions, after ten minutes of interaction, the experimenter returned to the room and stopped the video recording. Parents were then invited to complete the demographic questionnaire either in the lab or at home. Each session lasted approximately 40 min. At the end of each session, participants were informed about the focus of the study on parental distraction and were given the possibility to retract their participation if they no longer agreed with the aims of the study; no participants retracted. All participants received a CHF20 gift voucher for a bookstore.

      Measures Demographic questionnaire

      A 13-item demographic questionnaire contained questions about the child (e.g., age, nationality, child’s place in siblings and dominant childcare arrangement) and the parent (e.g., age, gender, education and marital status).

      Interaction skills

      The interaction skills of the child, the parent and of the dyad taken together were assessed during the 10-minute observed play using the Coding Interactive Behavior scheme [CIB (52, 53); for French validation]. This tool codes parent-child interactions across three types of scales: child, parent, and dyad.

      The child scale consists of items divided into three composite scores: social involvement, withdrawal/negative emotionality, and compliance; for the purposes of this study, we focus on the social involvement and withdrawal composite scores. The child social involvement composite score is coded on the following 9 items: child gaze/joint attention, positive affect, child affection to parent, alert, fatigue (reversed score), child vocalization/verbal output, child initiation, competent use of environment, and creative symbolic play. The child withdrawal composite score is coded on the following 4 items: negative emotionality/fussy, withdrawal, emotional lability, and child avoidance of parent.

      The parent scale type consists of items divided into four composite scores: sensitivity, intrusiveness, limit setting, and negative mood; for the purposes of this study the two former composite scores are used. The parent sensitivity composite score is coded on the following 10 items: acknowledging child’s signals, elaborating on child's signals, parent gaze/joint attention, positive affect, vocal appropriateness/clarity, appropriate range of affect, resourcefulness, praising of the child, affectionate touch, and parent supportive presence. The parent intrusiveness composite score is coded on the following 4 items: forcing, overriding child's signals, parent anxiety, and criticizing the child.

      The dyad scale consists of items divided into two composite scores. The dyadic reciprocity composite score is coded on the following 3 items: dyadic reciprocity, adaptation-regulation, and fluency. The negative state composite score is coded on the 2 following items: constriction and tension.

      Each item is coded on a scale from 1 to 5, as follows: no manifestation of the item's behavior is observed (1), some manifestations are present but not frequent or constant during the interaction (3), manifestations of the item's behavior are frequent and constant throughout the interaction (5). Codes of 2 or 4 can be used to indicate a tendency towards a low (2) or high (4) level. Accordingly, higher scores indicated higher child social involvement, higher parental sensitivity, but also higher child withdrawal, and higher parental intrusiveness.

      For the purposes of this study, the 10 min play session was divided into the first 5 min of the interaction (Time 1, no distraction) and the remaining 5 min of interaction (Time 2, no distraction for the control group, paper-pen distraction, or screen distraction). Each time (Time 1 and Time 2) was coded separately.

      Coders were trained to use the CIB scheme by a licensed coder who obtained reliability with Ruth Feldman’s team. Once coders reached reliability with the licensed coder on a different video dataset, they coded the video data from the present study. A randomly selected 20% of the video data was double-coded; inter-rater agreement was 80%, indicating that both coders gave the same score or a score that differed by maximum one point at the 5-point Likert scale in 80% of the cases.

      Communication skills

      Verbal communication skills were assessed using the word tokens and word types produced by the child and the parent during the 10 min play interaction. Additionally, non-verbal communication skills were evaluated in children through the gestures they produced during the 10 min interaction.

      Word tokens and types scores were based on the transcription of the speech produced by the child and the parent. Speech was transcribed following an adaptation of transcribing conventions from Hoff (54). Transcriptions were analyzed for total number of words produced during the interaction (i.e., word tokens, e.g., “go, go!” counts as two word tokens) and for total number of different words produced during the interaction (i.e., word types; e.g., “go, go!” counts as one word type) using the CLAN software. Word tokens and word types frequencies were extracted for each protagonist, i.e., child and parent. For the purposes of this study, each protagonist received a word tokens and types frequency score for Time 1 and Time 2.

      We further coded for children's nonverbal communication skills during the parent-child interaction, following earlier work (55). Gesture was defined as a communicative hand (e.g., pointing at a ball, extending open palm toward a ball) or body movement (e.g., shaking head sideways to convey negation, extending arms sideways to convey airplane) that was directed to the parent and that did not manipulate objects, such as hammering a peg. All gestures were empty-handed with the exception of show gestures, during which the child brought an object to the parent's attention by holding it up. The frequency of gestures produced by the child was determined for Time 1 and Time 2 of the parent-child interaction session. A randomly selected 20% of the video data was double-coded; inter-rater agreement was 80%.

      Data analysis

      The dependent variables in our analyses were the following: score for child social involvement during the interaction, score for child withdrawal during the interaction, total number of gestures produced by the child, total number of word tokens produced by the child, total number of word types produced by the child, score for parental sensitivity during the interaction, score for parental intrusiveness during the interaction, total number of word tokens produced by the parent, total number of word types produced by the parent, score for dyadic reciprocity during interaction, and score for dyadic negative states during interaction; all dependent variables showed a non-normal distribution. In order to answer our first research question, namely to determine whether parental distraction—independently of whether it is a paper-pen distraction or a screen distraction—alters parent-child interaction and communication, we performed a Wilcoxon signed-rank on the dependent variables between Time 1 and Time 2 for the paper-pen and screen conditions taken together. In order to determine whether parental distraction from filling out a questionnaire on a screen (i.e., technoference) alters parent-child interaction and communication above and beyond parental distraction from filling out a questionnaire on a paper-pen format, we performed a set of Kruskall–Wallis analyses on the dependent variables between the three experimental conditions (no distraction, paper-pen distraction, screen distraction) at Time 2. Last, for the dependent variables that showed significant differences between the three conditions at Time 2, we performed Mann-Whitney tests in order to obtain post-hoc comparisons.

      Results Does parental distraction affect parent-child interaction and communication?

      Comparing parent-child interaction and communication between Time 1 (i.e., no distraction) and Time 2 (paper-pen or screen parental distraction) shows that parental distraction matters (see Table 1).

      Descriptive statistics (means; standard deviation scores in parentheses) for the dependent variables across the two assessment times (Time 1, Time 2) for the paper-pen and screen conditions taken together. The last column displays the p-values for the difference test between the two times.

      Time 1 Time 2 p-value
      Interaction Parental sensitivity 4.46 (.29) 3.76 (.55) .001***
      Parental intrusiveness 1.44 (.45) 1.41 (.53) .532
      Child social involvement 3.85 (.42) 3.43 (.52) .001***
      Child withdrawal 1.28 (.58) 1.51 (.67) .01**
      Dyadic reciprocity 4.46 (.70) 3.21 (.84) .001***
      Dyadic negative states 1.34 (.54) 1.98 (.44) .001***
      Communication Child word tokens 44.53 (37.60) 41.74 (41.63) .350
      Child word types 21.50 (17.43) 21.06 (18.49) .610
      Child gestures 9.06 (8.02) 7.63 (6.95) .299
      Parent word tokens 389.82 (149.02) 278.49 (140.63) .001***
      Parent word types 152.70 (48.36) 129.83 (48.63) .012*

      *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

      Regarding the quality of the parent-child interaction, our results showed that all of the examined variables, except for parental intrusiveness, differed between Time 1 and 2. Specifically, when parents were asked to fill out a questionnaire at Time 2 (either on a paper-pen or screen format), the quality of the interaction was significantly altered compared to Time 1 when they were only instructed to interact with the child: parents were less sensitive to their children's communicative signals and needs (z = 4.595, p < .001), children engaged less with the parent (z = 3.233, p < .001) and also showed more withdrawal behaviors (z = 2.590, p = .01), and the dyads interacted in less reciprocal ways (z = 4.962, p = .001), showing more negative states (z = 4.737, p = .001).

      In terms of communication scores, however, results showed that only parental speech changed between Time 1 and Time 2, such that parents talked less to their children—both in terms of word quantity (z = 3.641, p = .001) and word diversity (z = 3.079, p = .012)—when they were distracted by filling out the questionnaire (Time 2).

      Taken together, the analyses showed that when parents were distracted by filling out the questionnaire—independently on whether it was on a paper-pen or a screen format—the quality of the interaction as well as the child-addressed parental speech deteriorated.

      Does technoference affect parent-child interaction and communication skills?

      Having established that parental distraction alters parent-child interaction and communication, we asked whether the nature of the distraction—digital vs. non-digital—matters as well.

      Results showed that, in terms of interaction quality, parental sensitivity, H(2, n = 50) = 17.977, p < .001, child social engagement, H(2, n = 50) = 6.379, p = .041, and dyadic reciprocity, H(2, n = 50) = 16.727, p < .001, as well as dyadic negative states, H(2, n = 50) = 10.567, p = .005, were significantly different across the three experimental conditions at Time 2 (see Table 2). Regarding communication, only parental production of word tokens showed a tendency towards a significance between the conditions, H(2, n = 51) = 5.838, p = .054. Mann–Whitney comparisons revealed that, for all of the dependent variables that showed significant between-condition differences, the no distraction condition always differed significantly from the paper-pen as well as the screen condition (ps ≤ .04). Importantly, for none of the dependent variables, there were significant difference between the paper-pen distraction condition and the screen distraction condition (ps ≥ .59). This last result reveals that the nature of the distraction—namely, from a paper-pen or a screen activity—does not matter for the parent-child interaction and communication.

      Descriptive statistics (means; standard deviation scores in parentheses) for the dependent variables at Time 2 by condition. The last column displays the p-values of the difference test between the three conditions.

      No distraction Paper-pen distraction Screen distraction p-value
      Interaction Parental sensitivity 4.46 (.33) 3.77 (.51) 3.77 (.60) .001***
      Parental intrusiveness 1.56 (.60) 1.35 (.44) 1.51 (.62) .733
      Child social involvement 3.78 (.27) 3.37 (.60) 3.56 (.35) .041*
      Child withdrawal 1.28 (.36) 1.37 (.49) 1.67 (.82) .465
      Dyadic reciprocity 4.38 (.73) 3.28 (.79) 3.29 (.88) .001***
      Dyadic negative states 1.45 (.53) 2.01 (.39) 1.95 (.52) .001**
      Communication Child word tokens 50.44 (56.74) 34.94 (29.63) 45.82 (51.03) .912
      Child word types 26.44 (24.40) 19.35 (16.57) 20.82 (19.52) .753
      Child gestures 9.63 (8.55) 8.53 (8.04) 7.12 (5.83) .805
      Parent word tokens 368.75 (116.92) 267.23 (140.02) 287.82 (148.76) .054
      Parent word types 148.25 (34.21) 131.06 (50.30) 125.76 (48.29) .259

      p ≤ 0.1; *p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

      Discussion

      In this study we asked whether technoference affects the interaction and the communication between parents and their children during a 10 min play. Observing 50 parent-child dyads, we found that parental distraction matters for the quality of interaction and for the quantity of communicative acts, independently on whether parents were distracted by a paper-pen questionnaire or by a questionnaire on a screen.

      Effect of distraction on parent-child interaction and communication

      Our first main result shows that when parental focus of attention is shifted from the child to another activity, the quality of interaction is negatively impacted. Specifically, when parents were distracted during the parent-child play, parents were less sensitive to children's communicative signals, children showed lower social involvement, and the dyads showed less reciprocity and more negative states in their exchanges. These findings align with previous theoretical and empirical work highlighting that children, especially young children, need the attention of their parents during moments of interaction and play. When parents pay attention to their children, they acknowledge and respond to the child's behavior and needs, which, in turn, contributes to the child's early social, communicative and emotional development (10).

      Importantly, our study suggests that the fact that parents are not focused on the interaction with their child has a negative effect not only on themselves (i.e., parents being less sensitive to the child), but also on the child and on the dyad altogether. Precisely, from early on, children are able to detect that the parent is non-contingent and unsynchronized when being distracted, which results in the child being less involved in the interaction with the parent and more withdrawn, as indexed by behaviors such as sharing less joint attention, showing less positive affect, producing less verbal output, initiating less interactions, etc. Unsurprisingly, the parental distraction during the parent-child interaction also negatively affects the interactive quality of the dyad: dyads engage in less give-and-take synchrony, coordinate less levels of arousal and stimulation, show less smooth and fluent flow of activity and involvement and show higher levels of constriction and tension.

      Our findings showed that parental distraction also affects the communicative exchanges between the parent and the child during the 10 min play. Specifically, parents produced less word tokens (i.e., measure of verbal quantity) and less word types (i.e., measure of verbal variability) in the conditions when parents were distracted. This result confirms our hypothesis according to which parental distraction negatively influences the communicative bids between the parent and the child. Interestingly, we did not find differences in the verbal or non-verbal communication that children were addressing to parents across the undistracted (Time 1) vs. distracted condition (Time 2). It is possible that when parents were distracted, children continued to produce communicative bids in order to regain the attention of their parent, thus maintaining a similar level of communication. This hypothesis remains to be further examined in future studies.

      Lack of effect of technoference on parent-child interaction and communication

      Our second main result is that technoference, namely parental distraction due to using a digital device, does not affect parent-child interaction, nor communication more so than non-digital parental distraction. More specifically, we failed to found a difference between parental distraction due to using a screen compared to parental distraction due to a non-digital activity (i.e., paper-pen condition). This result is in conflict with our hypothesis—we expected that technoference will show a more pronounced negative effect on parent-child interaction quality and communication compared to the non-digital distraction, given that screens are especially distracting to parents (51). Why the lack of effect then?

      A first explanation lies within the existing evidence. While the vast majority of studies on technoference point to negative effects, including on parent-child interaction and communication, these studies are either qualitative, thus failing to provide comparisons from experimental conditions, or compare conditions of parents using screens (i.e., technoference) to conditions of parents not using screens [i.e., paying attention to the child; for a review, see (25)]. Therefore, general conclusions about the negative effects of technoference have been drawn, although without examining whether these effects stem specifically from parental distraction by screens or simply from parental distraction.

      A major strength of the present study is to provide comparisons of parent-child interaction and communication across three experimental conditions: undistracted parents, parents distracted by a non-digital activity (i.e., completing a paper-pen questionnaire) and parents distracted by an activity on a screen (i.e., technoference). Such comparisons suggest that parents are not more distracted by a screen than by another non-digital activity. Recent literature providing evidence from similar comparisons show similar findings. For example, comparing parent-child interactions while parents used a cell phone to parent-child interactions while parents completed a paper survey, Gaudreau et al. (50) did not find a difference in parental, nor child responsiveness. Similarly, while Abels et al. (23) show that when caregivers use mobile media they are less responsive to children's bids for attention, their findings indicate that this appears to also be true when caregivers are involved in other non-child-related activities.

      Taken together, our results add to the existing evidence showing that parents do not seem to be more distracted when using a screen compared to other types of distraction. This might be so because screens are so ubiquitous in today's society, that both parents and children have become accustomed to such devices. Stockdale et al. (56) suggests that this could imply a form of self-regulation learning for the child. In the same vein, we can also assume that the parent becomes accustomed to and implements communicative strategies when using a screen during interactions with the child. This assumption could reduce the negative effects on the quality of the interaction.

      It is also likely that digital devices prompt a joint attention phenomenon; namely, children could find screens attractive, thus increasing the likelihood of joining in the parent's focus of attention drawn from the screen—possibly more so than when parents are involved in a non-screen activity. There is evidence that when parents and children are co-viewing and especially when parent use this co-viewing as an opportunity for interaction, conversation and sharing (5), children do benefit from such screen use, including for their language development (57).

      Limitations

      While the present cross-sectional study adds important findings to the existing literature, it also presents a number of limitations. First, the sample represents a limitation in terms of size and representativeness. It includes 50 dyads (considered a high number for observational studies) and is relatively homogeneous in terms of parent gender (vast majority of mothers), parent education (highly educated parents), and marital status (over-representation of married parents/couples). Second, ecological validity is affected by the experimental setting, which is not representative of everyday parent-child interactions in a natural environment. This may result in a motivational bias with regard to participation, with parents in difficulty with their child avoiding taking part in the study. This may also manifest itself as a social desirability bias through the desire to satisfy social expectations in terms of child rearing. Importantly, the type of parental digital distraction in this study (i.e., completing a questionnaire on a tablet) is different in many ways from the real world, everyday ways in which parents use screens in the presence of their children. Specifically, in our study, parental digital distraction did not include the personal or professional context of screen use, neither any emotional aspect in the nature of the digital distractor, such as for example when consulting work emails or responding to personal messages. In such naturalistic situations of screen use, parents are likely more compelled to use screens and presumably they are more absorbed in the screen use and distracted from the interaction with the child.

      Last, parental gestures were not coded due to personnel shortage. It would have been interesting to have the coding of parents’ gestures in order to better understand certain effects observed in the interaction. In fact, several authors (5860) have stressed the importance of the synchronicity aspect in the interaction, which implies a mutual influence between one and the other.

      Drawing from these limitations, future studies are needed in order to provide better understanding of the effect parental use of screens might or might not have on young children. Importantly, longitudinal studies controlling for a number of significant confounds are truly needed in order to capture possible causal links. These include, but are not limited to, the level of immersion (61) or absorption (25, 30, 57) of the parent during screen use and children's everyday experiences and habits with parental screen use.

      Conclusion

      This study suggests that technoference does disrupt parent-child interaction, but in similar ways than an equivalent non-digital parental distraction. The finding adds to the extensive literature on the importance of parental involvement for the quality of the parent-child interaction. It also allows to dedramatize a certain “moral panic” (62) surrounding screen use. More specifically, it suggests that it might not the use of the screen itself that is derogatory for the interaction; rather, it could the fact that the parent is kept away from the interaction, independently from whether it is a digital or a non-digital distraction.

      Data availability statement

      The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

      Ethics statement

      The studies involving humans were approved by Commission interdisciplinaire d'Ethique de la Faculté des sciences sociales et politiques de l'Université de Lausanne (E_SSP_042021_00002). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

      Author contributions

      SC: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. AF: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. NM: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. FQ: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. ND: Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision.

      Funding

      The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

      This study received partial funding from the University of Applied Sciences of Western Switzerland (HES-SO) as well as from Foundation Action Innocence (Switzerland).

      Conflict of interest

      The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

      Publisher's note

      All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

      References Institut national de la statistique et des études économiques (Insee). Ordinateur et Accès à Internet: Les Inégalités D’équipement Persistent Selon le Niveau de Vie (2019). Available online at: https://www.insee.fr/fr/statistiques/5057474 (Accessed September 2023). Comparis. Étude Smartphone 2020 (2020). Available online at: https://res.cloudinary.com/comparis-cms/image/upload/v1650524241/download_center/Comparis_Smartphonestudie_2020_FR_qlr4nm.pdf (Accessed September 2023). Public Health France. Étude de Santé sur L’environnement, la Biosurveillance, L’activité Physique et la Nutrition (Esteban), 2014–2016. Saint-Maurice: Santé publique France (2017). Available online at: www.santepubliquefrance.fr LauricellaARCingelDPBeaudoin-RyanLRobbMBSaphirMWartellaEA. The Common Sense Census: Plugged-in Parents of Tweens and Teens. Common Sense Media (2016). Available online at: https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-plugged-in-parents-of-tweens-and-teens-2016 (Accessed September 01, 2023). DubuissonHAujoulatIMauletN. Etude exploratoire sur les médiations parentales dans les interactions entre parents et enfants de 3 à 6 ans autour des écrans mobiles tactiles (2020). Available online at: https://dial.uclouvain.be/downloader/downloader.php?pid=thesis%3A23826&datastream=PDF_01&cover=cover-mem (Accessed September 2023). McDanielBTCoyneSM. Technology interference in the parenting of young children: implications for mothers' perceptions of coparenting. J Soc Sci. (2016) 53, 43543. Pew Research Center. Parenting Children in the Age of Screens (2020). Available online at: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2020/07/28/parenting-children-in-the-age-of-screens/ (Accessed September 2023). RadeskyJSKistinCJZuckermanBNitzbergKGrossJKaplan- SanoffM Patterns of mobile device use by caregivers and children during meals in fast food restaurants. Pediatrics. (2014) 133(4):8439. 10.1542/peds.2013-3703 HinikerASobelKSuhHSungYCLeeCPKientzJA. Texting while parenting: how adults use mobile phones while caring for children at the playground. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (2015). 10.1145/2702123.2702199 BornsteinMHTamis-LeMondaCS. Mother–infant interaction. Blackwell Handbook of Infant Development. London: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. (2004). p. 26995. RochatPStrianoT. Social-cognitive development in the first year. In: RochatP, editor. Early Social Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum (1999). p. 334. TrevarthenC. The self born in intersubjectivity: the psychology of an infant communicating. In: NeisserU, editors. The Perceived Self. New York: Cambridge University Press (1993). p. 12173. AinsworthMDS. The development of infant–parent attachment. In: CaldwellBMRiciuttiHN, editors. Review of Child Development Research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (1973) 3. p. 194. BowlbyJ. Attachment and Loss. New York: Basic Books (1973). SternDN. The role of feelings for an interpersonal self. In: NeisserU, editor. The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1993). p. 20515. BloomL. The Transition from Infancy to Language. New York: Cambridge University Press (1993). BornsteinMH. Infant into conversant: language and nonlanguage processes in developing early communication. In: BudwigNUzgirisIZWertschJV, editors. Communication: An Arena of Development. Stamford, CT: Ablex (2000). p. 10929. FeldmanR. On the origins of background emotions: from affect synchrony to symbolic expression. Emotion. (2007) 7(3):601. 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.60117683216 MackayLJKomanchukJHaydenKALetourneauN. Impacts of parental technoference on parent-child relationships and child health and developmental outcomes: a scoping review protocol. Syst Rev. (2022) 11(1):17. 10.1186/s13643-022-01918-334980263 DarlingNSteinbergLD. Parenting style as context: an integrative model. Psychol Bull. (1993) 113:48796. 10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487 McDanielBT. Parent distraction with phones, reasons for use, and impacts on parenting and child outcomes: a review of the emerging research. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. (2019) 1(2):7280. 10.1002/hbe2.139 RadeskyJSMillerALRosenblumKLAppuglieseDKacirotiNLumengJC. Maternal mobile device use during a structured parent–child interaction task. Acad Pediatr. (2015) 15(2):23844. 10.1016/j.acap.2014.10.00125454369 AbelsMVanden AbeeleMvan TelgenTvan MeijlH. Nod, nod, ignore: an exploratory observational study on the relation between parental mobile media use and parental responsiveness towards young children. In: Luef EM, Marin MM, editors. The Talking Species: Perspectives on the Evolutionary, Neuronal, and Cultural Foundations of Language. Graz: Uni-Press Verlag. (2018). p. 195228. BeamishNFisherJRoweH. Parents’ use of mobile computing devices, caregiving and the social and emotional development of children: a systematic review of the evidence. Australas Psychiatry. (2018) 27(2):13243. 10.1177/103985621878976430234373 Braune-KrickauKSchneebeliLPehlke-MildeJGemperleMKochRWylA. Smartphones in the nursery: parental smartphone use and parental sensitivity and responsiveness within parent–child interaction in early childhood (0–5 years): a scoping review. Infant Ment Health J. (2021) 42(2):16175. 10.1002/imhj.2190833452702 KroghMTEgmoseIStuartACMadsenEBHaaseTWVæverMS. A longitudinal examination of daily amounts of screen time and technoference in infants aged 2–11 months and associations with maternal sociodemographic factors. Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 63:101543. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.10154333652202 OchoaWReichSMFarkasG. The observed quality of caregiver-child interactions with and without a mobile screen device. Acad Pediatr. (2021) 21(4):6208. 10.1016/j.acap.2020.07.01232673762 EliasNLemishD. Parents’ social uses of mobile phones in public places: the case of eateries in two national contexts. Int J Commun. (2021) 15:19. LemishDEliasNFloegelD. “Look at me !” parental use of mobile phones at the playground. Mobile Med Commun. (2019) 8(2):17087. 10.1177/2050157919846916 TharnerAMortensenAHHolmsgaardEMVæverMS. Mothers’ smartphone use and mother-infant interactive behavior in the postpartum period. Pediatr Res. (2021) 91(1):811. 10.1038/s41390-021-01451-433731805 WolfersLNKitzmannSSauerSSommerN. Phone use while parenting: an observational study to assess the association of maternal sensitivity and smartphone use in a playground setting. Comput Human Behav. (2020) 102:318. 10.1016/j.chb.2019.08.013 McDanielBTRadeskyJS. Technoference: Parent distraction with technology and associations with child behavior problems. Child Dev. (2017) 89(1):1009. 10.1111/cdev.1282228493400 McDanielBTRadeskyJS. Technoference: longitudinal associations between parent technology use, parenting stress, and child behavior problems. Pediatr Res. (2018) 84(2):2108. 10.1038/s41390-018-0052-629895837 RadeskyJSLeungCYYAppuglieseDPMillerALLumengJCRosenblumKL. Maternal mental representations of the child and mobile phone use during parent-child mealtimes. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2018) 39(4):3107. 10.1097/dbp.000000000000055629485515 MyruskiSGulyayevaOBirkSPérez-EdgarKBussKADennis-TiwaryTA. Digital disruption? Maternal mobile device use is related to infant social-emotional functioning. Dev Sci. (2018) 21(4):e12610. 10.1111/desc.1261028944600 KonradCHillmannMRisplerJNiehausLNeuhoffLBarrR. Quality of mother-child interaction before, during, and after smartphone use. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:116. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.616656 Rozenblatt-PerkalYDavidovitchMGueron-SelaN. Infants’ physiological and behavioral reactivity to maternal mobile phone use–an experimental study. Comput Human Behav. (2022) 127:107038. 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107038 SameroffAJFieseBH. Transactional regulation: the developmental ecology of early intervention. Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (2000) 2(4). p. 135. ChristakisDAGilkersonJRichardsJAZimmermanFJGarrisonMMXuD Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2009) 163(6):554. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.6119487612 GinsburgKR. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics. (2007) 119(1):18291. 10.1542/peds.2006-269717200287 KellyYSackerADel BonoEFrancesconiMMarmotM. What role for the home learning environment and parenting in reducing the socioeconomic gradient in child development? Findings from the millennium cohort study. Arch Dis Child. (2011) 96(9):8327. 10.1136/adc.2010.19591721666278 McFarlaneEDodgeRABurrellLCrowneSChengTLDugganAK. The importance of early parenting in at-risk families and children’s social–emotional adaptation to school. Acad Pediatr. (2010) 10(5):3307. 10.1016/j.acap.2010.06.01120816655 IversonJMCapirciOLongobardiECaselliMC. Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cogn Dev. (1999) 14(1):5775. 10.1016/s0885-2014(99)80018-5 NamyLLAcredoloLGoodwynS. Verbal labels and gestural routines in parental communication with young children. J Nonverbal Behav. (2000) 24(2):6379. 10.1023/A:1006601812056 HartBRisleyTR. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore: Brookes (1995). HuttenlocherJHaightWBrykASeltzerMLyonsT. Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Dev Psychol. (1991) 27(2):236. 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 KirkorianHLPempekTAMurphyLASchmidtMEAndersonDR. The impact of background television on parent–child interaction. Child Dev. (2009) 80(5):13509. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01337.x19765004 ReedJHirsh-PasekKGolinkoffRM. Learning on hold: cell phones sidetrack parent-child interactions. Dev Psychol. (2017) 53(8):142836. 10.1037/dev000029228650177 CorkinMTHendersonAMEPetersonERCostantiniSKSharplinHSMorrisonS. Associations between technoference, quality of parent-infant interactions, and infants’ vocabulary development. Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 64:101611. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.10161134303915 GaudreauCHirsh-PasekKGolinkoffRM. What’s in a distraction? The effect of parental cell phone use on parents’ and children’s question-asking. Dev Psychol. (2022) 58(1):5568. 10.1037/dev000126834881965 HertleinK. Digital dwelling: technology in couple and family relationships. Fam Relat. (2012) 61(3):37487. 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2012.00702.x FeldmanR. Coding Interactive Behaviour Manual. Ramat Gan, Israel: Bar-Illan University (1998). (Unpublished manual). Viaux-SavelonSLeclereCAidaneEBodeauNCamon-SenechalLVatageotS. Validation de la version française du coding interactive behavior sur une population d’enfants à la naissance et à 2 mois. Neuropsychiatr Enfance Adolesc. (2014) 62(1):5360. 10.1016/j.neurenf.2013.11.010 HoffE. Transcription Manual. Boca Raton: The Language Development Lab of Florida Atlantic University (2012). ÖzçalışkanŞGoldin-MeadowS. Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition. (2005) 96(3):B10113. 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.001 StockdaleLAPorterCLCoyneSMEssigLWBoothMKeenan-KroffS Infants’ response to a mobile phone modified still-face paradigm: links to maternal behaviors and beliefs regarding technoference. Infancy. (2020) 25(5):57192. 10.1111/infa.1234232857440 MorrisAJFilippettiMLRigatoS. The impact of parents’ smartphone use on language development in young children. Child Dev Perspect. (2022) 16(2):1039. 10.1111/cdep.12449 FeldmanR. Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. (2015) 27(4pt1):100723. 10.1017/s095457941500065626439059 MarcEPicardD. Interaction. In: Barus-MichelDJ, editors. Vocabulaire de Psychosociologie. Toulouse, France: Érès (2016). p. 1918. 10.3917/eres.barus.2016.01.0191 RobinMBergonnier-DupuyG. Evolution du regard des chercheurs en psychologie sur le couple et la famille. Dans Couple Conjugal, Couple Parental: Vers de Nouveaux Modèles. Toulouse, France: Érès (2007). p. 1947. ModeckiKLLow-ChoySUinkBVernonLCorreiaHAndrewsK. Tuning into the real effect of smartphone use on parenting: a multiverse analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2020) 61(8):85565. 10.1111/jcpp.1328232638400 RibnerADMcHargG. Screens across the pond: findings from longitudinal screen time research in the US and UK. Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 63:101551. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.10155133706229
      ‘Oh, my dear Thomas, you haven’t heard the terrible news then?’ she said. ‘I thought you would be sure to have seen it placarded somewhere. Alice went straight to her room, and I haven’t seen her since, though I repeatedly knocked at the door, which she has locked on the inside, and I’m sure it’s most unnatural of her not to let her own mother comfort her. It all happened in a moment: I have always said those great motor-cars shouldn’t be allowed to career about the streets, especially when they are all paved with cobbles as they are at Easton Haven, which are{331} so slippery when it’s wet. He slipped, and it went over him in a moment.’ My thanks were few and awkward, for there still hung to the missive a basting thread, and it was as warm as a nestling bird. I bent low--everybody was emotional in those days--kissed the fragrant thing, thrust it into my bosom, and blushed worse than Camille. "What, the Corner House victim? Is that really a fact?" "My dear child, I don't look upon it in that light at all. The child gave our picturesque friend a certain distinction--'My husband is dead, and this is my only child,' and all that sort of thing. It pays in society." leave them on the steps of a foundling asylum in order to insure [See larger version] Interoffice guff says you're planning definite moves on your own, J. O., and against some opposition. Is the Colonel so poor or so grasping—or what? Albert could not speak, for he felt as if his brains and teeth were rattling about inside his head. The rest of[Pg 188] the family hunched together by the door, the boys gaping idiotically, the girls in tears. "Now you're married." The host was called in, and unlocked a drawer in which they were deposited. The galleyman, with visible reluctance, arrayed himself in the garments, and he was observed to shudder more than once during the investiture of the dead man's apparel. HoME香京julia种子在线播放 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.gzzzyc.com.cn
      ihfjhs.com.cn
      www.gmchain.com.cn
      www.ftfmmt.com.cn
      hztfdz.com.cn
      www.huas97.com.cn
      lmchain.com.cn
      udxw.com.cn
      nqchain.com.cn
      www.woooyoo.com.cn
      处女被大鸡巴操 强奸乱伦小说图片 俄罗斯美女爱爱图 调教强奸学生 亚洲女的穴 夜来香图片大全 美女性强奸电影 手机版色中阁 男性人体艺术素描图 16p成人 欧美性爱360 电影区 亚洲电影 欧美电影 经典三级 偷拍自拍 动漫电影 乱伦电影 变态另类 全部电 类似狠狠鲁的网站 黑吊操白逼图片 韩国黄片种子下载 操逼逼逼逼逼 人妻 小说 p 偷拍10幼女自慰 极品淫水很多 黄色做i爱 日本女人人体电影快播看 大福国小 我爱肏屄美女 mmcrwcom 欧美多人性交图片 肥臀乱伦老头舔阴帝 d09a4343000019c5 西欧人体艺术b xxoo激情短片 未成年人的 插泰国人夭图片 第770弾み1 24p 日本美女性 交动态 eee色播 yantasythunder 操无毛少女屄 亚洲图片你懂的女人 鸡巴插姨娘 特级黄 色大片播 左耳影音先锋 冢本友希全集 日本人体艺术绿色 我爱被舔逼 内射 幼 美阴图 喷水妹子高潮迭起 和后妈 操逼 美女吞鸡巴 鸭个自慰 中国女裸名单 操逼肥臀出水换妻 色站裸体义术 中国行上的漏毛美女叫什么 亚洲妹性交图 欧美美女人裸体人艺照 成人色妹妹直播 WWW_JXCT_COM r日本女人性淫乱 大胆人艺体艺图片 女同接吻av 碰碰哥免费自拍打炮 艳舞写真duppid1 88电影街拍视频 日本自拍做爱qvod 实拍美女性爱组图 少女高清av 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 台湾luanlunxiaoshuo 洛克王国宠物排行榜 皇瑟电影yy频道大全 红孩儿连连看 阴毛摄影 大胆美女写真人体艺术摄影 和风骚三个媳妇在家做爱 性爱办公室高清 18p2p木耳 大波撸影音 大鸡巴插嫩穴小说 一剧不超两个黑人 阿姨诱惑我快播 幼香阁千叶县小学生 少女妇女被狗强奸 曰人体妹妹 十二岁性感幼女 超级乱伦qvod 97爱蜜桃ccc336 日本淫妇阴液 av海量资源999 凤凰影视成仁 辰溪四中艳照门照片 先锋模特裸体展示影片 成人片免费看 自拍百度云 肥白老妇女 女爱人体图片 妈妈一女穴 星野美夏 日本少女dachidu 妹子私处人体图片 yinmindahuitang 舔无毛逼影片快播 田莹疑的裸体照片 三级电影影音先锋02222 妻子被外国老头操 观月雏乃泥鳅 韩国成人偷拍自拍图片 强奸5一9岁幼女小说 汤姆影院av图片 妹妹人艺体图 美女大驱 和女友做爱图片自拍p 绫川まどか在线先锋 那么嫩的逼很少见了 小女孩做爱 处女好逼连连看图图 性感美女在家做爱 近距离抽插骚逼逼 黑屌肏金毛屄 日韩av美少女 看喝尿尿小姐日逼色色色网图片 欧美肛交新视频 美女吃逼逼 av30线上免费 伊人在线三级经典 新视觉影院t6090影院 最新淫色电影网址 天龙影院远古手机版 搞老太影院 插进美女的大屁股里 私人影院加盟费用 www258dd 求一部电影里面有一个二猛哥 深肛交 日本萌妹子人体艺术写真图片 插入屄眼 美女的木奶 中文字幕黄色网址影视先锋 九号女神裸 和骚人妻偷情 和潘晓婷做爱 国模大尺度蜜桃 欧美大逼50p 西西人体成人 李宗瑞继母做爱原图物处理 nianhuawang 男鸡巴的视屏 � 97免费色伦电影 好色网成人 大姨子先锋 淫荡巨乳美女教师妈妈 性nuexiaoshuo WWW36YYYCOM 长春继续给力进屋就操小女儿套干破内射对白淫荡 农夫激情社区 日韩无码bt 欧美美女手掰嫩穴图片 日本援交偷拍自拍 入侵者日本在线播放 亚洲白虎偷拍自拍 常州高见泽日屄 寂寞少妇自卫视频 人体露逼图片 多毛外国老太 变态乱轮手机在线 淫荡妈妈和儿子操逼 伦理片大奶少女 看片神器最新登入地址sqvheqi345com账号群 麻美学姐无头 圣诞老人射小妞和强奸小妞动话片 亚洲AV女老师 先锋影音欧美成人资源 33344iucoom zV天堂电影网 宾馆美女打炮视频 色五月丁香五月magnet 嫂子淫乱小说 张歆艺的老公 吃奶男人视频在线播放 欧美色图男女乱伦 avtt2014ccvom 性插色欲香影院 青青草撸死你青青草 99热久久第一时间 激情套图卡通动漫 幼女裸聊做爱口交 日本女人被强奸乱伦 草榴社区快播 2kkk正在播放兽骑 啊不要人家小穴都湿了 www猎奇影视 A片www245vvcomwwwchnrwhmhzcn 搜索宜春院av wwwsee78co 逼奶鸡巴插 好吊日AV在线视频19gancom 熟女伦乱图片小说 日本免费av无码片在线开苞 鲁大妈撸到爆 裸聊官网 德国熟女xxx 新不夜城论坛首页手机 女虐男网址 男女做爱视频华为网盘 激情午夜天亚洲色图 内裤哥mangent 吉沢明歩制服丝袜WWWHHH710COM 屌逼在线试看 人体艺体阿娇艳照 推荐一个可以免费看片的网站如果被QQ拦截请复制链接在其它浏览器打开xxxyyy5comintr2a2cb551573a2b2e 欧美360精品粉红鲍鱼 教师调教第一页 聚美屋精品图 中韩淫乱群交 俄罗斯撸撸片 把鸡巴插进小姨子的阴道 干干AV成人网 aolasoohpnbcn www84ytom 高清大量潮喷www27dyycom 宝贝开心成人 freefronvideos人母 嫩穴成人网gggg29com 逼着舅妈给我口交肛交彩漫画 欧美色色aV88wwwgangguanscom 老太太操逼自拍视频 777亚洲手机在线播放 有没有夫妻3p小说 色列漫画淫女 午间色站导航 欧美成人处女色大图 童颜巨乳亚洲综合 桃色性欲草 色眯眯射逼 无码中文字幕塞外青楼这是一个 狂日美女老师人妻 爱碰网官网 亚洲图片雅蠛蝶 快播35怎么搜片 2000XXXX电影 新谷露性家庭影院 深深候dvd播放 幼齿用英语怎么说 不雅伦理无需播放器 国外淫荡图片 国外网站幼幼嫩网址 成年人就去色色视频快播 我鲁日日鲁老老老我爱 caoshaonvbi 人体艺术avav 性感性色导航 韩国黄色哥来嫖网站 成人网站美逼 淫荡熟妇自拍 欧美色惰图片 北京空姐透明照 狼堡免费av视频 www776eom 亚洲无码av欧美天堂网男人天堂 欧美激情爆操 a片kk266co 色尼姑成人极速在线视频 国语家庭系列 蒋雯雯 越南伦理 色CC伦理影院手机版 99jbbcom 大鸡巴舅妈 国产偷拍自拍淫荡对话视频 少妇春梦射精 开心激动网 自拍偷牌成人 色桃隐 撸狗网性交视频 淫荡的三位老师 伦理电影wwwqiuxia6commqiuxia6com 怡春院分站 丝袜超短裙露脸迅雷下载 色制服电影院 97超碰好吊色男人 yy6080理论在线宅男日韩福利大全 大嫂丝袜 500人群交手机在线 5sav 偷拍熟女吧 口述我和妹妹的欲望 50p电脑版 wwwavtttcon 3p3com 伦理无码片在线看 欧美成人电影图片岛国性爱伦理电影 先锋影音AV成人欧美 我爱好色 淫电影网 WWW19MMCOM 玛丽罗斯3d同人动画h在线看 动漫女孩裸体 超级丝袜美腿乱伦 1919gogo欣赏 大色逼淫色 www就是撸 激情文学网好骚 A级黄片免费 xedd5com 国内的b是黑的 快播美国成年人片黄 av高跟丝袜视频 上原保奈美巨乳女教师在线观看 校园春色都市激情fefegancom 偷窥自拍XXOO 搜索看马操美女 人本女优视频 日日吧淫淫 人妻巨乳影院 美国女子性爱学校 大肥屁股重口味 啪啪啪啊啊啊不要 操碰 japanfreevideoshome国产 亚州淫荡老熟女人体 伦奸毛片免费在线看 天天影视se 樱桃做爱视频 亚卅av在线视频 x奸小说下载 亚洲色图图片在线 217av天堂网 东方在线撸撸-百度 幼幼丝袜集 灰姑娘的姐姐 青青草在线视频观看对华 86papa路con 亚洲1AV 综合图片2区亚洲 美国美女大逼电影 010插插av成人网站 www色comwww821kxwcom 播乐子成人网免费视频在线观看 大炮撸在线影院 ,www4KkKcom 野花鲁最近30部 wwwCC213wapwww2233ww2download 三客优最新地址 母亲让儿子爽的无码视频 全国黄色片子 欧美色图美国十次 超碰在线直播 性感妖娆操 亚洲肉感熟女色图 a片A毛片管看视频 8vaa褋芯屑 333kk 川岛和津实视频 在线母子乱伦对白 妹妹肥逼五月 亚洲美女自拍 老婆在我面前小说 韩国空姐堪比情趣内衣 干小姐综合 淫妻色五月 添骚穴 WM62COM 23456影视播放器 成人午夜剧场 尼姑福利网 AV区亚洲AV欧美AV512qucomwwwc5508com 经典欧美骚妇 震动棒露出 日韩丝袜美臀巨乳在线 av无限吧看 就去干少妇 色艺无间正面是哪集 校园春色我和老师做爱 漫画夜色 天海丽白色吊带 黄色淫荡性虐小说 午夜高清播放器 文20岁女性荫道口图片 热国产热无码热有码 2015小明发布看看算你色 百度云播影视 美女肏屄屄乱轮小说 家族舔阴AV影片 邪恶在线av有码 父女之交 关于处女破处的三级片 极品护士91在线 欧美虐待女人视频的网站 享受老太太的丝袜 aaazhibuo 8dfvodcom成人 真实自拍足交 群交男女猛插逼 妓女爱爱动态 lin35com是什么网站 abp159 亚洲色图偷拍自拍乱伦熟女抠逼自慰 朝国三级篇 淫三国幻想 免费的av小电影网站 日本阿v视频免费按摩师 av750c0m 黄色片操一下 巨乳少女车震在线观看 操逼 免费 囗述情感一乱伦岳母和女婿 WWW_FAMITSU_COM 偷拍中国少妇在公车被操视频 花也真衣论理电影 大鸡鸡插p洞 新片欧美十八岁美少 进击的巨人神thunderftp 西方美女15p 深圳哪里易找到老女人玩视频 在线成人有声小说 365rrr 女尿图片 我和淫荡的小姨做爱 � 做爱技术体照 淫妇性爱 大学生私拍b 第四射狠狠射小说 色中色成人av社区 和小姨子乱伦肛交 wwwppp62com 俄罗斯巨乳人体艺术 骚逼阿娇 汤芳人体图片大胆 大胆人体艺术bb私处 性感大胸骚货 哪个网站幼女的片多 日本美女本子把 色 五月天 婷婷 快播 美女 美穴艺术 色百合电影导航 大鸡巴用力 孙悟空操美少女战士 狠狠撸美女手掰穴图片 古代女子与兽类交 沙耶香套图 激情成人网区 暴风影音av播放 动漫女孩怎么插第3个 mmmpp44 黑木麻衣无码ed2k 淫荡学姐少妇 乱伦操少女屄 高中性爱故事 骚妹妹爱爱图网 韩国模特剪长发 大鸡巴把我逼日了 中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片中国张柏芝做爱片 大胆女人下体艺术图片 789sss 影音先锋在线国内情侣野外性事自拍普通话对白 群撸图库 闪现君打阿乐 ady 小说 插入表妹嫩穴小说 推荐成人资源 网络播放器 成人台 149大胆人体艺术 大屌图片 骚美女成人av 春暖花开春色性吧 女亭婷五月 我上了同桌的姐姐 恋夜秀场主播自慰视频 yzppp 屄茎 操屄女图 美女鲍鱼大特写 淫乱的日本人妻山口玲子 偷拍射精图 性感美女人体艺木图片 种马小说完本 免费电影院 骑士福利导航导航网站 骚老婆足交 国产性爱一级电影 欧美免费成人花花性都 欧美大肥妞性爱视频 家庭乱伦网站快播 偷拍自拍国产毛片 金发美女也用大吊来开包 缔D杏那 yentiyishu人体艺术ytys WWWUUKKMCOM 女人露奶 � 苍井空露逼 老荡妇高跟丝袜足交 偷偷和女友的朋友做爱迅雷 做爱七十二尺 朱丹人体合成 麻腾由纪妃 帅哥撸播种子图 鸡巴插逼动态图片 羙国十次啦中文 WWW137AVCOM 神斗片欧美版华语 有气质女人人休艺术 由美老师放屁电影 欧美女人肉肏图片 白虎种子快播 国产自拍90后女孩 美女在床上疯狂嫩b 饭岛爱最后之作 幼幼强奸摸奶 色97成人动漫 两性性爱打鸡巴插逼 新视觉影院4080青苹果影院 嗯好爽插死我了 阴口艺术照 李宗瑞电影qvod38 爆操舅母 亚洲色图七七影院 被大鸡巴操菊花 怡红院肿么了 成人极品影院删除 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 欧美女子与狗随便性交 苍井空的bt种子无码 熟女乱伦长篇小说 大色虫 兽交幼女影音先锋播放 44aad be0ca93900121f9b 先锋天耗ばさ无码 欧毛毛女三级黄色片图 干女人黑木耳照 日本美女少妇嫩逼人体艺术 sesechangchang 色屄屄网 久久撸app下载 色图色噜 美女鸡巴大奶 好吊日在线视频在线观看 透明丝袜脚偷拍自拍 中山怡红院菜单 wcwwwcom下载 骑嫂子 亚洲大色妣 成人故事365ahnet 丝袜家庭教mp4 幼交肛交 妹妹撸撸大妈 日本毛爽 caoprom超碰在email 关于中国古代偷窥的黄片 第一会所老熟女下载 wwwhuangsecome 狼人干综合新地址HD播放 变态儿子强奸乱伦图 强奸电影名字 2wwwer37com 日本毛片基地一亚洲AVmzddcxcn 暗黑圣经仙桃影院 37tpcocn 持月真由xfplay 好吊日在线视频三级网 我爱背入李丽珍 电影师傅床戏在线观看 96插妹妹sexsex88com 豪放家庭在线播放 桃花宝典极夜著豆瓜网 安卓系统播放神器 美美网丝袜诱惑 人人干全免费视频xulawyercn av无插件一本道 全国色五月 操逼电影小说网 good在线wwwyuyuelvcom www18avmmd 撸波波影视无插件 伊人幼女成人电影 会看射的图片 小明插看看 全裸美女扒开粉嫩b 国人自拍性交网站 萝莉白丝足交本子 七草ちとせ巨乳视频 摇摇晃晃的成人电影 兰桂坊成社人区小说www68kqcom 舔阴论坛 久撸客一撸客色国内外成人激情在线 明星门 欧美大胆嫩肉穴爽大片 www牛逼插 性吧星云 少妇性奴的屁眼 人体艺术大胆mscbaidu1imgcn 最新久久色色成人版 l女同在线 小泽玛利亚高潮图片搜索 女性裸b图 肛交bt种子 最热门有声小说 人间添春色 春色猜谜字 樱井莉亚钢管舞视频 小泽玛利亚直美6p 能用的h网 还能看的h网 bl动漫h网 开心五月激 东京热401 男色女色第四色酒色网 怎么下载黄色小说 黄色小说小栽 和谐图城 乐乐影院 色哥导航 特色导航 依依社区 爱窝窝在线 色狼谷成人 91porn 包要你射电影 色色3A丝袜 丝袜妹妹淫网 爱色导航(荐) 好男人激情影院 坏哥哥 第七色 色久久 人格分裂 急先锋 撸撸射中文网 第一会所综合社区 91影院老师机 东方成人激情 怼莪影院吹潮 老鸭窝伊人无码不卡无码一本道 av女柳晶电影 91天生爱风流作品 深爱激情小说私房婷婷网 擼奶av 567pao 里番3d一家人野外 上原在线电影 水岛津实透明丝袜 1314酒色 网旧网俺也去 0855影院 在线无码私人影院 搜索 国产自拍 神马dy888午夜伦理达达兔 农民工黄晓婷 日韩裸体黑丝御姐 屈臣氏的燕窝面膜怎么样つぼみ晶エリーの早漏チ○ポ强化合宿 老熟女人性视频 影音先锋 三上悠亚ol 妹妹影院福利片 hhhhhhhhsxo 午夜天堂热的国产 强奸剧场 全裸香蕉视频无码 亚欧伦理视频 秋霞为什么给封了 日本在线视频空天使 日韩成人aⅴ在线 日本日屌日屄导航视频 在线福利视频 日本推油无码av magnet 在线免费视频 樱井梨吮东 日本一本道在线无码DVD 日本性感诱惑美女做爱阴道流水视频 日本一级av 汤姆avtom在线视频 台湾佬中文娱乐线20 阿v播播下载 橙色影院 奴隶少女护士cg视频 汤姆在线影院无码 偷拍宾馆 业面紧急生级访问 色和尚有线 厕所偷拍一族 av女l 公交色狼优酷视频 裸体视频AV 人与兽肉肉网 董美香ol 花井美纱链接 magnet 西瓜影音 亚洲 自拍 日韩女优欧美激情偷拍自拍 亚洲成年人免费视频 荷兰免费成人电影 深喉呕吐XXⅩX 操石榴在线视频 天天色成人免费视频 314hu四虎 涩久免费视频在线观看 成人电影迅雷下载 能看见整个奶子的香蕉影院 水菜丽百度影音 gwaz079百度云 噜死你们资源站 主播走光视频合集迅雷下载 thumbzilla jappen 精品Av 古川伊织star598在线 假面女皇vip在线视频播放 国产自拍迷情校园 啪啪啪公寓漫画 日本阿AV 黄色手机电影 欧美在线Av影院 华裔电击女神91在线 亚洲欧美专区 1日本1000部免费视频 开放90后 波多野结衣 东方 影院av 页面升级紧急访问每天正常更新 4438Xchengeren 老炮色 a k福利电影 色欲影视色天天视频 高老庄aV 259LUXU-683 magnet 手机在线电影 国产区 欧美激情人人操网 国产 偷拍 直播 日韩 国内外激情在线视频网给 站长统计一本道人妻 光棍影院被封 紫竹铃取汁 ftp 狂插空姐嫩 xfplay 丈夫面前 穿靴子伪街 XXOO视频在线免费 大香蕉道久在线播放 电棒漏电嗨过头 充气娃能看下毛和洞吗 夫妻牲交 福利云点墦 yukun瑟妃 疯狂交换女友 国产自拍26页 腐女资源 百度云 日本DVD高清无码视频 偷拍,自拍AV伦理电影 A片小视频福利站。 大奶肥婆自拍偷拍图片 交配伊甸园 超碰在线视频自拍偷拍国产 小热巴91大神 rctd 045 类似于A片 超美大奶大学生美女直播被男友操 男友问 你的衣服怎么脱掉的 亚洲女与黑人群交视频一 在线黄涩 木内美保步兵番号 鸡巴插入欧美美女的b舒服 激情在线国产自拍日韩欧美 国语福利小视频在线观看 作爱小视颍 潮喷合集丝袜无码mp4 做爱的无码高清视频 牛牛精品 伊aⅤ在线观看 savk12 哥哥搞在线播放 在线电一本道影 一级谍片 250pp亚洲情艺中心,88 欧美一本道九色在线一 wwwseavbacom色av吧 cos美女在线 欧美17,18ⅹⅹⅹ视频 自拍嫩逼 小电影在线观看网站 筱田优 贼 水电工 5358x视频 日本69式视频有码 b雪福利导航 韩国女主播19tvclub在线 操逼清晰视频 丝袜美女国产视频网址导航 水菜丽颜射房间 台湾妹中文娱乐网 风吟岛视频 口交 伦理 日本熟妇色五十路免费视频 A级片互舔 川村真矢Av在线观看 亚洲日韩av 色和尚国产自拍 sea8 mp4 aV天堂2018手机在线 免费版国产偷拍a在线播放 狠狠 婷婷 丁香 小视频福利在线观看平台 思妍白衣小仙女被邻居强上 萝莉自拍有水 4484新视觉 永久发布页 977成人影视在线观看 小清新影院在线观 小鸟酱后丝后入百度云 旋风魅影四级 香蕉影院小黄片免费看 性爱直播磁力链接 小骚逼第一色影院 性交流的视频 小雪小视频bd 小视频TV禁看视频 迷奸AV在线看 nba直播 任你在干线 汤姆影院在线视频国产 624u在线播放 成人 一级a做爰片就在线看狐狸视频 小香蕉AV视频 www182、com 腿模简小育 学生做爱视频 秘密搜查官 快播 成人福利网午夜 一级黄色夫妻录像片 直接看的gav久久播放器 国产自拍400首页 sm老爹影院 谁知道隔壁老王网址在线 综合网 123西瓜影音 米奇丁香 人人澡人人漠大学生 色久悠 夜色视频你今天寂寞了吗? 菲菲影视城美国 被抄的影院 变态另类 欧美 成人 国产偷拍自拍在线小说 不用下载安装就能看的吃男人鸡巴视频 插屄视频 大贯杏里播放 wwwhhh50 233若菜奈央 伦理片天海翼秘密搜查官 大香蕉在线万色屋视频 那种漫画小说你懂的 祥仔电影合集一区 那里可以看澳门皇冠酒店a片 色自啪 亚洲aV电影天堂 谷露影院ar toupaizaixian sexbj。com 毕业生 zaixian mianfei 朝桐光视频 成人短视频在线直接观看 陈美霖 沈阳音乐学院 导航女 www26yjjcom 1大尺度视频 开平虐女视频 菅野雪松协和影视在线视频 华人play在线视频bbb 鸡吧操屄视频 多啪啪免费视频 悠草影院 金兰策划网 (969) 橘佑金短视频 国内一极刺激自拍片 日本制服番号大全magnet 成人动漫母系 电脑怎么清理内存 黄色福利1000 dy88午夜 偷拍中学生洗澡磁力链接 花椒相机福利美女视频 站长推荐磁力下载 mp4 三洞轮流插视频 玉兔miki热舞视频 夜生活小视频 爆乳人妖小视频 国内网红主播自拍福利迅雷下载 不用app的裸裸体美女操逼视频 变态SM影片在线观看 草溜影院元气吧 - 百度 - 百度 波推全套视频 国产双飞集合ftp 日本在线AV网 笔国毛片 神马影院女主播是我的邻居 影音资源 激情乱伦电影 799pao 亚洲第一色第一影院 av视频大香蕉 老梁故事汇希斯莱杰 水中人体磁力链接 下载 大香蕉黄片免费看 济南谭崔 避开屏蔽的岛a片 草破福利 要看大鸡巴操小骚逼的人的视频 黑丝少妇影音先锋 欧美巨乳熟女磁力链接 美国黄网站色大全 伦蕉在线久播 极品女厕沟 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 人人摸人人妻做人人看 44kknn 娸娸原网 伊人欧美 恋夜影院视频列表安卓青青 57k影院 如果电话亭 avi 插爆骚女精品自拍 青青草在线免费视频1769TV 令人惹火的邻家美眉 影音先锋 真人妹子被捅动态图 男人女人做完爱视频15 表姐合租两人共处一室晚上她竟爬上了我的床 性爱教学视频 北条麻妃bd在线播放版 国产老师和师生 magnet wwwcctv1024 女神自慰 ftp 女同性恋做激情视频 欧美大胆露阴视频 欧美无码影视 好女色在线观看 后入肥臀18p 百度影视屏福利 厕所超碰视频 强奸mp magnet 欧美妹aⅴ免费线上看 2016年妞干网视频 5手机在线福利 超在线最视频 800av:cOm magnet 欧美性爱免播放器在线播放 91大款肥汤的性感美乳90后邻家美眉趴着窗台后入啪啪 秋霞日本毛片网站 cheng ren 在线视频 上原亚衣肛门无码解禁影音先锋 美脚家庭教师在线播放 尤酷伦理片 熟女性生活视频在线观看 欧美av在线播放喷潮 194avav 凤凰AV成人 - 百度 kbb9999 AV片AV在线AV无码 爱爱视频高清免费观看 黄色男女操b视频 观看 18AV清纯视频在线播放平台 成人性爱视频久久操 女性真人生殖系统双性人视频 下身插入b射精视频 明星潜规测视频 mp4 免賛a片直播绪 国内 自己 偷拍 在线 国内真实偷拍 手机在线 国产主播户外勾在线 三桥杏奈高清无码迅雷下载 2五福电影院凸凹频频 男主拿鱼打女主,高宝宝 色哥午夜影院 川村まや痴汉 草溜影院费全过程免费 淫小弟影院在线视频 laohantuiche 啪啪啪喷潮XXOO视频 青娱乐成人国产 蓝沢润 一本道 亚洲青涩中文欧美 神马影院线理论 米娅卡莉法的av 在线福利65535 欧美粉色在线 欧美性受群交视频1在线播放 极品喷奶熟妇在线播放 变态另类无码福利影院92 天津小姐被偷拍 磁力下载 台湾三级电髟全部 丝袜美腿偷拍自拍 偷拍女生性行为图 妻子的乱伦 白虎少妇 肏婶骚屄 外国大妈会阴照片 美少女操屄图片 妹妹自慰11p 操老熟女的b 361美女人体 360电影院樱桃 爱色妹妹亚洲色图 性交卖淫姿势高清图片一级 欧美一黑对二白 大色网无毛一线天 射小妹网站 寂寞穴 西西人体模特苍井空 操的大白逼吧 骚穴让我操 拉好友干女朋友3p